Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 03 April 2013
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic Visual Dysfunction in Schizophrenia: A View into the Mechanisms of Madness? View all 31 articles

Base rates, blindness, and schizophrenia

  • 1University Behavioral HealthCare, University of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey, Piscataway, NJ, USA
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, Robert Wood Johnson Medical School, University of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey, Piscataway, NJ, USA

A commentary on
Cognitive and neuroplasticity mechanisms by which congenital or early blindness may confer a protective effect against schizophrenia

by Silverstein, S. M., Wang, Y., and Keane, B. P. (2012). Front. Psychol. 3:624. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00624

Our paper on congenital and early (C/E) blindness and schizophrenia was an effort to account for the previously reported negative relationship between these conditions, in light of recent work indicating what the study of C/E blind people reveals about brain function and organization (Cattaneo and Vecchi, 2011; Kupers et al., 2011; Ricciardi and Pietrini, 2011). In particular, we hypothesized that the neuroplastic changes inherent to C/E blindness protect against schizophrenia by promoting strengths in those cognitive domains where schizophrenia is characterized by deficits.

Since our paper was published, we have received several comments asking a version of this question: can the lack of reports of people with both conditions be explained simply by the rarity of each condition, so that the chances of a person having both are so low that even if such people existed, it is unlikely their condition will have been reported? This is an important question, as when studying low base rate phenomena, distorted conclusions can easily be reached (Meehl and Rosen, 1955). We addressed this issue in the original version of our manuscript, but then removed it from the final version due to length considerations. Here, we note our earlier comments.

The conclusion that there are no C/E blind people with schizophrenia is based on a small number of studies that involved relatively small samples. Clearly, this argument would be strengthened by larger, population-based studies. This is because, as a simple calculation demonstrates, a case of congenital blindness and schizophrenia would be extremely rare even if there was no protective effect of blindness: if schizophrenia occurs at a rate of 0.72% in the population (McGrath et al., 2008) and congenital blindness occurs at an estimated rate of 0.03% in people born in the 1970s and 1980s (based on Robinson et al., 1987), then the joint probability of a person having both conditions, if the two are independent, would be 0.0002% or 2 out of every 1 million people. Although this is a low prevalence rate, it is equal to or higher than the rates for several other well-known conditions (e.g., Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease, hereditary spastic paraplegia, Hermansky-Pudlak Syndrome). Based on this estimated prevalence rate, in the United States alone (with a population of 311, 591, 917, as of July 2011, according the US census), there should be approximately 620 congenitally blind people with schizophrenia. When cases of blindness with an onset in the first year of life (i.e., early blindness) are taken into account, the percentage would be larger. Therefore, it is remarkable that in over 60 years, and with several investigations [including several before DSM-III (1980) when criteria for schizophrenia were broader than at present], not a single case of a C/E blind schizophrenia patient has been reported. Moreover, several published studies, and our experience as well, included surveying Directors of agencies that serve large numbers of blind people, and none of them could recall ever seeing a person who had both conditions. It is also interesting that rates of C/E blindness are significantly higher in developing, compared to industrialized, countries. Therefore, if C/E blindness did not protect against the development of schizophrenia, comorbidity would be more likely to be reported in such countries. However, this has not occurred. In short, available evidence, probabilistic estimates, and the striking contrasts, within the same domains of cognition, between superior functioning in C/E blindness and impaired functioning in schizophrenia, combine to suggest a protective relationship. If the conditions did co-occur at chance levels, reports of such cases should appear at least somewhat as often as those of many other rare medical conditions, especially since reports of an absence of schizophrenia in C/E blind people have appeared since 1950 (Chevigny and Braverman, 1950). The absence of such reports is noteworthy.

One research strategy that could generate further evidence on this issue is the study of schizotypal symptoms in the C/E blind. These subsyndromal psychotic symptoms have a higher base rate than schizophrenia itself (i.e., 10–15% vs. ~1%) (Fossati and Lenzenweger, 2009), and people with these symptoms share some of the same cognitive and biological impairments as people with the full disorder (Aichert et al., 2012; Cochrane et al., 2012). Studies of schizotypal symptoms would allow for the determination of whether C/E blindness protects against the full spectrum of schizophrenia-related illness or just schizophrenia itself. If no schizotypal symptoms were observed, this would be evidence for the existence of protective mechanisms against all schizophrenia-related psychopathology. If, however, schizotypal symptoms were found, samples of C/E blind people could be studied to gain insight into the neurobiological changes that protect against the development of clinical psychosis. As such, expanding the schizophrenia phenotype studied in the C/E blind holds potential for increasing our understanding of the mechanisms involved in schizophrenia.

In addition to these points, we noted in our original paper that other congenital sensory impairments have not been associated with absence of schizophrenia, that congenital deafness and later blindness, such as found in Usher Syndrome, whose prevalence has been estimated at 0.005% (Rosenberg et al., 1997) has been associated with psychotic disorders at rates between 4–24% (Waldeck et al., 2001), and that non-psychotic psychiatric disorders are observed in C/E blindness. These data suggest a unique relationship between C/E blindness and schizophrenia. However, we acknowledge that the absence of evidence (of people with both conditions) is not evidence of absence. That said, given the potentially important lessons for understanding, preventing, and treating schizophrenia that brain reorganization in C/E blindness provides, we believe it is useful to pursue this line of thought. At the very least, if C/E blindness did not prevent schizophrenia, then in light of the cognitive changes secondary to C/E blindness, it would be informative to determine whether it mitigates the associated cognitive impairment. Finally, we hope to raise awareness in the psychiatric community of past literature relating to C/E blindness and schizophrenia, so that if people with both conditions exist, their cases will be reported.

Acknowledgments

We thank Michael Green for his constructive criticism on an earlier draft of this commentary.

References

Aichert, D. S., Williams, S. C., Moller, H. J., Kumari, V., and Ettinger, U. (2012). Functional neural correlates of psychometric schizotypy: an fMRI study of antisaccades. Psychophysiology 49, 345–356.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cattaneo, Z., and Vecchi, T. (2011). Blind Vision. Cambridge: MIT Press.

Chevigny, H., and Braverman, S. (1950). The Adjustment of the Blind. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Cochrane, M., Petch, I., and Pickering, A. D. (2012). Aspects of cognitive functioning in schizotypy and schizophrenia: evidence for a continuum model. Psychiatry Res. 196, 230–234.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fossati, A., and Lenzenweger, M. F. (2009). Natura fascit saltus: discontinuities in the latent liability to schizophrenia and their implications for clinical psychiatry. Giorn Ital. Psicpatol 15, 219–230.

Kupers, R., Pietrini, P., Ricciardi, E., and Ptito, M. (2011). The nature of consciousness in the visually deprived brain. Front. Psychol. 2:19. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McGrath, J., Saha, S., Chant, D., and Welham, J. (2008). Schizophrenia: a concise overview of incidence, prevalence, and mortality. Epidemiol. Rev. 30, 67–76.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Meehl, P. E., and Rosen, A. (1955). Antecedent probability and the efficiency of psychometric signs, patterns, or cutting scores. Psychol. Bull. 52, 194–216.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Ricciardi, E., and Pietrini, P. (2011). New light from the dark: what blindness can teach us about brain function. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 24, 357–363.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Robinson, G. C., Jan, J. E., and Kinnis, C. (1987). Congenital ocular blindness in children, 1945 to 1984. Am. J. Dis. Child 141, 1321–1324.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rosenberg, T., Haim, M., Hauch, A. M., and Parving, A. (1997). The prevalence of Usher syndrome and other retinal dystrophy-hearing impairment associations. Clin. Genet. 51, 314–321.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Waldeck, T., Wyszynski, B., and Medalia, A. (2001). The relationship between Usher's syndrome and psychosis with Capgras syndrome. Psychiatry 64, 248–255.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Citation: Silverstein SM, Wang Y and Roché MW (2013) Base rates, blindness, and schizophrenia. Front. Psychol. 4:157. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00157

Received: 10 March 2013; Accepted: 13 March 2013;
Published online: 03 April 2013.

Edited by:

Michael Green, University of California, Los Angeles, USA

Reviewed by:

Michael Green, University of California, Los Angeles, USA

Copyright © 2013 Silverstein, Wang and Roché. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: steven_silverstein@urmc.rochester.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.