Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 12 August 2014
Sec. Cognitive Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Towards an embodied science of intersubjectivity: Widening the scope of social understanding research View all 43 articles

Tackling the social cognition paradox through multi-scale approaches

  • 1Human Brain and Behavior Laboratory, Center for Complex Systems and Brain Sciences, FAU, Boca Raton, FL, USA
  • 2Intelligent System Research Centre, University of Ulster, Derry, Northern Ireland
  • 3CRICM UMR-S975, UPMC-Paris 6, Paris, France
  • 4CNRS, UMR 7225, Paris, France
  • 5ICM, Paris, France

Recent debates regarding the primacy of social interaction versus individual cognition appear to be caused by the lack of an integrative account of the multiple scales at play. We suggest that reconciling individual autonomy and dyadic interactive viewpoints requires the taking into account of different time scales (e.g., development, learning) and levels of organization (e.g., genetic, neural, behavioral, social). We argue that this challenge requires the joint development of tools for two-body and second person neuroscience, along with the theoretical concepts and methods of coordination dynamics and systems biology. Such a research program may be particularly fruitful in deciphering complex socio-developmental diseases that are known to involve alterations on multiple levels.

The Ontogeny of Social Cognition: A Chicken-Egg Issue?

Despite a propensity to interact with others, our ability to socialize seems neither given nor fixed once and for all (Dumas, 2011). As Sheets-Johnstone (2011) has pointed out “we come into the world moving; we are precisely not stillborn.” The question of the ontogeny of social cognition (mirror neurons included) is grounded in our propensity to move. This primacy of movement can even be observed before birth: motorneurons appear well before their sensory counterparts in embryo; a large repertoire of spontaneous (thus self-organized) movements—e.g., making a fist, kicking, sucking—already exists (Kelso, 2002; Piontelli, 2010). Even twin fetuses demonstrate distinctive movements directed to each other (Castiello et al., 2010). At this stage, the “social events” are essentially movements. Does this mean, however, that there is no element of “social cognition” in such encounters? We think not.

Behavioral coordination acts as a powerful linkage between persons, even early in life. Infants are sensitive to contingent movements of the mother (Nadel et al., 1999) and the first dyadic interactions already exhibit co-regulation, “a continuous mutual adjustment of actions and intentions” (Fogel and Garvey, 2007). The disposition of human and monkey newborns to imitate (Meltzoff and Moore, 1983; Kugiumutzakis, 1993; Nagy et al., 2005; Ferrari et al., 2006; Soussignan et al., 2011) is not due to a passive coupling of perception and action. Rather it is an active attempt to adapt and gradually refine their own movements with respect to others. When imitated, human infants and newborn macaques display affiliative behavior toward the imitator (Paukner et al., 2009), as do low-functioning children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) (Nadel et al., 2000). The two facets of imitation, imitate and be imitated, constitute dual roles that can be traded, thereby allowing turn-taking (Nadel-Brulfert and Baudonnière, 1982). All that is needed is anticipation of the partner's next movement.

Here it seems we arrive at a cross-road: key ingredients of social cognition already appear to be present very early. Co-regulation of synchrony, anticipation of the other's intentions, joint attention on a physical target, are central facets of social interaction. Does this mean they all emerge from the developing Mirror Neuron System (MNS)? Even if the early capacity to couple perception and action is associated with a proto MNS (Lepage and Théoret, 2007), we appear to be confronted with a circular logic problem: you need a MNS for social interaction but you need to interact to form a MNS. Although there is limited evidence for mirror neurons in early development (Catmur, 2013), sensorimotor experience may indeed be key to creating mirror neuron responses through Hebbian learning (Keysers and Perrett, 2004; see also Allen and Williams, 2011). See also the epigenetic view of Ferrari et al. (2013).

The idea that the MNS underlies not only motor exchanges but also high-level social cognition is now challenged by the proposal of a complementary role for the “mentalizing network” (Keysers and Gazzola, 2007; Uddin et al., 2007; Sperduti et al., 2014). A main task is to decipher possible top-down and bottom-up processes in social cognition. Such an endeavor requires, at the very least, joint investigation of behavioral and neural dynamics during real social exchanges (Hari and Kujala, 2009; Schilbach, 2014).

The Rise of Two-Body and Second-Person Neuroscience

Although social neuroscience has gathered a lot of data on how individual human beings perceive social stimuli, a truly interactive social neuroscience still lags behind. The community seems to have reached a consensus on the importance of investigating social situations that involve reciprocal exchange and mutual engagement (Hari and Kujala, 2009; Schilbach et al., 2013). Technological developments such as hyperscanning (Tognoli et al., 2007; Dumas et al., 2010; Babiloni and Astolfi, 2012; Hasson et al., 2012; Konvalinka and Roepstorff, 2012) and human-machine interfaces (Kelso et al., 2009; Pfeiffer et al., 2011) have greatly helped operationalize various aspects of real-time social interaction, thereby narrowing the gap between what we know about off-line and on-line social cognition (Schilbach, 2014). The former not only involve the same brain structures identified in research on isolated individuals (Sperduti et al., 2014); the brain dynamics vary according to social context, e.g., spontaneous vs. instructed interaction (Dumas et al., 2012a; Guionnet et al., 2012; Sänger et al., 2012) and social role, e.g., leaders vs. followers (Dumas et al., 2012a; Sänger et al., 2013; Konvalinka et al., 2014).

A further challenge concerns the structure and timing of inter-individual coordination and its relationship with intra-individual processes. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) hyperscanning first showed strong anatomical and functional similarities across different individuals responding to the same perception, especially if it is social (Hasson et al., 2004). This finding extends to interactive contexts where inter-brain synchronization emerges in multiple frequency bands (Dumas et al., 2010; Müller et al., 2013). The related symmetrical and asymmetrical inter-brain patterns reflect how social interaction goes beyond a simple mirroring of the other and relies both on grasping other individuals' motor goals and inferring their intentions (Nadel and Dumas, 2014). Moreover, unlike intra-brain dynamics which primarily involves high frequency rhythms, the inter-brain dynamics appear to operate at lower frequencies (Müller et al., 2013). Thus, the temporal interplay between brain networks involved in social interaction, such as the so-called mirror and mentalizing systems, may be modulated by dynamics at the dyadic level, as in turn-taking (Wilson and Wilson, 2005). Moreover, social cognition cannot be understood only on the bases of intra- or inter-personal dynamics but rather in their common hyper-brain space including both intra- and inter-brain coupling dynamics (e.g., Montague et al., 2002; De Vico Fallani et al., 2010; Sänger et al., 2012, 2013; Müller et al., 2013).

Social Dynamics as a Bridge Between Scales

Cognition is constantly evolving during interactions with the environment and others. In order to sustain covariation, members of a social interaction must engage in active co-regulation (Fogel, 1993) and co-anticipation (Nadel and Dumas, 2014), potentially leading to the co-ownership of the action (Dumas et al., 2012a). Such genuine sharing of the interaction with others has been proposed as participatory sense-making (De Jaegher and Di Paolo, 2007) where social interaction plays a constitutive role for individual cognition (De Jaegher, 2009; Froese et al., 2014). The chicken-egg paradox here vanishes since both interactive and non-interactive mechanisms co-develop and mutually shape each other's development (Di Paolo and De Jaegher, 2012). Although still debated (Gallotti and Frith, 2013), this proposal is now supported by both modeling (Froese and Di Paolo, 2010; Froese et al., 2013) and experimental research (Auvray et al., 2009; Froese et al., 2014). In studies that have assessed the emergence of collective intelligence through dialog (Bahrami et al., 2010; Bang et al., 2014) interaction has been shown to constrain individual information processing (Fusaroli et al., 2014).

Social cognition thus relies on a braiding of neural, behavioral, and social processes (Hari and Kujala, 2009; Kelso et al., 2013). Neurobiological models of socio-cognitive functions have already been proposed (Gallese et al., 2004; Keysers and Perrett, 2004; Friston et al., 2011), though the dynamical components of human interaction are still largely missing (Adolphs, 2003). The theoretical and empirical framework of coordination dynamics has shown that neural, behavioral, and social scales may be studied and understood from a common perspective (Kelso, 1995; Kelso et al., 2009, 2013). As in other theories that aim to elaborate mathematical formalisms for cognition (e.g., Tononi, 2008; Friston, 2010), the objective of coordination dynamics is to identify general principles, the mechanistic realizations of which may be found in a variety of different systems at multiple levels of description. To be more than just words, coordination dynamics had to establish experimentally that criterial features of self-organization (e.g., order parameters, control parameters, stability, instability) actually existed in human behavior and that they could be mapped explicitly on to a theoretical model of the self-organizing dynamics. Then it had to show how information (e.g., about goals, intentions, the environment, etc.) shapes and is shaped by the self-organizing dynamics. Coordination dynamics relies on the same concepts and mathematical formalisms across different time scales and organizational levels and thus potentially offers inroads into a multi-scale account of social cognition.

In physics, multi-scale approaches have already uncovered universal principles, especially when matter undergoes phase transitions (Wilson, 1979). At the neural level, non-linear cross-scale interactions have been demonstrated experimentally (Le Van Quyen, 2011; see also Plenz and Niebur, 2014). In social neuroscience, nonlinearities are omnipresent in the underlying neural and social dynamics. Since functional networks display similar behavior across time-scales (Kelso, 1995; Bressler and Tognoli, 2006), a parsimonious account may be possible. Beyond the quest for parsimony and semantic clarity, having a mathematical formalism enables one to ask computationally relevant questions. For example, in the case of social neuroscience, neurocomputational modeling shows that the anatomical structure of the human brain favors both the complexity of intra-individual dynamics and the coupling in inter-individual dynamics (Dumas et al., 2012b). Regarding the debate about the constitutive role of social interaction, future computational studies can quantify macro-to-micro causal effects ranging from dyadic to individual processes (Hoel et al., 2013).

Conclusion

Social interaction challenges the boundaries between the field of cognitive science and how to divide observations across distinct time scales and organizational levels. Social neuroscience is taking up this challenge at both theoretical and methodological levels. Here we have argued that three major dimensions are of potential significance: integrating a developmental perspective, investigating real-time social interaction with a two-body or second person neuroscience, and adopting a multi-scale approach through complex systems' perspectives, in particular the concepts, methods and tools of coordination dynamics. These developments have already begun and should help further an understanding of disorders of social interaction such as autism.

As Abney et al. (2014) have remarked, in cognitive science “multiple theories should interact when describing the same phenomenon.” In social cognition, the case of autism provides a test bed for an integrative approach. Developmental psychopathology has uncovered a wide range of behavioral peculiarities of persons with autism (Burack et al., 2002); cognitive neuroscience has identified many biomarkers at both structural and functional levels; and systems biology has begun to relate genetic variants associated with cellular and metabolic pathways to individual behavior (Randolph-Gips, 2011). The next logical step is to bridge the gap between multiple levels (and disciplines). Two-body or second-person approaches have already drawn some connections between neural and social dynamics in neurotypical populations, and provide potentially powerful tools for the investigation of autism. Hyperscanning techniques, for instance, can be used to uncover relationships between phenotypes at the behavioral level and endophenotypes at neural levels. Inter-individual computational models combined with hyperscanning data could help elucidate causal relationships between structure and dynamics. Differences in brain anatomy may impact the ability of persons with autism to couple with others early in life thus decreasing their propensity to develop social skills (Dumas et al., 2012b). Computational neurogenetic approaches can help model the relationship between the genetics of autism and brain dynamics (Benuskova and Kasabov, 2008). Such integration of neurogenetics and systems biology may soon aid in tackling the heterogeneity observed in autism across genotype, neural endophenotype, and socio-behavioral phenotype levels.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

Guillaume Dumas and J. A. Scott Kelso are grateful for the support of NIMH Grant MH 080838.

References

Abney, D. H., Dale, R., Yoshimi, J., Kello, C. T., Tylén, K., and Fusaroli, R. (2014). Joint perceptual decision-making: a case study in explanatory pluralism. Front. Psychol. 5:330. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00330

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Adolphs, R. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 165–178. doi: 10.1038/nrn1056

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Allen, M., and Williams, G. (2011). Consciousness, plasticity, and connectomics: the role of intersubjectivity in human cognition. Front. Psychol. 2:20. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00020

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Auvray, M., Lenay, C., and Stewart, J. (2009). Perceptual interactions in a minimalist virtual environment. New Ideas Psychol. 27, 32–47. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.12.002

CrossRef Full Text

Babiloni, F., and Astolfi, L. (2012). Social neuroscience and hyperscanning techniques: past, present and future. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 44, 76–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.07.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bahrami, B., Olsen, K., Latham, P. E., Roepstorff, A., Rees, G., and Frith, C. D. (2010). Optimally interacting minds. Science 329, 1081–1085. doi: 10.1126/science.1185718

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bang, D., Fusaroli, R., Tylén, K., Olsen, K., Latham, P., Lau, J., Bahrami, B., et al. (2014). Does interaction matter? Testing whether a confidence heuristic can replace interaction in collective decision-making. Conscious. Cogn. 27, 13–23. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.02.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Benuskova, L., and Kasabov, N. (2008). Modeling brain dynamics using computational neurogenetic approach. Cogn. Neurodyn. 2, 319–334. doi: 10.1007/s11571-008-9061-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bressler, S. L., and Tognoli, E. (2006). Operational principles of neurocognitive networks. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 60, 139–148. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.12.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Burack, J. A., Iarocci, G., Bowler, D., and Mottron, L. (2002). Benefits and pitfalls in the merging of disciplines: the example of developmental psychopathology and the study of persons with autism. Dev. Psychopathol. 14, 225–237. doi: 10.1017/S095457940200202X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Castiello, U., Becchio, C., Zoia, S., Nelini, C., Sartori, L., Blason, L., et al. (2010). Wired to be social: the ontogeny of human interaction. PLoS ONE 5:e13199. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013199

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Catmur, C. (2013). Sensorimotor learning and the ontogeny of the mirror neuron system. Neurosci. Lett. 540, 21–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.10.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

De Jaegher, H. (2009). Social understanding through direct perception? Yes, by interacting. Conscious. Cogn. 18, 535–542. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.10.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

De Jaegher, H., and Di Paolo, E. (2007). Participatory sense-making. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 6, 485–507. doi: 10.1007/s11097-007-9076-9

CrossRef Full Text

De Vico Fallani, F., Nicosia, V., Sinatra, R., Astolfi, L., Cincotti, F., Mattia, D., et al. (2010). Defecting or not defecting: how to “read” human behavior during cooperative games by EEG measurements. PLoS ONE 5:e14187. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014187

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Di Paolo, E., and De Jaegher, H. (2012). The interactive brain hypothesis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:163. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00163

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dumas, G. (2011). Towards a two-body neuroscience. Commun. Integr. Biol. 4, 349. doi: 10.4161/cib.4.3.15110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dumas, G., Chavez, M., Nadel, J., and Martinerie, J. (2012b). Anatomical connectivity influences both intra-and inter-brain synchronizations. PLoS ONE 7:e36414. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036414

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dumas, G., Martinerie, J., Soussignan, R., and Nadel, J. (2012a). Does the brain know who is at the origin of what in an imitative interaction? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:128. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00128

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dumas, G., Nadel, J., Soussignan, R., Martinerie, J., and Garnero, L. (2010). Inter-brain synchronization during social interaction. PLoS ONE 5:e12166. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012166

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ferrari, P. F., Tramacere, A., Simpson, E. A., and Iriki, A. (2013). Mirror neurons through the lens of epigenetics. Trends Cohn. Sci. 17, 450–457. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.07.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ferrari, P. F., Visalberghi, E., Paukner, A., Fogassi, L., Ruggiero, A., and Suomi, S. J. (2006). Neonatal imitation in rhesus macaques. PLoS Biol. 4:e302. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040302

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fogel, A. (1993). “Two principles of communication: co-regulation and framing,” in New Perspectives in Early Communicative Development, eds J. Nadel and L. Camaioni (London: Routledge), 9–22.

Fogel, A., and Garvey, A. (2007). Alive communication. Infant Behav. Dev. 30, 251–257. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.02.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 127–138. doi: 10.1038/nrn2787

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Friston, K., Mattout, J., and Kilner, J. (2011). Action understanding and active inference. Biol. Cybern. 104, 137–160. doi: 10.1007/s00422-011-0424-z

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Froese, T., and Di Paolo, E. A. (2010). Modelling social interaction as perceptual crossing: an investigation into the dynamics of the interaction process. Connect. Sci. 22, 43–68. doi: 10.1080/09540090903197928

CrossRef Full Text

Froese, T., Gershenson, C., and Rosenblueth, D. A. (2013). “The dynamically extended mind,” in 2013 IEEE Congress on Evolutionary Computation (CEC) (Cancún: IEEE), 1419–1426. doi: 10.1109/CEC.2013.6557730

CrossRef Full Text

Froese, T., Iizuka, H., and Ikegami, T. (2014). Embodied social interaction constitutes social cognition in pairs of humans: a minimalist virtual reality experiment. Sci. Rep. 4:3672. doi: 10.1038/srep03672. Available online at: http://www.nature.com/srep/2014/140114/srep03672/full/srep03672.html

CrossRef Full Text

Fusaroli, R., Rączaszek-Leonardi, J., and Tylén, K. (2014). Dialog as interpersonal synergy. New Ideas Psychol. 32, 147–157. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2013.03.005

CrossRef Full Text

Gallese, V., Keysers, C., and Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 396–403. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.07.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gallotti, M., and Frith, C. D. (2013). Social cognition in the we-mode. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 160–165. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.02.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Guionnet, S., Nadel, J., Bertasi, E., Sperduti, M., Delaveau, P., and Fossati, P. (2012). Reciprocal imitation: toward a neural basis of social interaction. Cereb. Cortex 22, 971–978. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr177

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hari, R., and Kujala, M. V. (2009). Brain basis of human social interaction: from concepts to brain imaging. Physiol. Rev. 89, 453–479. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00041.2007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hasson, U., Ghazanfar, A. A., Galantucci, B., Garrod, S., and Keysers, C. (2012). Brain-to-brain coupling: a mechanism for creating and sharing a social world. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 114–121. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hasson, U., Nir, Y., Levy, I., Fuhrmann, G., and Malach, R. (2004). Intersubject synchronization of cortical activity during natural vision. Science 303, 1634–1640. doi: 10.1126/science.1089506

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hoel, E. P., Albantakis, L., and Tononi, G. (2013). Quantifying causal emergence shows that macro can beat micro. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 19790–19795. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1314922110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kelso, J. A. S. (1995). Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Kelso, J. A. S. (2002). The complementary nature of coordination dynamics: self-organization and the origins of agency. J. Nonlin. Phenomena Complex Syst. 5, 364–371.

Kelso, J. A., de Guzman, G. C., Reveley, C., and Tognoli, E. (2009). Virtual partner interaction (VPI): exploring novel behaviors via coordination dynamics. PLoS ONE 4:e5749. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005749

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kelso, J. A. S., Dumas, G., and Tognoli, E. (2013). Outline of a general theory of behavior and brain coordination. Neural Netw. 37, 120–131. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2012.09.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keysers, C., and Gazzola, V. (2007). Integrating simulation and theory of mind: from self to social cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 194–196. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.02.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keysers, C., and Perrett, D. I. (2004). Demystifying social cognition: a Hebbian perspective. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 501–507. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.09.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Konvalinka, I., Bauer, M., Stahlhut, C., Hansen, L. K., Roepstorff, A., and Frith, C. D. (2014). Frontal alpha oscillations distinguish leaders from followers: multivariate decoding of mutually interacting brains. Neuroimage 94, 79–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Konvalinka, I., and Roepstorff, A. (2012). The two-brain approach: how can mutually interacting brains teach us something about social interaction? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:215. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00215

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kugiumutzakis, G. (1993). “Intersubjective vocal imitation in early mother-infant interaction,” in New Perspectives in Early Communicative Development, eds J. Nadel and L. Camaioni (London: Routledge), 23–47.

Lepage, J. F., and Théoret, H. (2007). The mirror neuron system: grasping others' actions from birth? Dev. Sci. 10, 513–523. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00631.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Le Van Quyen, M. (2011). The brainweb of cross-scale interactions. New Ideas Psychol. 29, 57–63. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2010.11.001

CrossRef Full Text

Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Dev. 54, 702–709. doi: 10.2307/1130058

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Montague, P. R., Berns, G. S., Cohen, J. D., McClure, S. M., Pagnoni, G., Dhamala, M., et al. (2002). Hyperscanning: simultaneous fMRI during linked social interactions. Neuroimage 16, 1159–1164. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1150

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Müller, V., Sänger, J., and Lindenberger, U. (2013). Intra- and inter-brain synchronization during musical improvisation on the guitar. PLoS ONE 8:e73852. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073852

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nadel, J., Carchon, I., Kervella, C., Marcelli, D., and Réserbat-Plantey, D. (1999). Expectancies for social contingency in 2-month-olds. Dev. Sci. 2, 164–173. doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00065

CrossRef Full Text

Nadel, J., Croue, S., Kervella, C., Mattlinger, M.-J., Canet, P., Hudelot, C., et al. (2000). Do autistic children have ontological expectancies concerning human behavior? Autism 4, 133–145. doi: 10.1177/1362361300004002003

CrossRef Full Text

Nadel, J., and Dumas, G. (2014). The interacting body: intra- and inter-individual processes during imitation. J. Cogn. Educ. Psychol. 13, 163–175. doi: 10.1891/1945-8959.13.2.163

CrossRef Full Text

Nadel-Brulfert, J., and Baudonnière, P. M. (1982). The social function of reciprocal imitation in 2-year-old peers. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 5, 95–109. doi: 10.1177/016502548200500105

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nagy, E., Compagne, H., Orvos, H., Pal, A., Molnar, P., Janszky, I., et al. (2005). Index finger movement imitation by human neonates: motivation, learning, and left-hand preference. Pediatr. Res. 58, 749–753. doi: 10.1203/01.PDR.0000180570.28111.D9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Paukner, A., Suomi, S. J., Visalberghi, E., and Ferrari, P. F. (2009). Capuchin monkeys display affiliation toward humans who imitate them. Science 325, 880–883. doi: 10.1126/science.1176269

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pfeiffer, U. J., Timmermans, B., Bente, G., Vogeley, K., and Schilbach, L. (2011). A non-verbal turing test: differentiating mind from machine in gaze-based social interaction. PLoS ONE 6:e27591. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027591

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Piontelli, A. (2010). Development of normal fetal movements. Milan: Springer Verlag Italia. doi: 10.1007/978-88-470-1402-2

CrossRef Full Text

Plenz, D., and Niebur, E. (eds.). (2014). Criticality in Neural Systems. Weinheim: Wiley-VCH. doi: 10.1002/9783527651009

CrossRef Full Text

Randolph-Gips, M. (2011). “Autism: a systems biology disease,” in 2011 First IEEE International Conference on Healthcare Informatics, Imaging and Systems Biology (HISB) (San Jose, CA: IEEE), 359–366. doi: 10.1109/HISB.2011.13

CrossRef Full Text

Sänger, J., Müller, V., and Lindenberger, U. (2012). Intra- and inter- brain synchronization and network properties when playing guitar in duets. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:312. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00312

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sänger, J., Müller, V., and Lindenberger, U. (2013). Directionality in hyperbrain networks discriminates between leaders and followers in guitar duets. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:234. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00234

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schilbach, L. (2014). On the relationship of online and offline social cognition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:278. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00278

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Reddy, V., Costall, A., Bente, G., Schlicht, T., et al. (2013). Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behav. Brain Sci. 36, 393–414. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12000660

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sheets-Johnstone, M. (2011). The Primacy of Movement, Vol. 82. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins Publishing. doi: 10.1075/aicr.82

CrossRef Full Text

Soussignan, R., Courtial, A., Canet, P., Danon-Apter, G., and Nadel, J. (2011). Human newborns match tongue protrusion of disembodied human and robotic mouths. Dev. Sci. 14, 385–394. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00984.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sperduti, M., Guionnet, S., Fossati, P., and Nadel, J. (2014). Mirror neuron system and mentalizing system connect during online social interaction. Cogn. Process. 1–10. doi: 10.1007/s10339-014-0600-x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tognoli, E., Lagarde, J., DeGuzman, G. C., and Kelso, J. S. (2007). The phi complex as a neuromarker of human social coordination. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 104, 8190–8195. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0611453104

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tononi, G. (2008). Consciousness as integrated information: a provisional manifesto. Biol. Bull. 215, 216–242. doi: 10.2307/25470707

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Uddin, L. Q., Iacoboni, M., Lange, C., and Keenan, J. P. (2007). The self and social cognition: the role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 153–157. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.01.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wilson, K. G. (1979). Problems in physics with many scales of length. Sci. Am. 241, 140–157. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0879-158

CrossRef Full Text

Wilson, M., and Wilson, T. P. (2005). An oscillator model of the timing of turn-taking. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 12, 957–968. doi: 10.3758/BF03206432

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: multiscale, social interaction, developmental psychology, two-body neuroscience, second person perspective, hyperscanning, coordination dynamics, complex systems

Citation: Dumas G, Kelso JAS and Nadel J (2014) Tackling the social cognition paradox through multi-scale approaches. Front. Psychol. 5:882. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00882

Received: 14 May 2014; Accepted: 24 July 2014;
Published online: 12 August 2014.

Edited by:

Ezequiel Alejandro Di Paolo, Ikerbasque - Basque Foundation for Science, Spain

Reviewed by:

Viktor Müller, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Germany
Ezequiel Alejandro Di Paolo, Ikerbasque - Basque Foundation for Science, Spain

Copyright © 2014 Dumas, Kelso and Nadel. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: dumas@ccs.fau.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.