Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pharmacol., 12 September 2017
Sec. Neuropharmacology
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuropharmacological, Neurobiological and Behavioral Mechanisms of Learning and Memory View all 38 articles

Stress-Induced Reduction of Dorsal Striatal D2 Dopamine Receptors Prevents Retention of a Newly Acquired Adaptive Coping Strategy

  • 1Department of Psychology, Center ‘Daniel Bovet’, Sapienza Università di Roma, Rome, Italy
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States
  • 3Fondazione Santa Lucia (IRCCS), Rome, Italy

The inability to learn an adaptive coping strategy in a novel stressful condition leads to dysfunctional stress coping, a marker of mental disturbances. This study tested the involvement of dorsal striatal dopamine receptors in the dysfunctional coping with the Forced Swim test fostered by a previous experience of reduced food availability. Adult male mice were submitted to a temporary (12 days) reduction of food availability [food-restricted (FR)] or continuously free-fed (FF). Different groups of FF and FR mice were used to evaluate: (1) dorsal striatal mRNA levels of the two isoforms of the dopamine D2 receptor (D2S, D2L). (2) Forced Swim-induced c-fos expression in the dorsal striatum; (3) acquisition and 24 h retention of passive coping with Forced Swim. Additional groups of FF mice were tested for 24 h retention of passive coping acquired during a first experience with Forced Swim immediately followed by intra-striatal infusion of vehicle or two doses of the dopamine D2/D3 receptors antagonist sulpiride or the D1/D5 receptors antagonist SCH23390. Previous restricted feeding selectively reduced mRNA levels of both D2 isoforms and abolished Forced Swim-induced c-fos expression in the left Dorsolateral Striatum and selectively prevented 24 h retention of the coping strategy acquired in a first experience of Forced Swim. Finally, temporary blockade of left Dorsolateral Striatum D2/D3 receptors immediately following the first Forced Swim experience selectively reproduced the behavioral effect of restricted feeding in FF mice. In conclusion, the present results demonstrate that mice previously exposed to a temporary reduction of food availability show low striatal D2 receptors, a known marker of addiction-associated aberrant neuroplasticity, as well as liability to relapse into maladaptive stress coping strategies. Moreover, they offer strong support to a causal relationship between reduction of D2 receptors in the left Dorsolateral Striatum and impaired consolidation of newly acquired adaptive coping.

Introduction

The ability to respond to stressors with adaptive coping strategies is determinant for the psychological well-being of human and non-human animals (Koolhaas et al., 2010; Maier and Watkins, 2010; Cabib and Puglisi-Allegra, 2012; Helmreich et al., 2012; Pomerantz et al., 2012; Andolina et al., 2013; de Kloet and Molendijk, 2016). Indeed, dysfunctional stress coping characterizes different mental diseases (Taylor and Stanton, 2007; Aldao and Nolen-Hoeksema, 2010; Moritz et al., 2016). Therefore, animal models of stress coping have major translational value for research.

Although immobility expressed by rodents in the Forced Swim test (FSt) has been used as a measure of depressive-like behavior, there is a large consensus on the view that this behavioral response is an adaptive strategy to cope with a stressful situation that cannot be avoided nor escaped (Cabib et al., 2012; Andolina et al., 2013; Campus et al., 2015; de Kloet and Molendijk, 2016). During their first FSt experience (10 min for mice 15 min for rats) animals show initial expression of vigorous active coping (swimming around and struggling to climb the container’s walls). These responses decrease overtime whereas episodes of immobility (only small movements required to keep the head above water) increase in frequency and duration. The immobility response prevents useless and risky loss of energy, thus it is acquired and consolidated as long-term memory to be immediately adopted on subsequent encounters with the stressor (Mitchell and Meaney, 1991; Colelli et al., 2014; Reul, 2014).

Development or expression of immobility in FSt is disrupted by proximal stress experiences (Molina et al., 1994; Alcaro et al., 2002; Becker et al., 2008; Mozhui et al., 2010), an observation that further support the translational value of this animal model because proximal adverse experiences contribute to development of mental diseases (Dias-Ferreira et al., 2009; Daskalakis et al., 2013; Gourley et al., 2013; Diwadkar et al., 2014; Reul, 2014; Eagle et al., 2015). The neurobiological mechanisms mediating the disruptive effects of proximal stress experiences on subsequent coping with FSt are still poorly understood, although disturbances of learning processes could play a major role (Mitchell and Meaney, 1991; Colelli et al., 2014; Reul, 2014).

In the present study we tested the involvement of dorsal striatal D2 dopamine receptors (D2R) in impaired consolidation of newly acquired passive coping response to FSt fostered by a temporary reduction of food availability. Indeed, strong evidence supports a role for the dorsolateral striatum in consolidation of a long-term memory of immobility in FSt (Colelli et al., 2014; Campus et al., 2015) and recent findings offer support to the involvement of impaired memory consolidation in stress-induced disruption of FSt coping in mice (Campus et al., 2016). Moreover, different types of stressors have been shown to alter availability of D2R in the mouse brain (Puglisi-Allegra et al., 1991; Cabib et al., 1998; Patrono et al., 2015) and D2R play a major role in memory consolidation (Sigala et al., 1997; Setlow and McGaugh, 1999; Manago et al., 2009; Lee and Chirwa, 2015). Finally, unseasonal reduction of food availability represents an unpredictable stressor in natural settings (Wingfield and Kitaysky, 2002), thus it models an ecologically meaningful stressor in laboratory settings.

Materials and Methods

Animals and Housing

Male mice of the inbred DBA/2J strain (Charles River, Como, Italy) were purchased at 6 weeks of age and housed in groups of four in standard breeding cages with food and water ad libitum on a 12-h dark/light cycle (lights on between 07:00 and 19:00 h) at a temperature of 22 ± 1°C.

At 7 weeks of age mice were all individually housed and assigned to free-feeding (FF) or food-restricted (FR) condition. FF mice received food once daily in a quantity adjusted to exceed daily consumption (15 g). FR mice received food once daily in a quantity adjusted to lose 15% of the initial body weight within the first 3 days and maintain the reached weight for the following 9 days. On the evening of the 12th day of individual housing mice from both FF and FR groups were given food ad libitum and left undisturbed for 48 h before behavioral testing or tissue collection.

All animals were treated humanely in accordance with the principles expressed in the Declaration of Helsinki. Experimental protocols and related procedures were approved by Italian Ministry of Public Health. All efforts were made to minimize animal suffering, according to European Directive 2010/63/EU on the use of animals for research.

RNA Isolation and Gene Expression Analysis by Quantitative Real-time RT-PCR

Mice were killed by cervical dislocation and then decapitated for brains excision. Brain was quickly removed, frozen in dry ice and sliced with a freezing microtome. A stainless steel needle (1 mm inside diameter) was used to punch DLS and DMS from coronal slices no thicker than 300 μm (three slices from 1.05 to 0.15 from bregma, MBL Mouse Brain Atlas DBA/21). Locations of punches are shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. (A) mRNA level of the two isoforms of D2 dopamine receptors (short D2S; long D2L) in the dorsomedial and dorsolateral striatum of continuously free fed (FF) and temporarily food restricted (FF) mice. Data are expressed as mean arbitrary units ± SEM. (B) Punches location and dimension (example from the left hemisphere). #Main effect of the feeding condition (see Results).

Punches were processed for total RNA extraction using the RNeasy Mini kit (Qiagen). To reduce variability in the total RNA yields, the automated QiaCube instrument (Qiagen, Hilden, Germany) was used to purify RNAs. Purified RNA samples were treated with DNase I to remove genomic DNA and cDNA was synthesized using Superscript III reverse transcriptase (Life Technologies, Rockville, MD, United States), acc.to the protocol supplied by the manufacturer.

Real-time RT-PCR was performed on a EcoTM Illumina thermal cycle using QuantiFast Sybr Green PCR Master Mix (Qiagen, Inc., Valencia, CA, United States). Amplification reaction conditions were as follows: 95°C for 10 min, then 40 cycles of 95°C for 15 s and 58°C for 60 s.

Primer sequences were: D2L_forward: 5′-AACTGTACCCACCCTGAGGA-3′; D2S_forward 5′-CACCACTCAAGGATGCTGCCCG-3′; D2_reverse: 5′-GTTGCTATGTAGACCGTG-3′; β′actin_forward 5′-GAAATCGTGCGTGACATCAAAG-3′; β′actin_reverse 5′-TGTAGTTTCATGGATGCCACAG-3′. The size of amplified PCR product was 228 bp (D2L), 155 bp (D2S), and 216 bp (beta-actin).

The amplification efficiency for each primer pair was preliminarily determined by amplifying serial dilution (0.32–200 ng) of total striatum cDNA obtaining a linear standard curve. All primer pairs showed good linearity and similar amplification efficiency (98–99%) with all primers pairs. To control for products specificity, a melting temperature dissociation curve analysis was performed, from 55 to 95°C, measuring fluorescence every 0.5°C and the amplified PCR products were visualized after electrophoresis on 1.8% agarose gel. Each experiment was performed in triplicate and values were averaged. For each primer pairs, negative controls without cDNA were included to rule out genomic DNA contamination. Beta actin expression was found to be unaffected by our experimental design in all brain regions examined, and was therefore used as the internal control for normalization. Similar results were obtained when GAPDH was used for normalization.

Forced Swimming Test

Apparatus and procedures were previously described (Colelli et al., 2014; Campus et al., 2015). Briefly, the procedure consisted of a first and a second experience separated by 24 h time interval. Each mouse was gently laid in the water contained in a glass cylinder and left for 10 min on the first experience (training) and 5 min on the second (test). Both sessions were registered by a digital video camera located frontally to the apparatus and connected to a computer located within a different room.

Duration (sec) of struggling to climb out, swimming, and immobility (absence of all movements not required to float), was scored on videotapes by a trained experimenter, unaware of the experimental groups each animal belonged to, using EthoVision (Noldus Netherlands).

Immunohistochemistry

Mice were killed by cervical dislocation and then decapitated for brains excision. Brains were removed, post-fixed, and cryoprotected as described previously (Conversi et al., 2006; Colelli et al., 2010). Tissue was sliced on the coronal plane into 40 μm thick sections. Rabbit anti- c-fos (1/20000; Oncogene Sciences) was used as primary antibody. Peroxidase staining was obtained by standard avidin–biotin procedure using the rabbit Vectastain Elite ABC Kit (Vector Laboratories, Inc., Burlingame, CA, United States) and chromogenic reaction was developed by incubating sections with metal-enhanced DAB (Vector Laboratories, Inc., Burlingame, CA, United States), according to the protocol supplied by the manufacturer. Images of dorsolateral (DLS) and dorsomedial (DMS) striatum were acquired with a Nikon Eclipse 80i microscope equipped with a Nikon DS-5M CCD camera. The analysis of images was performed by using the public domain image analysis software IMAGEJ 1.48 (Abramoff et al., 2004). Immunoreactive nuclei quantification was expressed as density (number of nuclei/0.1 mm2).

Temporary Inactivation of Dopamine Receptors in the Dorsolateral Striatum

After 1 week of individual housing, continuously free-fed (FF) mice were implanted with stainless-steel guide cannulas unilaterally in the left dorsolateral striatum (DLS) as previously described (Colelli et al., 2014; Campus et al., 2015). Briefly, mice were anesthetized with Zoletil 100, Virbac, Milano, Italy (tiletamine HCl 50 mg/ml+zolazepam HCl 50 mg/ml) and Rompun 20, Bayer S.p.A Milano, Italy (xylazine 20 mg/ml) purchased commercially, and mounted on a stereotaxic apparatus (David Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA, United States). A single stainless-steel guide cannula (Unimed, Geneva, Switzerland: 7 mm in length, 0.50 mm in external diameter) was inserted in the left DLS (+0.8 mm anterior to bregma, ± 2.3 mm lateral to midline, -2.5 mm ventral from the skull according to Franklin and Paxinos, 2001). One week later, drugs were infused (total volume 0.4 μl, flow rate of 0.2 μl/min) through a stainless-steel injection cannula (Unimed, Geneva, Switzerland, 0.11 mm in internal diameter) connected by polyethylene catheter tubing to a 1 μl Hamilton micro-syringes (Hamilton, Co., Reno, NV, United States), as previously described (Colelli et al., 2014; Campus et al., 2015). Infusions were performed immediately after the firs FSt experience and behavioral test was performed 24 h later.

Doses of dopamine agonists were chosen in preliminary experiments. The D1 dopamine receptor antagonist SCH 23390 (Sigma Aldrich) was dissolved in a 0.90% saline solution and infused at one of two doses (0.5 or 1 μg/mouse); the D2/D3 dopamine receptor antagonist sulpiride (Sigma Aldrich) was first dissolved in 100% dimethyl sulfoxide (DMSO) and then diluted in 0.90% saline up to reaching two different final concentrations (0.5 or 1 μg/mouse). The final DMSO concentration never exceeded 10%; 0.90% saline or 0.90% saline +10% DMSO were infused in control (0) groups.

Experimental Protocols and Statistics

Because of previous evidences for the selective involvement of the left DLS in consolidation of a long-term memory of passive coping acquired in FSt (Colelli et al., 2014; Campus et al., 2015), we separately evaluated D2R mRNA levels and c-fos immunostaining in the left and right hemispheres and statistically tested a possible bias by including the factor ‘Hemisphere’ in the ANOVAs.

All FR mice were behaviorally tested or sacrificed after 12 days of restricted feeding followed by 48 h of free food availability (14 days of individual housing). All FF mice were behaviorally tested or sacrificed following 14 days of individual housing.

A total of eight groups of FR and of 14 groups of FF mice were used for the present experiment.

One group of FF and one group of FR mice (n = 6 each) were used to quantify D2R mRNA levels. D2R mRNA determinations obtained from triplicate experiments were averaged for each subject and statistically analyzed by three-way ANOVAs (Isoform = two levels: D2L, D2S; Feeding = two levels: FF, FR and Hemisphere = two levels: Left, Right). Additionally, we evaluated a possible difference in hemispheric availability of the two D2R isoforms in FF and FR mice separately, by two-way ANOVAs (Isoform × Hemisphere).

Two groups of FF and two groups of FR mice were used for c-fos immunostaining experiments. Six mice from each feeding condition were sacrificed immediately after removal from their home cages (naïve). Six mice from each feeding condition were sacrificed 50 min after a first experience of forced swim (10 min, FSt). Statistical analyses of c-fos data were performed on number of immunostained nuclei/0.1 mm2 from each sampled area. Three-way ANOVAs were used (two between factors: Experience of forced swim = two levels: Naïve, Experienced; Feeding = two levels: FF, FR; and one within factor: Hemisphere = two levels: Left, Right). When allowed by the results, individual between-groups comparisons were performed post hoc (Sidak correction).

A group of FF (n = 6) and a group of FR (n = 6) mice were used to evaluate the effects of a previous experience of restricted feeding on behavioral responses to FSt. Statistical analyses were performed on duration (sec) of the different behaviors expressed in the first and second block of 5 min of the first FSt experience and on the 5 min test performed 24 h later. A two-way ANOVAs for repeated measures (min blocks, three levels: 0–5, 6–10 min, test) with Feeding as between factor was employed. When allowed by results obtained, significant difference with behavior expressed during the first 5 min of exposure to FSt was tested post hoc (Paired t-test).

Six groups (one group for vehicle and one for each of the two doses of each antagonist) of FF mice (n = 7 each) were used to evaluate the effects of immediate post-experience infusion of DA antagonist in the left DLS on consolidation of a passive coping response (immobility). All mice were exposed to 10 min of forced swimming on the 14th day of individual housing and tested the following day. Immobility duration (sec) expressed on the final test was statistically analyzed by a two-way ANOVA for independent measures (Feeding, two levels = FF, FR; Dose, three levels = 0, 0.5, 1 μg/mouse). When allowed, individual between-groups comparisons were performed post hoc (Sidak correction).

Results

Restricted Feeding Selectively Reduced D2R mRNA Level in the Left Dorsolateral Striatum

Figure 1 shows data on dorsal striatal D2L and D2S mRNA levels indicating a selective decrease of both D2R isoform limitedly to the left dorsolateral striatum (DLS) of FR mice.

Indeed, statistical analyses only revealed a significant interaction between the three factors (Feeding × Hemisphere × Isoform) in the DLS [F(1,40) = 7.307; p < 0.05] due to significant [F(1,10) = 7.29; p < 0.05] reduction of mRNA levels of both isoforms in the FR group (Figure 1).

Because larger D2R availability has been observed in the left striatum of rats and healthy humans (Schneider et al., 1982; Tomer et al., 2008), we separately compared hemispheric levels of the two D2R isoforms in the different striatal compartment of FF and FR mice. A significant hemispheric difference was found in the DLS of FF mice only, due to a left bias for both isoforms [F(1,20) = 5.136; p < 0.05].

Restricted Feeding Selectively Prevented Forced Swim-Induced c-fos Expression in the Left Dorsolateral Striatum

In Figure 2 are shown data on induction of c-fos immunostaining by a first FSt (10 min) experience in the right and left striatum of FF and FR mice. These data indicate a selective induction of c-fos expression in the left DLS of FF mice that was absent in FR mice.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Effects of 12 days or restricted feeding on c-fos immunostaining promoted by a first experience (10 min) of Forced Swim. (A) Quantitative data (mean density/0.1 mm2 ± SEM). #Main effect of the Forced Swimming experience (see Results). xp < 0.005 vs. Naive (not exposed to FS) (Sidak correction for multiple comparisons). p < 0.05 vs. continuously free-fed (FF) mice exposed to Forced Swim (Sidak correction for multiple comparisons). (B) Representative staining of samples from the left DMS (top) and left DLS of continuously free-fed (FF) and temporarily food-restricted (FR) mice exposed to 10 min of forced swimming.

Thus, statistical analyses revealed a significant main effect of FSt experience in the dorsomedial striatum (DMS) [F(1,40) = 38.36; p < 0.0001] due to a significant increase of c-fos immunostaining in FSt-experienced mice regardless of the hemisphere or of the feeding condition. A significant global interaction was revealed for c-fos expression in the DLS [F(1,40) = 4.182; p < 0.05] due to a significant c-fos expression in the left DLS of FF mice only (Figure 2).

Either Restricted Feeding or Pharmacological Inactivation of D2/D3 Receptors in the Left Dorsolateral Striatum Prevented 24 h Retention of the Passive Coping Strategy Acquired in the First Experience with Forced Swim Test

Data on behavior expressed in FSt are presented in Figure 3; they indicate that whereas both FF and FR mice developed a passive coping strategy in the course of the first experience with FSt only FF mice retained this strategy for the following 24 h. Moreover, they indicate that pharmacological blockade of D2/D3R in the left DLS immediately after the first FSt experience reproduces the behavioral effects of restricted feeding in FF mice.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. (A) Effects of restricted feeding on acquisition (0–5, 5–10 min) and retrieval (Test) of immobility (top), swimming (middle), and struggling (bottom) behavior. (B) Effects of immediate post-training infusion of the D1 receptor antagonist SCH 23390 (top) or the D2 receptor antagonist sulpiride (bottom) in the left DLS on immobility expressed on the retention test performed 24 h later. Data are expressed as mean duration (±SEM) of behavior. xp < 0.05 vs. behavior expressed during the first 5 min of FS experience (0–5) (paired t-test). @p < 0.05 vs. vehicle infusion (0) (Sidak correction for multiple comparisons).

Figure 3A shows mean duration of the different behaviors expressed during the first (two blocks: 0–5, 6–10 min) and second (test: 5 min) experience of FSt by FF and FR mice. Statistical analyses revealed a significant interaction between factors (Feeding × Repeated measure) for both immobility: F(2,20) = 4.251; p < 0.05, and Swim: F(2,20) = 5.541; p < 0.05. Post hoc comparisons (paired t-test) revealed a significant increase of immobility and a significant reduction of swimming between the first and second 5 min of the first FSt experience regardless of the feeding condition. Levels of immobility and swimming were still significantly different from those expressed during the first 5 min of FSt experience in FF mice tested 24 h later but not in FR mice (Figure 3A).

In Figure 3B are reported data on the effects of immediately post-FSt unilateral infusion of SCH23390 (D1 antagonist) or Sulpiride (D2/D3 antagonist) in the left DLS on immobility expressed on the retrieval test performed 24 h later. Statistical analyses did not reveal any significant effect of the D1R antagonist whereas infusion of the D2/D3 antagonist dose-dependently reduced the amount of immobility expressed on the retrieval test [F(2,17) = 4.45; p < 0.05].

Discussion

Major findings of the present study are: (1) the decrease of D2R mRNA in the left DLS of FR mice; (2) the selective inhibition of FSt-induced c-fos expression in the left DLS of FR mice; (3) the shared ability of pharmacological blockade of D2/D3R in the left DLS and of previous experience of reduced food availability to prevent 24 h retention of FSt-induced immobility.

In a first set of experiments we observed a selective reduction of D2R mRNA levels in the left DLS of FR mice. To the best of our knowledge this is the first report of a reduction of striatal D2R availability following exposure to adverse environmental conditions in mice. Indeed, previous studies using different chronic/repeated stress protocols and different measures of receptors availability reported an increase of D2R in the ventral striatum and no significant changes in the dorsal striatum of stressed mice (Cabib et al., 1988, 1998; Lim et al., 2011; Patrono et al., 2015).

Reduced striatal D2R availability is considered a main marker of addiction-associated aberrant plasticity because it has been reported in human addicts (Martinez et al., 2004; Tomasi and Volkow, 2013), in monkeys after chronic escalating methamphetamine (Groman et al., 2012) or prolonged cocaine self-administration (Nader et al., 2006), and in rats following cocaine self-administration (Besson et al., 2013). Thus, the finding of the present study is coherent with the ability of restricted feeding to produce phenotypes fostered by prolonged exposure to addictive drugs in laboratory animals (Carr, 2011; Zheng et al., 2012; Branch et al., 2013). Indeed, mice exposed to the protocol used for the present experiments show behavioral markers of addiction-associated aberrant neuroplasticity (Cabib et al., 2000).

Because of previous observations that the splicing of the D2R gene might influence addiction liability in humans (Moyer et al., 2011) and is influenced by exposure to addictive drugs in mice (Giordano et al., 2006), we measured the relative abundance of the two D2R isoforms. Our finding that both D2L and D2S undergo a similar decrease of their expression level in the left DLS of FR mice do not support an effect of restricted feeding on D2R gene splicing, pinpointing a possible modulation of the transcriptional activation of this gene.

Finally, the present study revealed that FR-induced decrease of D2R mRNA was confined to the left DLS. This finding could explain previous failure to demonstrate significant effects of stress on DLS D2R availability (Cabib et al., 1988, 1998; Lim et al., 2011; Patrono et al., 2015). Moreover, it is in line with evidences of lateralized brain stress responses (Berridge et al., 1999; Sullivan and Dufresne, 2006; Cerqueira et al., 2008; Lupinsky et al., 2010) and with lateralization of mesostriatal DA transmission (Schneider et al., 1982; Molochnikov and Cohen, 2014). To this regard, it is worth pointing out that higher levels of D2R have been observed in the left striatum in both healthy humans and rats (Schneider et al., 1982; Tomer et al., 2008), a phenomenon confirmed by results of the present experiments in the DLS of FF but not FR mice.

Because of our previous finding of a main role of the left DLS in consolidation of immobility acquired in FSt (Colelli et al., 2014), the selective reduction of D2R in the left DLS of FR mice was strongly suggestive of an alteration of the memory processes engaged by FSt in these mice. Therefore, we evaluated the effects of a previous FR experience on development and 24 h retention of FSt-induced passive coping. The results obtained indicated a selective effect of restricted feeding on retention rather then on expression or acquisition of the coping strategy (Figure 3A), supporting a role for altered memory processing in the impaired coping with FSt observed in FR mice.

The experience of restricted feeding also abolished FSt-induced c-fos expression in the left DLS (Figure 2). Indeed, in line with previous findings (Colelli et al., 2014), FF mice responded to a first 10 min-long FSt experience with increased c-fos expression in the left DLS a response that was not observable in FR mice. The effect of restricted feeding on c-fos expression induced by a first experience of forced swim support the hypothesis of altered consolidation of a newly acquired passive coping strategy. Indeed, expression of c-fos has been associated and causally related with consolidation of long-term memories (Lamprecht and Dudai, 1996; Bilang-Bleuel et al., 2002; Guzowski, 2002; He et al., 2002; Herry and Mons, 2004; Katche et al., 2010; Stafford et al., 2012; Reul, 2014; Saunderson et al., 2016). On the other hand, there is strong evidence for a major involvement of D2R in DA-dependent induction of striatal c-fos expression (Badiani et al., 1999; Bertran-Gonzalez et al., 2008; Kharkwal et al., 2016), thus these finding indirectly support a role for reduced D2R availability and reduced FSt-induced c-fos-expression in the left DLS of FR mice.

In a final set of experiments we directly tested the hypothesis that reduced D2R availability in the left DLS impair consolidation of passive coping acquired in FSt. To this aim we infused either a D2/D3 or of a D1 receptors antagonist in the left DLS immediately after the first FSt experience because post-training manipulations influence the consolidation of learning as long-term memory without affecting retrieval (McGaugh, 2000). The results obtained demonstrated that selective blockade of D2/D3R in the left DLS prevents 24 h retention of the immobility response acquired in the first FSt experience by FF mice. Indeed unilateral infusion of sulpiride, but not of SCH23390, in the left DLS dose-dependently reduced immobility expressed on the retention test performed 24 h later.

It should be pointed out that although sulpiride is a mixed D2/D3 receptors antagonist, there are very few D3R in the DLS (Murray et al., 1994; Erritzoe et al., 2014). Moreover, stimulation of D3R has been shown to interfere with all types of learning whereas stimulation of D2R facilitates consolidation of lasting memory (Sigala et al., 1997; Manago et al., 2009; Nakajima et al., 2013; Vicente et al., 2016). Therefore, the findings of this set of experiments offer support to a causal relationship between reduced availability of D2R in the left DLS of FR mice and impaired consolidation of coping acquired in FSt.

Conclusion

The results of the present study support the conclusion that reduced dorsal striatal D2R availability fostered by a proximal adverse experience in adult mice can disrupt consolidation of newly acquired coping strategies leading to expression of dysfunctional coping on subsequent encounters with the stressor.

This conclusion has high translational value. Indeed, unseasonal reduction of food availability represents an unpredictable stressor in natural settings (Wingfield and Kitaysky, 2002), thus it models an ecologically meaningful stressor in laboratory settings. Moreover, altered learning mechanisms have been involved in the development of different behavioral disturbances (Robbins and Everitt, 2002; Goodman et al., 2014; Gillan et al., 2016). In addition, low striatal D2R availability has been reported in behavioral disturbances not associated with experience of addictive drugs (Wang et al., 2001; Denys et al., 2004; Schneier et al., 2007; Perani et al., 2008; Tomasi and Volkow, 2013; Broft et al., 2015). Finally, there is strong evidence that the brain circuit supporting consolidation of passive coping in FSt is also involved in response extinction (Campus et al., 2015, 2016; Goodman et al., 2016), suggesting a general role of reduced dorsal striatal D2R in liability to relapse into maladaptive behavior.

Author Contributions

PC and SiC designed the research and analyzed and interpreted the data; PC and CO performed behavioral experiments; PC performed c-fos experiments as well as pharmacological experiments, MF and SoC collected, analyzed and interpreted data on striatal levels of D2R mRNA; SP-A, CO, and MF revised the work critically for important intellectual content. SiC wrote the manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by Sapienza Università di Roma (Ateneo 2015; 2016).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^http://www.mbl.org/atlas165/atlas165_start.html

References

Abramoff, M. D., Magelhaes, P. J., and Ram, S. J. (2004). Image processing with Imagej. Biophoton. Int. 11, 36–42.

Google Scholar

Alcaro, A., Cabib, S., Ventura, R., and Puglisi-Allegra, S. (2002). Genotype- and experience-dependent susceptibility to depressive-like responses in the forced-swimming test. Psychopharmacology 164, 138–143. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1161-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aldao, A., and Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2010). Specificity of cognitive emotion regulation strategies: a transdiagnostic examination. Behav. Res. Ther. 48, 974–983. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andolina, D., Maran, D., Valzania, A., Conversi, D., and Puglisi-Allegra, S. (2013). Prefrontal/amygdalar system determines stress coping behavior through 5-HT/GABA connection. Neuropsychopharmacology 38, 2057–2067. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Badiani, A., Oates, M. M., Day, H. E., Watson, S. J., Akil, H., and Robinson, T. E. (1999). Environmental modulation of amphetamine-induced c-fos expression in D1 versus D2 striatal neurons. Behav. Brain Res. 103, 203–209. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00041-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, C., Zeau, B., Rivat, C., Blugeot, A., Hamon, M., and Benoliel, J. J. (2008). Repeated social defeat-induced depression-like behavioral and biological alterations in rats: involvement of cholecystokinin. Mol. Psychiatry 13, 1079–1092. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, C. W., Mitton, E., Clark, W., and Roth, R. H. (1999). Engagement in a non-escape (displacement) behavior elicits a selective and lateralized suppression of frontal cortical dopaminergic utilization in stress. Synapse 32, 187–197. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2396(19990601)32:3<187::AID-SYN5>3.0.CO;2-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertran-Gonzalez, J., Bosch, C., Maroteaux, M., Matamales, M., Herve, D., Valjent, E., et al. (2008). Opposing patterns of signaling activation in dopamine D1 and D2 receptor-expressing striatal neurons in response to cocaine and haloperidol. J. Neurosci. 28, 5671–5685. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1039-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Besson, M., Pelloux, Y., Dilleen, R., Theobald, D. E., Lyon, A., Belin-Rauscent, A., et al. (2013). Cocaine modulation of frontostriatal expression of Zif268, D2, and 5-HT2c receptors in high and low impulsive rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 38, 1963–1973. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.95

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bilang-Bleuel, A., Rech, J., De Carli, S., Holsboer, F., and Reul, J. M. (2002). Forced swimming evokes a biphasic response in CREB phosphorylation in extrahypothalamic limbic and neocortical brain structures in the rat. Eur. J. Neurosci. 15, 1048–1060. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.01934.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Branch, S. Y., Goertz, R. B., Sharpe, A. L., Pierce, J., Roy, S., Ko, D., et al. (2013). Food restriction increases glutamate receptor-mediated burst firing of dopamine neurons. J. Neurosci. 33, 13861–13872. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5099-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broft, A., Slifstein, M., Osborne, J., Kothari, P., Morim, S., Shingleton, R., et al. (2015). Striatal dopamine type 2 receptor availability in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 233, 380–387. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.06.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabib, S., Campus, P., and Colelli, V. (2012). Learning to cope with stress: psychobiological mechanisms of stress resilience. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 659–672. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2012-0080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabib, S., Giardino, L., Calza, L., Zanni, M., Mele, A., and Puglisi-Allegra, S. (1998). Stress promotes major changes in dopamine receptor densities within the mesoaccumbens and nigrostriatal systems. Neuroscience 84, 193–200. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(97)00468-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabib, S., Kempf, E., Schleef, C., Oliverio, A., and Puglisi-Allegra, S. (1988). Effects of immobilization stress on dopamine and its metabolites in different brain areas of the mouse: role of genotype and stress duration. Brain Res. 441, 153–160. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(88)91393-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabib, S., Orsini, C., Le Moal, M., and Piazza, P. V. (2000). Abolition and reversal of strain differences in behavioral responses to drugs of abuse after a brief experience. Science 289, 463–465. doi: 10.1126/science.289.5478.463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabib, S., and Puglisi-Allegra, S. (2012). The mesoaccumbens dopamine in coping with stress. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 79–89. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campus, P., Colelli, V., Orsini, C., Sarra, D., and Cabib, S. (2015). Evidence for the involvement of extinction-associated inhibitory learning in the forced swimming test. Behav. Brain Res. 278, 348–355. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campus, P., Maiolati, M., Orsini, C., and Cabib, S. (2016). Altered consolidation of extinction-like inhibitory learning in genotype-specific dysfunctional coping fostered by chronic stress in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 315, 23–35. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carr, K. D. (2011). Food scarcity, neuroadaptations, and the pathogenic potential of dieting in an unnatural ecology: binge eating and drug abuse. Physiol. Behav. 104, 162–167. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.04.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cerqueira, J. J., Almeida, O. F., and Sousa, N. (2008). The stressed prefrontal cortex. Left? Right! Brain Behav. Immun. 22, 630–638. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2008.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colelli, V., Campus, P., Conversi, D., Orsini, C., and Cabib, S. (2014). Either the dorsal hippocampus or the dorsolateral striatum is selectively involved in consolidation of forced swim-induced immobility depending on genetic background. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 111, 49–55. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colelli, V., Fiorenza, M. T., Conversi, D., Orsini, C., and Cabib, S. (2010). Strain-specific proportion of the two isoforms of the dopamine D2 receptor in the mouse striatum: associated neural and behavioral phenotypes. Genes Brain Behav. 9, 703–711. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.00604.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conversi, D., Bonito-Oliva, A., Orsini, C., and Cabib, S. (2006). Habituation to the test cage influences amphetamine-induced locomotion and Fos expression and increases FosB/DeltaFosB-like immunoreactivity in mice. Neuroscience 141, 597–605. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daskalakis, N. P., Bagot, R. C., Parker, K. J., Vinkers, C. H., and De Kloet, E. R. (2013). The three-hit concept of vulnerability and resilience: toward understanding adaptation to early-life adversity outcome. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 1858–1873. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Kloet, E. R., and Molendijk, M. L. (2016). Coping with the forced swim stressor: towards understanding an adaptive mechanism. Neural Plast. 2016:6503162. doi: 10.1155/2016/6503162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denys, D., Van Der Wee, N., Janssen, J., De Geus, F., and Westenberg, H. G. (2004). Low level of dopaminergic D2 receptor binding in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 55, 1041–1045. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.01.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dias-Ferreira, E., Sousa, J. C., Melo, I., Morgado, P., Mesquita, A. R., Cerqueira, J. J., et al. (2009). Chronic stress causes frontostriatal reorganization and affects decision-making. Science 325, 621–625. doi: 10.1126/science.1171203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diwadkar, V. A., Bustamante, A., Rai, H., and Uddin, M. (2014). Epigenetics, stress, and their potential impact on brain network function: a focus on the schizophrenia diatheses. Front. Psychiatry 5:71. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagle, A. L., Gajewski, P. A., Yang, M., Kechner, M. E., Al Masraf, B. S., Kennedy, P. J., et al. (2015). Experience-dependent induction of hippocampal DeltaFosB controls learning. J. Neurosci. 35, 13773–13783. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2083-15.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erritzoe, D., Tziortzi, A., Bargiela, D., Colasanti, A., Searle, G. E., Gunn, R. N., et al. (2014). In vivo imaging of cerebral dopamine D3 receptors in alcoholism. Neuropsychopharmacology 39, 1703–1712. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.18

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franklin, K. B. J., and Paxinos, G. (2001). The Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Gillan, C. M., Robbins, T. W., Sahakian, B. J., Van Den Heuvel, O. A., and Van Wingen, G. (2016). The role of habit in compulsivity. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 26, 828–840. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.12.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giordano, T. P. III., Satpute, S. S., Striessnig, J., Kosofsky, B. E., and Rajadhyaksha, A. M. (2006). Up-regulation of dopamine D(2)L mRNA levels in the ventral tegmental area and dorsal striatum of amphetamine-sensitized C57BL/6 mice: role of Ca(v)1.3 L-type Ca(2+) channels. J. Neurochem. 99, 1197–1206. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2006.04186.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodman, J., Marsh, R., Peterson, B. S., and Packard, M. G. (2014). Annual research review: the neurobehavioral development of multiple memory systems: implications for childhood and adolescent psychiatric disorders. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 55, 582–610. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodman, J., Ressler, R. L., and Packard, M. G. (2016). The dorsolateral striatum selectively mediates extinction of habit memory. Neurobiol. Learn. Memory 136, 54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gourley, S. L., Swanson, A. M., and Koleske, A. J. (2013). Corticosteroid-induced neural remodeling predicts behavioral vulnerability and resilience. J. Neurosci. 33, 3107–3112. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2138-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Groman, S. M., Lee, B., Seu, E., James, A. S., Feiler, K., Mandelkern, M. A., et al. (2012). Dysregulation of D(2)-mediated dopamine transmission in monkeys after chronic escalating methamphetamine exposure. J. Neurosci. 32, 5843–5852. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0029-12.2012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guzowski, J. F. (2002). Insights into immediate-early gene function in hippocampal memory consolidation using antisense oligonucleotide and fluorescent imaging approaches. Hippocampus 12, 86–104. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, J., Yamada, K., and Nabeshima, T. (2002). A role of Fos expression in the CA3 region of the hippocampus in spatial memory formation in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 26, 259–268. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00332-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helmreich, D. L., Tylee, D., Christianson, J. P., Kubala, K. H., Govindarajan, S. T., O’neill, W. E., et al. (2012). Active behavioral coping alters the behavioral but not the endocrine response to stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 1941–1948. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herry, C., and Mons, N. (2004). Resistance to extinction is associated with impaired immediate early gene induction in medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20, 781–790. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03542.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katche, C., Bekinschtein, P., Slipczuk, L., Goldin, A., Izquierdo, I. A., Cammarota, M., et al. (2010). Delayed wave of c-Fos expression in the dorsal hippocampus involved specifically in persistence of long-term memory storage. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 349–354. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0912931107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kharkwal, G., Radl, D., Lewis, R., and Borrelli, E. (2016). Dopamine D2 receptors in striatal output neurons enable the psychomotor effects of cocaine. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, 11609–11614. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1608362113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koolhaas, J. M., De Boer, S. F., Coppens, C. M., and Buwalda, B. (2010). Neuroendocrinology of coping styles: towards understanding the biology of individual variation. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 31:307–321. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2010.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamprecht, R., and Dudai, Y. (1996). Transient expression of c-Fos in rat amygdala during training is required for encoding conditioned taste aversion memory. Learn. Mem. 3, 31–41. doi: 10.1101/lm.3.1.31

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, K. N., and Chirwa, S. (2015). Blocking dopaminergic signaling soon after learning impairs memory consolidation in guinea pigs. PLOS ONE 10:e0135578. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0135578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, M. M., Xu, J., Holtzman, D. M., and Mach, R. H. (2011). Sleep deprivation differentially affects dopamine receptor subtypes in mouse striatum. Neuroreport 22, 489–493. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32834846a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lupinsky, D., Moquin, L., and Gratton, A. (2010). Interhemispheric regulation of the medial prefrontal cortical glutamate stress response in rats. J. Neurosci. 30, 7624–7633. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1187-10.2010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maier, S. F., and Watkins, L. R. (2010). Role of the medial prefrontal cortex in coping and resilience. Brain Res. 1355, 52–60. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.08.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manago, F., Castellano, C., Oliverio, A., Mele, A., and De Leonibus, E. (2009). Role of dopamine receptors subtypes, D1-like and D2-like, within the nucleus accumbens subregions, core and shell, on memory consolidation in the one-trial inhibitory avoidance task. Learn. Mem. 16, 46–52. doi: 10.1101/lm.1177509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinez, D., Broft, A., Foltin, R. W., Slifstein, M., Hwang, D.-R., Huang, Y., et al. (2004). Cocaine dependence and D2 receptor availability in the functional subdivisions of the striatum: relationship with cocaine-seeking behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 1190–1202. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGaugh, J. L. (2000). Memory–a century of consolidation. Science 287, 248–251. doi: 10.1126/science.287.5451.248

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, J. B., and Meaney, M. J. (1991). Effects of corticosterone on response consolidation and retrieval in the forced swim test. Behav. Neurosci. 105, 798–803. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.105.6.798

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molina, V. A., Heyser, C. J., and Spear, L. P. (1994). Chronic variable stress or chronic morphine facilitates immobility in a forced swim test: reversal by naloxone. Psychopharmacology 114, 433–440. doi: 10.1007/BF02249333

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molochnikov, I., and Cohen, D. (2014). Hemispheric differences in the mesostriatal dopaminergic system. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:110. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moritz, S., Ludtke, T., Westermann, S., Hermeneit, J., Watroba, J., and Lincoln, T. M. (2016). Dysfunctional coping with stress in psychosis. An investigation with the maladaptive and adaptive coping styles (MAX) questionnaire. Schizophr. Res. 175, 129–135. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2016.04.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moyer, R. A., Wang, D., Papp, A. C., Smith, R. M., Duque, L., Mash, D. C., et al. (2011). Intronic polymorphisms affecting alternative splicing of human dopamine D2 receptor are associated with cocaine abuse. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 753–762. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mozhui, K., Karlsson, R. M., Kash, T. L., Ihne, J., Norcross, M., Patel, S., et al. (2010). Strain differences in stress responsivity are associated with divergent amygdala gene expression and glutamate-mediated neuronal excitability. J. Neurosci. 30, 5357–5367. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5017-09.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, A. M., Ryoo, H. L., Gurevich, E., and Joyce, J. N. (1994). Localization of dopamine D3 receptors to mesolimbic and D2 receptors to mesostriatal regions of human forebrain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 91, 11271–11275. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.23.11271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nader, M. A., Morgan, D., Gage, H. D., Nader, S. H., Calhoun, T. L., Buchheimer, N., et al. (2006). PET imaging of dopamine D2 receptors during chronic cocaine self-administration in monkeys. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1050–1056. doi: 10.1038/nn1737

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakajima, S., Gerretsen, P., Takeuchi, H., Caravaggio, F., Chow, T., Le Foll, B., et al. (2013). The potential role of dopamine D(3) receptor neurotransmission in cognition. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 23, 799–813. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2013.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patrono, E., Di Segni, M., Patella, L., Andolina, D., Valzania, A., Latagliata, E. C., et al. (2015). When chocolate seeking becomes compulsion: gene-environment interplay. PLOS ONE 10:e0120191. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0120191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perani, D., Garibotto, V., Gorini, A., Moresco, R. M., Henin, M., Panzacchi, A., et al. (2008). In vivo PET study of 5HT(2A) serotonin and D(2) dopamine dysfunction in drug-naive obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuroimage 42, 306–314. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pomerantz, O., Paukner, A., and Terkel, J. (2012). Some stereotypic behaviors in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) are correlated with both perseveration and the ability to cope with acute stressors. Behav. Brain Res. 230, 274–280. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.02.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puglisi-Allegra, S., Kempf, E., Schleef, C., and Cabib, S. (1991). Repeated stressful experiences differently affect brain dopamine receptor subtypes. Life Sci. 48, 1263–1268. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(91)90521-C

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reul, J. M. (2014). Making memories of stressful events: a journey along epigenetic, gene transcription, and signaling pathways. Front. Psychiatry 5:5. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robbins, T. W., and Everitt, B. J. (2002). Limbic-striatal memory systems and drug addiction. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 78, 625–636. doi: 10.1006/nlme.2002.4103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saunderson, E. A., Spiers, H., Mifsud, K. R., Gutierrez-Mecinas, M., Trollope, A. F., Shaikh, A., et al. (2016). Stress-induced gene expression and behavior are controlled by DNA methylation and methyl donor availability in the dentate gyrus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, 4830–4835. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1524857113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, L. H., Murphy, R. B., and Coons, E. E. (1982). Lateralization of striatal dopamine (D2) receptors in normal rats. Neurosci. Lett. 33, 281–284. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(82)90385-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneier, F. R., Martinez, D., Abi-Dargham, A., Zea-Ponce, Y., Simpson, H. B., Liebowitz, M. R., et al. (2007). Striatal dopamine D(2) receptor availability in OCD with and without comorbid social anxiety disorder: preliminary findings. Depress Anxiety 25, 1–7. doi: 10.1002/da.20268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Setlow, B., and McGaugh, J. L. (1999). Involvement of the posteroventral caudate-putamen in memory consolidation in the Morris water maze. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 71, 240–247. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1998.3874

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sigala, S., Missale, C., and Spano, P. (1997). Opposite effects of dopamine D2 and D3 receptors on learning and memory in the rat. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 336, 107–112. doi: 10.1016/S0014-2999(97)01235-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stafford, J. M., Raybuck, J. D., Ryabinin, A. E., and Lattal, K. M. (2012). Increasing histone acetylation in the hippocampus-infralimbic network enhances fear extinction. Biol. Psychiatry 72, 25–33. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sullivan, R. M., and Dufresne, M. M. (2006). Mesocortical dopamine and HPA axis regulation: role of laterality and early environment. Brain Res. 1076, 49–59. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.12.100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, A. E., and Stanton, A. L. (2007). Coping resources, coping processes, and mental health. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 3, 377–401. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasi, D., and Volkow, N. D. (2013). Striatocortical pathway dysfunction in addiction and obesity: differences and similarities. Crit. Rev. Biochem. Mol. Biol. 48, 1–19. doi: 10.3109/10409238.2012.735642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomer, R., Goldstein, R. Z., Wang, G. J., Wong, C., and Volkow, N. D. (2008). Incentive motivation is associated with striatal dopamine asymmetry. Biol. Psychol. 77, 98–101. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vicente, A. M., Galvao-Ferreira, P., Tecuapetla, F., and Costa, R. M. (2016). Direct and indirect dorsolateral striatum pathways reinforce different action strategies. Curr. Biol. 26, R267–R269. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.02.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, G.-J., Volkow, N. D., Logan, J., Pappas, N. R., Wong, C. T., Zhu, W., et al. (2001). Brain dopamine and obesity. Lancet 357, 354–357. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)03643-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wingfield, J. C., and Kitaysky, A. S. (2002). Endocrine responses to unpredictable environmental events: stress or anti-stress hormones? Integr. Comp. Biol. 42, 600–609. doi: 10.1093/icb/42.3.600

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, D., De Vaca, S. C., and Carr, K. D. (2012). Food restriction increases acquisition, persistence and drug prime-induced expression of a cocaine-conditioned place preference in rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 100, 538–544. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2011.10.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dopamine receptors, dorsolateral striatum, helplessness behavior, hemispheric bias, memory consolidation, sustained threat

Citation: Campus P, Canterini S, Orsini C, Fiorenza MT, Puglisi-Allegra S and Cabib S (2017) Stress-Induced Reduction of Dorsal Striatal D2 Dopamine Receptors Prevents Retention of a Newly Acquired Adaptive Coping Strategy. Front. Pharmacol. 8:621. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2017.00621

Received: 02 July 2017; Accepted: 24 August 2017;
Published: 12 September 2017.

Edited by:

Antonella Gasbarri, University of L’Aquila, Italy

Reviewed by:

Viviana Trezza, Roma Tre University, Italy
Gina Lorena Quirarte, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico

Copyright © 2017 Campus, Canterini, Orsini, Fiorenza, Puglisi-Allegra and Cabib. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Simona Cabib, simona.cabib@uniroma1.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.