Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 31 August 2018
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings

Empathy and the Public Perception of Stillbirth and Memory Sharing: An Australian Case

  • School of Psychology, University of New England, Armidale, NSW, Australia

Objective: Stillbirth devastates families and leaves them struggling to grieve the death of their baby in a society that expects grief symptoms to decrease over time. Previous research has suggested that increased memory sharing opportunities can lead to positive mental health outcomes. The aim of the current study was to examine people’s perceptions of stillbirth as well as the perceived appropriateness of affected parents sharing memories of their child. In addition, we examined whether manipulating empathy would have an effect on people’s perceptions of stillbirth.

Method: Participants included 200 Australian men and women 18 to 74 years of age (M = 36.76, SD = 12.59) randomly allocated to one of three experimental conditions (i.e., low empathy, high empathy, and control). The high empathy group watched a video about stillbirth and was instructed to imagine how the people portrayed felt; the low empathy group watched the same video but was instructed to remain detached; and the control group watched an unrelated video. Participants were then asked how much money they would be willing to donate to a fictional stillbirth organization, followed by the completion of questionnaires measuring (a) perceptions of stillbirth, (b) empathy, and (c) the appropriateness of parents sharing memories of a stillborn child with different groups of people over time.

Results: The empathy manipulation had an effect on empathy and the willingness to help effected parents (high empathy vs. control). However, empathy did not have an effect on participants’ perceptions toward stillbirth nor appropriateness of sharing memories. The appropriateness of sharing memories decreased as time passed and social distance increased.

Discussion: Individuals who have experienced stillbirth need to be aware that societal expectations and their own expectations in relation to sharing memories may not correspond to each other and that they may need to educate their social group about their need to share memories. Removing the taboo surrounding stillbirth is vital for both parents and those to whom they would wish to communicate.

Introduction

The public perception in high income countries is that stillbirth does not happen often (Flenady et al., 2011), however, Australian statistics tell a different story. Stillbirth, as defined in Australia, is the death of a baby of at least 20 weeks gestation or 400 g birth weight (Hilder et al., 2014). In 2012, there were 2,255 reported stillbirths in Australia resulting in 7.2 fetal deaths per 1,000 births (Hilder et al., 2014). Despite these high numbers, it remains a highly under-researched area (Frøen et al., 2011). Most expecting parents are not aware that stillbirth is even an option as a birth outcome, that lifestyle factors can increase the incidence of stillbirth, or that many stillbirths are potentially preventable (Flenady et al., 2011).

The effect of stillbirth on families and society comes at a high cost. In high income countries like Australia, one in five mothers, following a stillbirth, suffers from significant long-term depression, anxiety, or post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Frøen et al., 2011). Christiansen et al. (2013) found that persistent PTSD symptoms continued long after the loss in both parents (up to 18 years). Following a stillbirth, higher levels of depression and anxiety in subsequent pregnancies are found in more recently bereaved mothers and those who felt a lack of social support when their baby was stillborn (Hughes et al., 1999; Turton et al., 2001). The effect of stillbirth on subsequent children puts them at risk of less desirable interactions with their mother (Turton et al., 2009) and disorganized attachments in subsequent children have been found to stem from unresolved grief issues in the mother (Turton et al., 2004). The effects of stillbirth impacts the entire family unit and can stress the couple relationship (Brierley-Jones et al., 2014).

Social stigma and loss of identity is commonly experienced by mothers of stillborns (e.g., Murphy, 2012; Cacciatore et al., 2013; Brierley-Jones et al., 2014; Wonch Hill et al., 2017). The struggle of whether to disclose the stillbirth or not is echoed in the literature by mothers who have experienced guilt, shame, social isolation, and exclusion from family, friends, colleagues, and strangers (Cacciatore, 2010; Thompson, 2013; Brierley-Jones et al., 2014). A survey of Australian parents affected by stillbirth found that they believed others felt the baby was a taboo subject, the mother should try to forget and have another baby, and the stillbirth was “natural selection” and therefore never meant to live (Frøen et al., 2011). Brierley-Jones et al. (2014) argued that the social stigma experienced by mothers occurred even for those who had worked through their grief; they were still subject to negative reactions regarding their experience, thus making it a social phenomenon.

The families of stillborns are faced both with their grief and the task of finding a social identity for their baby. Peelen (2009) explained the process of developing a social identity for a new baby is slow and generally occurs after birth, leaving stillborn babies omitted. Images of stillborn babies are one way a social identity for the baby can be facilitated and their place in the family sphere acknowledged (Godel, 2007).

The making of memories for families of stillborns begins in the hospital with the rituals parents choose to practice following the birth (Layne, 2000; Kobler et al., 2007; Crawley et al., 2013). Items like footprints and photographs, and rituals such as holding and talking about the baby or posting to memorial sites on social media (e.g., Facebook; Hayman et al., 2018), serve to facilitate the grief process, create memories and an identity for the baby as well as validating their experience as parents (Bleyen, 2010; Basile and Thorsteinsson, 2015). No research in Australia appears to have investigated people’s perceptions of the appropriateness of such practices by parents of stillborns or the extent to which they are shared with others.

Previous research has shown the benefits of memory making for families of stillborns (Cacciatore et al., 2008; Ryninks et al., 2014; Basile and Thorsteinsson, 2015). Other studies have indicated adverse effects, including increased PTSD symptoms connected to memory making, resulting from contact with the stillborn child (Hughes et al., 2002; Hughes and Riches, 2003). Research conducted by Crawley et al. (2013) was the first of its kind to investigate the association of sharing memories of the stillborn and its effect on maternal mental health. They found that PTSD and anxiety was lower in those with greater memory sharing and higher in those who desired to speak more about their baby, but felt they could not (Crawley et al., 2013). Similar research in the grief literature indicates that continued opportunities to share are important (Diamond et al., 2012).

The grief literature indicates that people expect there to be an end point to the period of grief, and that grief symptoms should decrease over time (Costa et al., 2007; Penman et al., 2014). These findings lead to the question of what people think is appropriate to share and for how long. If the opportunities to share memories of their stillborn babies increases maternal wellbeing, if there is a social or perceived stigma surrounding stillbirth, and if there is a possibility of an expected finite grieving period, overcoming those barriers are necessary to facilitate sharing opportunities.

Inducing empathy for a person in a stigmatized group can lead to improved attitudes toward the entire group (Batson et al., 2002). Empathy, as defined by Batson (1991), is an other-oriented, vicarious emotion produced by taking the perspective of a person perceived to be in need. Batson et al.’s (2002) research has led to an established, empathy-attitudes-action model whereby empathy for an individual in a stigmatized group leads to helping action for the entire group.

Replicating past results (Batson et al., 1997), Batson et al. (2002) utilized the empathy-attitudes-action model to increase helping behavior for a highly stigmatized group, heroin addicts. While parents of stillborns are not considered a highly stigmatized group in the same way as heroin addicts, they are potentially subject to social or perceived stigma. Thus the benefit of inducing members of the public to empathize with their experience could potentially lead to more positive social interactions for the parents which could facilitate the grieving process.

The results of Frøen et al.’s (2011) study indicates that increased understanding and support for parents is associated with more accurate societal knowledge of the causes of stillbirth. Raising awareness can lead to increased funding and research toward stillbirth with the overall goal being to reduce the incidence of stillbirth. According to Frøen et al. (2011), stillbirth prevention strategies will be most effective if aligned with strategies to provide better support and understanding for women.

The following hypotheses are proposed.

Compared to those in a low empathy or control condition, the induction of empathy (i.e., high empathy condition) would result in: (Hypothesis 1) more positive perceptions toward those who have experienced stillbirth; (Hypothesis 2) more group helping behavior as measured by the allocation of more funds to help; and (Hypothesis 3) responses showing higher levels of appropriateness of sharing for between one and five years post-loss. Hypothesis 4 predicted that the appropriateness of sharing with another person would get progressively less appropriate as the relationship distance between the protagonist and the other person grew. Finally, hypothesis 5 predicted that the perceived appropriateness of sharing memories would decrease with the passage of time for those in the low empathy or control conditions compared to those in the high empathy condition.

Materials and Methods

Participants

An Australian sample of 510 participants undertook this online experimental study. Three participants completed the survey but did not give consent to use their data and were thus excluded. After excluding those with incomplete survey data, 200 adults aged 18 years and over were retained (39.2% retention rate). The participants comprised 29 males and 171 females aged 18 to 74 years (M = 36.76, SD = 12.59), for other demographics see Table 1. Participants were randomly allocated to one of three conditions: high empathy (n = 69; 34.5%), low empathy (n = 75; 37.5%), or control (n = 56; 28%).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Demographics (N = 200).

Materials

Participants were asked a range of questions relating to their age, sex, marital status, level of education, parental status, and experiences of stillbirth, miscarriage, and child loss.

The Perinatal Grief Scale – Short Form (PGS)

The PGS (Potvin et al., 1989) is a 33-item instrument designed to measure perceptions of those who have experienced stillbirth and measured on a 5-point Likert type scale from 1 (strongly agree) to 5 (strongly disagree). The PGS consists of three subscales including active grief, difficulty coping, and despair. The current study measured others’ perceptions of stillbirth (Plagge and Antick, 2009). Thus the language was modified from first person to third person narrative (e.g., “I feel empty inside” to “She feels empty inside”). The PGS has demonstrated sound construct, convergent, and external validity (Toedter et al., 2001). For the current sample, higher scores reflect more perceived grief (α = 0.91), difficulty coping (α = 0.85), and despair (α = 0.91). The Cronbach’s alpha for the total scale was 0.96.

The Emotional Response Questionnaire (ERQ)

The ERQ (Toi and Batson, 1982) consists of adjectives describing different emotional states used to measure empathy. It includes six empathy adjectives: sympathetic, soft-hearted, moved, compassionate, tender, and warm. For each adjective, participants were asked how much they experienced the emotion while watching the video, ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (extremely). The scores were averaged to form an index of empathy (Batson, 1991). For the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha was 0.94.

The Memory Sharing Questionnaire (MSQ)

The MSQ asked participants their perceptions of the appropriateness of parents of stillborn babies sharing memories. The types of questions, employed from previous research (e.g., Bremborg and Rådestad, 2013), included talking about the baby, sharing photographs of the baby, and sharing keepsakes of the baby with the following groups of people: each other, family, friends, colleagues, and others. The questions were asked about the appropriateness of sharing during the first year of loss and again at five years post-loss (e.g., “Please choose how often you think it is appropriate, within the first year of loss, for the parents of stillborn babies to talk about the baby with friends?” and “Please choose how often you think it is appropriate, five years after the loss, for the parents of stillborn babies to talk about the baby with friends?”). The questions were measured on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (never) to 5 (very often). Subscales were calculated by totaling all of the “each other” (or “family”, “friends, “colleagues”, or “others”) responses for the talk, sharing photographs, and sharing keepsakes questions. The Cronbach’s alpha for within the first year of loss was 0.94 and for five years post-loss, 0.97. The subscales’ alphas ranged from 0.88 to 0.95.

Procedure

Participants were recruited for the study via a survey link posted on social media (e.g., Facebook) and a variety of internet forums (e.g., forums.whirlpool.net.au) in order to reach the general Australian population. The project was approved by the Human Ethics Committee of the University of New England (Approval No HE15-091).

Once implied consent was obtained for each participant, they were randomly allocated to one of three conditions: high empathy, low empathy, or control. This study required the use of deception to obtain desired results and was based on Batson’s framework (Batson et al., 2002).

Secondly, participants in the high empathy group watched a 5 min, 30 s video entitled, “Stillbirth Stories” (Raising Children Network, 2014, November 3) which showed a couple discussing their experience of stillbirth. The participants were given instructions to imagine how the parents would feel and to feel the full impact of what they had been through (Batson et al., 2002). Those in the low empathy group watched the same video as the high empathy group but were given instructions to remain objective and detached while watching the video. Finally, participants in the control group watched a 5 min, 20 s video entitled, “How to Make a Quilt using a Sudoku Pattern” (Smith, 2014, October 23) and were given no viewing instructions. The experimental groups’ video was chosen as the information was presented in a fairly neutral, matter-of-fact manner on a highly emotive topic. The control group’s video was chosen as it was unlikely to induce any empathy.

Thirdly, participants were provided with a fictional scenario explaining that they would be participating in a community survey by the IST Fund (a non-government organization that funds not-for-profit organizations) to get the public’s input into the allocation of how their funds should be distributed. They were told that the annual funds for all groups is capped at AU$150,000 in a financial year and that the maximum amount a single organization can receive is AU$50,000. The previous financial year had seen three groups receive funding. The survey was to determine if a fourth fictional organization, the Australian Stillbirth Association (ASA), would be accepted and how much money should go to it. Allocating money to the fictional group meant that the three existing groups (early intervention for children with special needs, single parent education, and animal rights) who wished to continue to receive funds at the same level would lose or have their funding reduced. The participants were asked to decide if this new organization, the ASA, should be included and how much money should be allocated from AU$0 to AU$50,000 in increments of AU$5,000. They received information on the ASA as an organization that supports families in making memories up to five years post-loss along with raising awareness of stillbirth. After making a decision regarding the allocation of funds, participants completed the PGS, ERQ (which acted as a manipulation check for the experience of empathy), and MSQ.

Finally, participants were given a debriefing sheet explaining why deception was used and the option to go into a draw to win an AU$100 gift voucher. If participants decided to participate, they were taken to a different survey that was not linked to the research survey thus keeping the responses anonymous.

Results

Measuring Perceptions of Stillbirth and Empathy (Hypothesis 1)

A one-way between groups ANOVA for the effect of group (i.e., control, low, and high empathy) on perceptions of stillbirth (as measured by the PGS) revealed no significant differences between the groups when empathy was induced, F(2,197) = 0.36, p = 0.697, and η2 = 0.004.

A one-way ANOVA for the effect of group (i.e., control, low, and high empathy) on empathy (as measured by the ERQ) showed a group (i.e., experimental manipulation) main effect, F(2,197) = 35.39, p = 0.001, and η2 = 0.26. Post hoc analyses with Tukey’s HSD revealed that the high empathy condition (M = 5.35, SD = 1.11) and the low empathy condition (M = 5.12, SD = 1.34) scored significantly higher on empathy compared to the control condition (M = 3.25, SD = 2.03; both ps < 0.001), indicating that the manipulation of empathy was partially successful. However, there was no significant difference between the high empathy and the low empathy condition, p = 0.641, and η2 = 0.004.

Empathy and Group Helping (Hypothesis 2)

A one-way ANOVA, groups (i.e., control, low, and high empathy), was conducted to explore the impact on helping (money allocated in the helping options), F(2,197) = 4.97, p = 0.008, and η2 = 0.05. Post hoc analyses with Tukey’s HSD revealed that those in the high empathy group (M = AU$38,985 and SD = AU$14,465) allocated significantly more money to help than those in the control group (M = AU$29,821 and SD = AU$16,867; p = 0.006). However, there was no significant difference between the allocations of money to help the group between those in the high empathy condition and those in the low empathy condition (M = AU$33,533 and SD = AU$17,721; p = 0.117), nor between those in the low empathy condition and those in the control condition (p = 0.408). Empathy scores had a low correlation, in the expected direction, with whether participants believed that the hypothetical organization should receive funding, r = -0.14, and p = 0.047.

Memory Sharing With Who? (Hypotheses 3 and 4)

To examine the differences between who it is appropriate to share memories with, we conducted a within-subjects repeated-measures MANOVA with group (i.e., control, low, and high empathy) by sharing with (i.e., each other, family, friends, colleagues, or others as measured by the MSQ) within both time periods (up to 1 and 5 years). The main effect for group was not statistically significant, Wilks’ lambda = 0.99, F(4,392) = 0.46, p = 0.768, and η2 = 0.005 and neither was the group by sharing with, Wilks’ lambda = 0.95, F(16,380) = 0.58, p = 0.899, and η2 = 0.024. However, there was a statistically significant effect for sharing with, Wilks’ lambda = 0.22, F(8,190) = 82.18, p < 0.001, and η2 = 0.776. All multiple pairwise comparisons (Sidak adjustment) within each year (e.g., family vs. friends in the up to 1 year and friends vs. colleagues at 5 years) showing statistically significant differences, p < 0.001. The appropriateness of sharing with another person got progressively less appropriate as the relationship distance between the protagonist and the other person grew (moving from top to bottom in column 1 in Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Mean (SD) for sharing with different groups of people, 1 and 5 years after stillbirth, repeated-measures ANOVA comparison (N = 200).

Time and Memory Sharing (Hypothesis 5)

A within-subjects repeated-measures MANOVA was run with two levels of time (up to 1 and 5 years) for five outcome measures sharing with (i.e., each other, family, friends, colleagues, or others). The overall effect was statistically significant, Wilks’ lambda = 0.50, F(5,195) = 38.55, p < 0.001, and η2 = .497, with individual repeated-measures ANOVAs supporting Hypothesis 5 and showing that appropriateness of sharing decreased with time (Table 2).

Discussion

The current study sought to investigate empathy and its effect on the Australian public’s perceptions of stillbirth and willingness to help parents of stillborns. It also sought to compare the effects of empathy and the passage of time on the Australian public’s perceived level of appropriateness of sharing memories about the stillborn baby by the parents.

The viewing instructions had a large effect on the empathy felt by participants in both the high and low empathy groups as compared to the control group, demonstrating that the induction of empathy was effective. While Batson et al.’s (2002) original study did not employ a control group, it was necessary for this study in order to accurately measure if the induction of empathy was having an effect. While no statistically significant difference was found between the high and low empathy groups, this could be due to the manipulation. Principally, simply telling people to be objective may not have been enough. Even though the experimental video was chosen as its format was considered neutral in its presentation, the topic of stillbirth is still highly emotive.

As hypothesized, the induction of empathy had a significant effect on the allocation of money to help the hypothetical organization, with a difference found between the high empathy condition and the control condition. However, no statistically significant differences were found between the high empathy and low empathy conditions or between the low empathy and control conditions. This could potentially be because helping parents who have experienced stillbirth is a cause that participants would have chosen to support regardless of whether empathy was induced or not. Thus, the lack of significance could be because participants have strong, established perceptions of stillbirth and what is considered appropriate when it comes to sharing memories. The influence of the empathy induction, therefore, may have made no difference to those established views. However, those in the high empathy condition were potentially moved to give more after feeling the effect of empathy, supporting the idea that a public education campaign regarding stillbirth and its effects on families may be warranted in order to create a socially supportive environment to enable families to grieve.

As time passed, participants’ perceptions of the appropriateness of sharing memories decreased significantly. As the social distance between affected parents and other people increased, participants considered sharing memories of the baby as becoming less appropriate. Thus, while sharing memories with immediate family is both acceptable and expected of grieving parents, this concession decreases as the relationship between people grows. Therefore, if parents’ attempts to share memories or discuss their stillborn are received in a non-positive manner due to the amount of time that has passed since the death, or if there is an implication that they should be “over it by now”, it can result in the feeling that they cannot share in the future. It could also leave parents feeling that their status as parent of a stillborn is something they need to keep quiet. According to research though, parents who have experienced a loss like this simply want their feelings validated and their experiences acknowledged (e.g., Üstündağ-Budak et al., 2015). However, coupled with the “taboo” of stillbirth, talking with non-family members can be challenging, considering that many people simply do not know what to say to someone who has experienced such loss.

The literature highlights the importance of both social support and memory sharing opportunities (e.g., Diamond et al., 2012; Crawley et al., 2013) and these results illustrate the need for an education campaign to raise awareness of stillbirth and the importance memory sharing is for parents. As discussed by Muller and Thompson (2003), sharing memories helps the bereaved come to terms with the death. It also has the potential to help others understand their experience. More opportunities for bereaved parents to be recognized would therefore be beneficial. For example, Australia officially declaring 15 October Pregnancy and Infant Loss Awareness day aims to provide support, awareness, and education regarding those who have experienced the loss of a child. Changes such as these will hopefully facilitate the creation of a more socially supportive environment for families of stillborn babies.

Limitations and Future Research

The recruitment process in the current study was skewed. More women than men participated (14.5%), and the majority of participants were married, in a de facto relationship, or living with their partner (61%), all of which limits its generalizability. There was a fairly normal distribution in age and level of education, however, each participant had access to the internet. Future studies may want to examine potential differences in empathy provided to and/or needed by men and women in relation to stillbirth and other traumatic events. However, such gender differences may be much smaller than has been suggested (MacGeorge et al., 2004) to the extent where they may be irrelevant.

In line with Batson et al.’s (2002) research, helping was measured before perceptions in order to avoid pressure to help the group to maintain consistency with a just-expressed attitude. This leads to the possibility of the participant expressing perceptions to align with their helping behavior due to self-perception. However, empathy did not have an effect on the perception measures and previous research (Batson et al., 1997) demonstrated empathy had effected attitudes without a helping intervention.

While this study demonstrated that high empathy influenced people to allocate money to help stillbirth parents, even the induction of empathy did not change their perceptions toward stillbirth or memory sharing. It is recommended that similar research be conducted with a more representative group of participants while investigating potential mediating and/or moderating variables that could influence results. Further recommendations include developing and trialing an educational stillbirth campaign to measure its effects on participants’ perceptions. Establishing more positive perceptions toward stillbirth parents could facilitate a safer, more accepting social environment for families to openly grieve, remember, and share memories of their babies leading to more positive psychological outcomes. Future campaigns aimed at educating and raising awareness of stillbirth within the general community, similar to those seen with sudden infant death syndrome, may be one positive step forward in eradicating the taboo of stillbirth (see e.g., Mitchell et al., 2011).

Ultimately, the empathy manipulation was unsuccessful in its attempt to create high and low empathy groups. Perhaps participants in the two experimental groups attempted to repress any sense of anxiety or distress they felt as a result of watching the video. However, this should not necessarily obviate the current results given that the high and low groups did experience increased empathy compared to the control group.

Conclusion

The results of the current study indicate that, in general, people are empathetic toward those who have experienced stillbirth, a highly emotive event. However, parents need to be able to share memories and thoughts relating to their child, given the cathartic nature of sharing experiences. As such, eliminating the taboo surrounding stillbirth, as well as educating people about how to talk about child loss is warranted. Informing people via resources such as the Stillbirth Foundation Australia1 has the potential to encourage discourse about stillbirth and bring the topic into the public arena. This, then, can only serve to support vulnerable parents – those coming to terms with the death of their baby.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://stillbirthfoundation.org.au/

References

Basile, M. L., and Thorsteinsson, E. B. (2015). Parents’ evaluation of support in Australian hospitals following stillbirth. PeerJ 3:e1049. doi: 10.7717/peerj.1049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batson, C. D. (1991). The Altruism Question: Toward a Social-Psychological Answer. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Batson, C. D., Chang, J., Orr, R., and Rowland, J. (2002). Empathy, attitudes, and action: can feeling for a member of a stigmatized group motivate one to help the group? Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 28, 1656–1666. doi: 10.1177/014616702237647

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batson, C. D., Polycarpou, M. P., Harmon-Jones, E., Imhoff, H. J., Mitchener, E. C., Bednar, L. L., et al. (1997). Empathy and attitudes: can feeling for a member of a stigmatized group improve feelings toward the group? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 72, 105–118. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.72.1.105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bleyen, J. (2010). “The materialities of absence after stillbirth: historical perspectives,” in The Matter of Death: Space, Place and Materiality, eds J. Hockey, C. Komaromy, and K. Woodthorpe (Basingstoke: Palgrave MacMillan), 69–84.

Google Scholar

Bremborg, A. D., and Rådestad, I. (2013). Memory triggers and anniversaries of stillborn children. Nordic J. Relig. Soc. 26, 157–174.

Google Scholar

Brierley-Jones, L., Crawley, R., Lomax, S., and Ayers, S. (2014). Stillbirth and stigma: the spoiling and repair of multiple social identities. OMEGA J. Death Dying 70, 143–168. doi: 10.2190/OM.70.2.a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacciatore, J. (2010). The unique experiences of women and their families after the death of a baby. Soc. Work Health Care 49, 134–148. doi: 10.1080/00981380903158078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacciatore, J., Frøen, J. F., and Killian, M. (2013). Condemning self, condemning other: blame and mental health in women suffering stillbirth. J. Ment. Health Couns. 35, 342–359. doi: 10.17744/mehc.35.4.15427g822442h11m

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacciatore, J., Rådestad, I., and Frøen, J. F. (2008). Effects of contact with stillborn babies on maternal anxiety and depression. Birth 35, 313–320. doi: 10.1111/j.1523-536X.2008.00258.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christiansen, D. M., Elklit, A., and Olff, M. (2013). Parents bereaved by infant death: PTSD symptoms up to 18 years after the loss. Gen. Hosp. Psychiatry 35, 605–611. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2013.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, B. M., Hall, L., and Stewart, J. (2007). Qualitative exploration of the nature of grief-related beliefs and expectations. OMEGA J. Death Dying 55, 27–56. doi: 10.2190/CL20-02G6-607R-8561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawley, R., Lomax, S., and Ayers, S. (2013). Recovering from stillbirth: the effects of making and sharing memories on maternal mental health. J. Reprod. Infant. Psychol. 31, 195–207. doi: 10.1080/02646838.2013.795216

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, H., Llewelyn, S., Relf, M., and Bruce, C. (2012). Helpful aspects of bereavement support for adults following an expected death: volunteers’ and bereaved peoples’ perspectives. Death Stud. 36, 541–564. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2011.553334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flenady, V., Middleton, P., Smith, G. C., Duke, W., Erwich, J. J., Khong, T. Y., et al. (2011). Stillbirths: the way forward in high-income countries. Lancet 377, 1703–1717. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60064-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frøen, J. F., Cacciatore, J., McClure, E. M., Kuti, O., Jokhio, A. H., Islam, M., et al. (2011). Stillbirths: why they matter. Lancet 377, 1353–1366. doi: 10.1016/S01406736(10)62232-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Godel, M. (2007). Images of stillbirth: memory, mourning and memorial. Visual Stud. 22, 253–269. doi: 10.1080/14725860701657159

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayman, J., Chamberlain, K., and Hopner, V. (2018). Disembodied social life: the ongoing social presence of the born-still on facebook. Mortality 23, 65–81. doi: 10.1080/13576275.2017.1304367

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hilder, L., Zhichao, Z., Parker, M., Jahan, S., and Chambers, G. M. (2014). Australia’s Mothers and Babies 2012. P. S. Series. Canberra: AIHW National Perinatal Epidemiology and Statistics Unit.

Google Scholar

Hughes, P., and Riches, S. (2003). Psychological aspects of perinatal loss. Curr. Opin. Obstet. Gynecol. 15, 107–111. doi: 10.1097/01.gco.0000063548.93768.17

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, P., Turton, P., and Evans, C. D. (1999). Stillbirth as risk factor for depression and anxiety in the subsequent pregnancy: cohort study. BMJ 318, 1721–1724. doi: 10.1136/bmj.318.7200.1721

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, P., Turton, P., Hopper, E., and Evans, C. D. (2002). Assessment of guidelines for good practice in psychosocial care of mothers after stillbirth: a cohort study. Lancet 360, 114–118. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(02)09410-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kobler, K., Limbo, R., and Kavanaugh, K. (2007). Meaningful moments: the use of ritual in perinatal and pediatric death. MCN Am. J. Matern. Child Nurs. 32, 288–295. doi: 10.1097/01.NMC.0000287998.80005.79

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Layne, L. L. (2000). ‘He was a real baby with baby things’. A material culture analysis of personhood, parenthood and pregnancy loss. J. Mat. Cult. 5, 321–345. doi: 10.1177/135918350000500304

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacGeorge, E. L., Graves, A. R., Feng, B., Gillihan, S. J., and Burleson, B. R. (2004). The myth of gender cultures: similarities outweigh differences in men’s and women’s provision of and responses to supportive communication. Sex. Roles 50, 143–175. doi: 10.1023/B:SERS.0000015549.88984.8d

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, E. A., Freemantle, J., Young, J., and Byard, R. W. (2011). Scientific consensus forum to review the evidence underpinning the recommendations of the Australian SIDS and kids safe sleeping health promotion programme - October 2010. J. Paediatr. Child Health 48, 626–633. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1754.2011.02215.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muller, E. D., and Thompson, C. L. (2003). The experiences of grief after bereavement: a phenomenological study with implications for mental health counseling. J. Ment. Health Couns. 25, 183–203. doi: 10.17744/mehc.25.3.wu4n7dljyekuh4ef

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, S. (2012). Reclaiming a moral identity: stillbirth, stigma and ‘moral mothers’. Midwifery 28, 476–480. doi: 10.1016/j.midw.2011.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peelen, J. (2009). Reversing the past: monuments for stillborn children. Mortality 14, 173–186. doi: 10.180/13576270902808043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penman, E. L., Breen, J. L., Hewitt, L. Y., and Prigerson, H. G. (2014). Public attitudes about normal and pathological grief. Death Stud. 33, 510–516. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2013.873839

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plagge, J., and Antick, J. R. (2009). Perceptions of Perinatal loss: Miscarriage Versus Stillbirth. Women’s Health and Urban Life 8. 113–127. Available at: http://hdl.handle.net/1807/17689

Potvin, L., Lasker, J., and Toedter, L. (1989). Measuring grief: a short version of the perinatal grief scale. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 11, 29–45. doi: 10.1007/BF00962697

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raising Children Network (2014). November 3. Stillbirth Stories. Available at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=mibtm6c238U

Ryninks, K., Robert-Collins, C., McKenzie-McHarg, K., and Horsch, A. (2014). Mothers’ experience of their contact with their stillborn infant: an interpretative phenomenological analysis. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 14:203. doi: 10.1186/1471-2393-14-203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, R. (2014). October 23. How to make a Quilt- Sudoku Quilt Pattern Video. Available at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=g9_t5DlEhss

Google Scholar

Thompson, S. (2013). “As if she never existed: changing understandings of perinatal loss in Australia in the twentieth and early twenty-first century,” in Understanding Reproductive Loss: Perspectives on Life, Death and Fertility, eds C. Komaromy, L. L. Layne, and S. Earle (London: Ashgate Publishing Ltd.), 168–178.

Google Scholar

Toedter, L., Lasker, J. N., and Janssen, H. J. (2001). International comparison of studies using the perinatal grief scale: a decade of research on pregnancy loss. Death Stud. 25, 205–228. doi: 10.1080/07481180125971

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toi, M., and Batson, C. D. (1982). More evidence that empathy is a source of altruistic motivation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 43, 281–292. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.43.2.281

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turton, P., Badenhorst, W., Pawlby, S., White, S., and Hughes, P. (2009). Psychological vulnerability in children next born after stillbirth: a case–control follow-up study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 50, 1451–1458. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02111.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turton, P., Hughes, P., Evans, C. D., and Fainman, D. (2001). Incidence, correlates and predictors of post-traumatic stress disorder in the pregnancy after stillbirth. Br. J. Psychiatry 178, 556–560. doi: 10.1192/bjp.178.6.556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turton, P., Hughes, P., Fonagy, P., and Fainman, D. (2004). An investigation into the possible overlap between PTSD and unresolved responses following stillbirth: an absence of linkage with only unresolved status predicting infant disorganization. Attach. Hum. Dev. 6, 241–253. doi: 10.1080/14616730412331281575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Üstündağ-Budak, A. M., Larkin, M., Harris, G., and Blissett, J. (2015). Mothers’ accounts of their stillbirth experiences and of their subsequent relationships with their living infant: an interpretative phenomenological analysis. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 15:263. doi: 10.1186/s12884-015-0700-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wonch Hill, P., Cacciatore, J., Shreffler, K. M., and Pritchard, K. M. (2017). The loss of self: the effect of miscarriage, stillbirth, and child death on maternal self-esteem. Death Stud. 41, 226–235. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2016.1261204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: stillbirth, empathy, memory sharing, public perception, empathy-attitudes-action model

Citation: Keeble CJ, Loi NM and Thorsteinsson EB (2018) Empathy and the Public Perception of Stillbirth and Memory Sharing: An Australian Case. Front. Psychol. 9:1629. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01629

Received: 19 December 2016; Accepted: 14 August 2018;
Published: 31 August 2018.

Edited by:

Gianluca Castelnuovo, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Italy

Reviewed by:

Samantha Murphy, The Open University, United Kingdom
Roxana D. Maiorescu-Murphy, Emerson College, United States

Copyright © 2018 Keeble, Loi and Thorsteinsson. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Natasha M. Loi, nloi2@une.edu.au

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.