Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pain Res., 16 December 2022
Sec. Pain Research Methods
Volume 3 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpain.2022.1037472

Lifetime ostracism experiences and mechanisms of pain

  • 1Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, United States
  • 2Department of Behavioral Health, Houston Methodist Hospital, Houston, TX, United States
  • 3Department of Psychology, University of Oklahoma, Norman, OK, United States
  • 4Diversity Science Research Cluster, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, United States
  • 5Texas A&M Institute for Neuroscience, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, United States

One social mechanism by which marginalization is enacted is via ostracism. Recent research has demonstrated ostracism's impact on physical health, but little is known about the relationship between accumulated lifetime experiences of ostracism and pain. Despite recent calls for added attention to social modulation of pain and social indicators of pain disparities, the impact of specific social factors on pain—including those of ostracism—are not well understood. Results of laboratory studies on the effects of acute ostracism experiences on pain sensitivity have been mixed. However, these studies have not considered lived and repeated experiences of ostracism, and primarily included single static measures of pain sensitivity. Additionally, inclusion and representation of the relationship between ostracism experiences and pain among people with minoritized identities are lacking in the current literature. In this study, we explored accumulated lifetime experiences of ostracism as a potential contributing factor to enhanced pain and one social mechanism by which societal inequity may create and maintain inequity in pain. We extracted measures of lifetime experiences of ostracism from six studies focused on social factors and (non-chronic) pain conducted between 2016 and 2020 (n = 505 adults). To retain and examine diversity within the sample, we used moderation and within-group analyses. Results indicate that greater experiences of lifetime ostracism are associated with lower cold pain tolerance, but not other pain measures, in the whole sample. Moderation and within-group analyses reveal opposing patterns of results between populations included in the extant literature (White participants, convenience samples) and those under-represented in the scientific literature (racialized groups, community samples). This study provides an example of a diversity science approach to examining social indicators of pain, illustrates the limited generalizability of previous studies on ostracism and pain, and highlights the need for increased representation and inclusion to understand mechanisms of pain and inequity.

1. Introduction

Minoritized communities experience a disproportionate burden of illness and disease. These health disparities are driven by systemic inequality embedded within institutions that serve to marginalize and disadvantage groups across socially constructed demographic lines (1, 2). One social mechanism that serves to marginalize others and has demonstrated impact on health is social ostracism. Ostracism, or the experience of being ignored or socially excluded by others, is common, and is experienced more frequently by individuals and communities with stigmatized and/or marginalized identities (3, 4). The impact of ostracism on mental health is well-established: those who have greater experiences of ostracism show greater levels of anxiety and depression, as well as lower self-reports of psychological well-being (57). Recent studies demonstrate the impact of ostracism—and related concepts such as social isolation and loneliness—on physical health both acutely [e.g., greater physiological responses such as increased blood pressure and inflammatory reactivity (8)], and chronically [e.g., development of chronic diseases such as coronary heart disease and stroke, as well as all-cause mortality (911)]. However, despite recent calls for greater attention to social modulators of pain (12, 13) and social indicators of pain disparities (1416), several important social factors—including ostracism—have been understudied in relation to pain.

Constructs related to ostracism—such as social isolation and loneliness—predict worse pain outcomes such as greater pain interference and disability among individuals with chronic pain (1722). Similar patterns may be expected for ostracism; however, ostracism critically differs from loneliness and social isolation in that it involves the directed action of others (i.e., being ignored or excluded by another person or group of people), and thus may be more systematically distributed to stigmatized groups or individuals than loneliness or isolation.

The few studies that have examined ostracism in the context of pain have used experimental paradigms and have yielded mixed results. Some research has found that experiencing ostracism facilitates pain as evidenced by increased or hypersensitivity to acute pain stimuli (23), while other studies found ostracism to have a numbing effect evidenced by decreased or hyposensitivity to pain (24, 25). Findings from Bernstein and Claypool (26) suggest that these mixed findings may be due to methodological differences in how feelings of ostracism were manipulated. In their study, Bernstein and Claypool used two different manipulations of ostracism that reflect either an acute, less severe level of ostracism (e.g., exclusion in an online ball-tossing game), or a chronic, more severe level of ostracism (e.g., results from a bogus personality test indicating a future of being alone). Less severe, acute levels of ostracism were associated with hypersensitivity while more severe levels of ostracism were associated with hyposensitivity to experimental pain stimuli. However, the previous studies investigating ostracism and physical pain have limited generalizability due to relying on experimentally manipulated measures of ostracism and simple or single measures of pain sensitivity (i.e., cold pain sensitivity).

The purpose of the current study was to examine the relationship between real-lived experiences of ostracism and laboratory tests of pain sensitivity. We hypothesized that greater lived experiences of ostracism would be associated with increased pain sensitivity across multiple pain measures. In recognition that experiences of ostracism are systematically different among racialized groups, we also hypothesized that lived ostracism would be experienced more among members of racialized groups. Finally, to probe ostracism as a potential social indicator of pain inequity, we explored the relationship between lived ostracism and laboratory pain within racialized groups.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Data source and extraction

Secondary data analysis was performed on data collected across six studies conducted between 2016 and 2020. Specifically, only measures of participant identity (demographics), lived ostracism, and laboratory pain were extracted. All studies were conducted using standardized training and study protocols, with the same equipment in the same laboratory—thus minimizing measurement variability and further enhancing power to detect small effects. Of the six studies, five relied on traditional forms of convenience sampling (four student samples recruited through student participant pools and one that recruited on campus and within the community using posted fliers) while one study recruited exclusively from the community (using fliers posted in local businesses and craigslist) and excluded individuals affiliated with the university. Four studies included experimental manipulations (random assignment to an experimental or control condition) and post-experimental pain assessment which could confound the impact of ostracism experiences on pain sensitivity, and thus for these studies, only participants in the neutral/non-experimental condition (k = 1) or only pre-manipulation baseline measures of pain (k = 3) are included in the present analysis. All studies were approved by the Texas A&M University Institutional Review Board.

2.2. Participants

A total of 511 adults were enrolled and completed studies that included the ostracism experiences scale and any laboratory pain measure. Six participants were excluded from analyses: three because they did not complete the primary ostracism measure, two because they requested their data not be retained, and one because a participant disclosed disqualifying information after completing the study (i.e., they provided inaccurate responses during eligibility screening), resulting in a final analysis sample of 505 participants. Eligibility criteria included being at least 18 years old, no recent or current chronic or acute pain, and no use of pain medications within the past 3 days. Participation in any of these studies was an exclusion criterion for the others, ensuring the independence of samples. Depending on the study, participants received either course credit or monetary compensation at a rate of $12–$20/h for their time. Sample demographics are reported in Table 1. A subset of the participants (i.e., 120 Latinx Americans) from the current analysis were included in a separate, previously reported analysis on multidimensional racialized discrimination experiences (27).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample descriptive characteristics by study.

2.3. Procedure

Prior to any study procedures, all participants were screened for study eligibility and completed a detailed in-person informed consent process. The specific laboratory pain measures included in analysis per study are reported in Table 2. As part of the standardized laboratory pain protocols implemented across studies, breaks were implemented between pain measures in order to avoid sensitization. All questionnaires were electronically administered using Qualtrics (Provo, UT).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Procedure order by study.

2.4. Materials

2.4.1. Ostracism experiences scale

The Ostracism Experiences Scale (OES) is an 8-item self-report survey that measures the frequency and accumulation of an individual's real-lived ostracism experiences (28, 29). Examples of items include: “In general, others keep me out-of-the-loop on information that is important to my close relationship” and “In general, others do not look at me when I’m in their presence”. Participants rated the frequency at which they have these experiences on a 6-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (Hardly Ever) to 7 (Almost Always). The items were summed to create a total score of real-lived ostracism experiences (α = 0.92, range across studies = 0.91–0.96), with higher scores indicating more ostracism.

2.4.2. Demographics

Age, sex, and racialized identity were assessed across studies. Participants were asked “What is your age” and provided their age in years using an open text box. Participants were prompted to provide their self-reported sex and racialized identity using the following items: “Sex: (select one) or Gender: (select one) [Male, Female, Other (please specify)]” and “Ethnicity: (select one) [African American/Black, Asian/Asian American, Hispanic/Latinx American, Native American/Alaskan Native, White/European American, Multiracial, Other (please specify)]”. We acknowledge that gender identity, and not sex, is the most appropriate measure to examine gender inequities in pain. Problematically, one study conflated sex with gender by asking for “gender” but only providing self-identification options for sex. All but this one study assessed sex and gender identity with separate questions. To maximize the sample size for inferential analyses, and because gender differences were not the primary aim of the present study, we use the imperfect measure of sex which allowed for self-identification but lacked explicit inclusion of intersex individuals. It is also important to note that the use of “other, please specify” is not inclusive and may have discouraged responses outside of the binary. Similarly, more recent studies include additional questions to more specifically and inclusively assess racialized, ethnic, and cultural identity. We use the initial question asking about racialized identity since it was included in all studies. However, the labels used are not inclusive of all identities and there is substantial heterogeneity in the lived and social experiences within racialized groups—particularly for multiracial identities.

2.5. Laboratory pain measures

Laboratory pain measures followed standardized protocols that we implement across studies, have previously reported (e.g., (27, 30, 31)), and summarize below.

2.5.1. Cold pain

2.5.1.1. Cold pain threshold

Cold pain threshold (CPTh) was operationalized either as the temperature at which a stimulus first became painful (CPTh°C) or the elapsed time at which a cold stimulus first became painful (CPThsec).

CPTh°C was assessed using a 30 mm × 30 mm ATS Thermal Stimulator (Pathway; Medoc, Ramat Yishai, Israel; k = 2) applied to the volar side of the right forearm. For each trial, the thermode gradually decreased in temperature, from a baseline of 32°C at a rate of 1°C/s until the participant pressed a button when the stimulus first became painful. If the stimulus reached the temperature limit (0°C), participants were asked to rate their current pain (0 (no pain) to 100 (the worst pain imaginable)), and if they indicated the presence of pain (>0), participant instructions were clarified before the next trial. In the analysis stage, such trials (where participants appeared to exceed CPTh) were marked as invalid and removed from calculation.

A total of 3 trials were conducted and CPTh°C was calculated as the average of the 3 trials.

CPThsec was obtained via submersion of the left hand up to the wrist in a 4°C circulating cold-water bath (k = 3; specifically a PD15R-30 Polyscience Circulating Bath (Niles, IL, U.S.A.) (k = 1) and a Thermo Firsher Scientific Circulating Bath (Newington, NH, U.S.A.) (k = 2). CPThsec was calculated as the number of seconds the participant kept their hand in the water until they first felt pain as indicated by saying the word “pain”.

2.5.1.2. Cold pain tolerance

Cold pain tolerance (CPTolsec) was assessed during the same trials as CPThsec, operationalized as the total number of seconds a participant kept their hand in the water, following instructions to keep their hand in the water until they could no longer tolerate the pain.

2.5.1.3. Cold pain intensity

Immediately after they removed their hand from the cold-water bath, participants were prompted to verbally indicate their current level of pain after removing their hand from the water (CPIntremoval; k = 3), as well as the maximum amount of pain they felt while their hand was still in the water (CPIntmax; k = 2) using the 0–100 scale described above.

2.5.2. Heat pain

2.5.2.1. Heat pain threshold

Heat pain threshold (HPTh°C) assessment followed the same procedure as CPTh°C (k = 2), except the temperature gradually increased from the 32°C baseline.

2.5.2.2. Heat pain tolerance

Heat pain tolerance (HPTol°C) (k = 2) was collected after the HPTh°C trials using the same equipment and procedure. The thermode gradually increased in temperature until the participant pressed a button when the pain from the heat became intolerable.

2.5.3. Pressure pain

Pressure pain threshold (PPThkPa) was collected (k = 2) using a single electronic algometer (Algometer Type II; SBMEDIC Electronics, Solna, Sweden). Pressure was applied to the trapezius muscle and increased steadily at a rate of 50 kPa/s until the participant verbally indicated when the pressure first produced a painful sensation by saying “pain”. PPThkPa was calculated by averaging the two closest values (kPa) out of all the administered trials. One study (k = 1) administered a maximum of 3 PPTh trials and 1 study continued assessment until the experimenters obtained 2 values within 50 kPa.

2.5.4. Temporal summation

Mechanical temporal summation (MTS) was assessed (k = 4) using weighted punctuate probes with a flat contact area of 0.2 mm diameter, delivering stimuli at a rate of 60 Hz (1 touch/second) to the middle phalange of the middle finger. MTS was calculated as the difference in verbal pain ratings on a 0–100 scale between the series of 10 stimuli and the single stimulus. A 128 mN probe was always used, though some (k = 3) studies also included tests for MTS using 256 and 512 mN probes. Larger, positive differences between the single and repeated stimulus ratings indicate greater summation of pain.

2.5.5. Conditioned pain modulation

Conditioned pain modulation (CPM) was assessed (k = 2) by combining the PPThkPa (test stimulus) and CPTolsec (conditioning stimulus) procedures. The test stimulus was applied after 20 s of the conditioning stimulus (or earlier if necessitated by participant hand-removal). The procedure ended (i.e., both stimuli were removed) when participants verbally indicated that the test stimulus first produced pain (PPThkPa). CPM was calculated as the difference between this assessment of PPThkPa and the assessment obtained earlier in the study (in the absence of a conditioning stimulus). Larger, positive values indicate a greater conditioned modulation of pain. One study included two trials for CPM. For this study, we calculated the CPM outcome variable for each trial separately and then averaged the two values to obtain a single CPM outcome variable.

2.5.6. After sensations

After sensations were assessed after CPIntmax (k = 2) using the same 0–100 scale. After sensations were assessed at least every 30 s until the participant rated their pain a zero. After sensations were calculated in 2 ways across each study: (1) the duration of after sensations (ASduration)—operationalized as the last time point (number of seconds) in which the participant rated pain above a zero, and (2) the intensity of after sensations (ASintensity)—operationalized as the participants’ pain rating 30 s after hand-removal (this time point was selected as the first common time point across studies).

2.6. Analyses

Given the exploratory nature of this study, we report all comparisons without alpha adjustment [e.g., (32)]. Before conducting inferential statistics, all variables were examined for normality and outliers were identified using graphical plots. Lifetime ostracism scores and laboratory pain measures were transformed—selecting the transformation that most effectively reduced the amount of skew—for inferential statistical analyses [e.g., (33)]. Before applying logarithmic transformations to variables containing negative or zero values, constants were added so that the minimum value was equal to 1 and all data points were included in statistical analyses. Bivariate correlations were conducted, and corresponding scatterplots inspected, to determine the relationship between ostracism experiences with laboratory pain.

Our approach to statistical considerations related to participant-level variables was informed by diversity science and anti-racism approaches—aiming to represent as opposed to control for diversity in lived experiences. Thus, we used moderation to examine potential within-group relationships in this study. Specifically, we first probed potential effects of racialized identity, sex, and age on the relationship between ostracism experiences and laboratory pain through moderation analyses and visual inspection of graphical plots. Moderation by study was also probed to detect potential design-related or history effects. Significant moderation was followed up with conditioning (i.e., the Johnson-Neyman technique) for continuous moderators and with within-group analyses for categorical moderators.

2.7. Missing data

All available data are included in analyses. In cases where there are fewer observations (sample size) for a given test, this was primarily due to the individual/primary study design (i.e., not all studies included all laboratory pain measures) as well as inclusion criteria for the present analysis (i.e., laboratory pain measures collected after an experimental manipulation were not extracted/included). Participants were also permitted to skip or stop any procedure at any time, though this was not common. We report the total sample used for each inferential analyses in the results section.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive statistics

Participants were on average 20.24 (SD = 4.43) years of age and primarily identified as female (65.1% identified as female, 34.5% as male, 0.2% did not identify as male or female but did not provide further self-identification, and 0.2% did not provide an answer). Studies aimed to recruit a diverse sample of racialized identities with 7.5% of participants identifying as Asian or Asian American, 11.3% as Black or African American, 32.3% as Latinx or Hispanic American, 4.6% as Multiracial, 42.6% as White, and <1% (n < 5) as each of the following: Native American/Alaska Native, Middle Eastern, Lebanese, and Afro-Dominican; 0.2% did not disclose their racialized or ethnic identification.

Descriptive statistics for the primary predictor and outcome variables are reported in Table 3. The majority of ostracism scores fell below the scale midpoint (midpoint = 28; M = 15.72, SD = 8.43). Although the full range of possible scores was observed in this sample, 14.7% of participants reported “Hardly Ever” experiencing ostracism as indicated by the lowest possible total score on the OES (minimum score = 8) while only 0.2% reported “Almost Always” experiencing ostracism (maximum score = 56).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Descriptive characteristics of lifetime ostracism and laboratory pain measures.

3.2. Bivariate correlations

Lifetime experiences of ostracism were not associated with age or sex and means did not differ across studies (p > 0.05). Greater lifetime ostracism experiences were significantly associated with lower CPTolsec [r(237) = −0.149, p = 0.022]. No other simple relationships were statistically significant (0.018 < r < 0.140, 0.106 < p < 0.812).

3.3. Moderation

3.3.1. Sex

Sex significantly moderated the relationship between lifetime ostracism experiences and CPThsec [ΔR2 = 0.024, F(1, 229) = 5.686, p = 0.018] and CPTolsec [ΔR2 = 0.020, F(1, 231) = 5.437, p = 0.021], such that the relationship was significant among male but not female participants. For male participants, greater lifetime ostracism experiences were associated with lower CPThsec [r(65) = −0.288, p = 0.020] and CPTolsec [r(66) = −0.332, p = 0.007; Figure 1].

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Sex significantly (p < 0.05) moderated the relationship between lifetime ostracism experiences and (A) cold pain threshold (CPThsec) and (B) cold pain tolerance (CPTolsec). The raw, untransformed values are presented in the figure.

3.3.2. Age

Age did not significantly moderate the relationship between lifetime ostracism experiences and laboratory pain (p > 0.10).

3.3.3. Study selection strategy

Probing for study effects revealed a consistent pattern such that when study effects were present, they were always in the direction of the community study revealing a different pattern than the studies that included convenience sampling. Therefore, we report (post-hoc) moderation by sampling strategy (convenience sample vs. community sample) rather than individual study (Figure 2). There was a significant moderation effect of sample population on the relationship between lifetime ostracism experiences and HPTol°C [ΔR2 = 0.050, F(1, 110) = 6.102, p = 0.015] and MTS at weight 256 mN [ΔR2 = 0.020, F(1, 195) = 4.055, p = 0.045]. Within group analyses show that lifetime ostracism experiences were associated with lower HPTol°C among the community sample [r(33) = −0.348, p = 0.047], but were not associated with HPTol°C among convenience samples [r(81) = 0.167, p = 0.136]. There were patterns of greater ostracism experiences to be associated with less MTS at weight 256 mN within the community sample [r(33) = −0.252, p = 0.158], but greater MTS within convenience samples [r(166) = 0.127, p = 0.102]; however these patterns were not statistically significant. While sample population was not a significant moderator for other laboratory pain measures, there were trends between lifetime ostracism experiences and CPM (p < 0.10). Greater lifetime ostracism experiences tended to be associated with less CPM within the community sample [r(33) = −0.312, p = 0.077], but not with CPM in convenience samples [r(81) = 0.089, p = 0.428]. Inspection of scatterplots suggests that across stimulus intensities, sample type was associated with opposite patterns of association between ostracism and MTS, such that the community sample showed patterns of slightly negative association between lifetime ostracism experiences and MTS, while the convenience sample showed slightly positive association.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Sample population significantly (p < 0.05) moderated the relationship between lifetime ostracism experiences and (D) heat pain tolerance (HPTol°C) and (B) mechanical temporal summation (MTS) using the 256 mN weighted probe. A similar, yet non-significant pattern was observed for the relationship between lifetime ostracism and (A & C) MTS using other weighted probes as well as (E) conditioned pain modulation (CPM). The raw, untransformed values are presented in the figure.

3.3.4. Racialized identity

Racialized identity significantly moderated the relationship between lifetime ostracism experiences and CPThsec [ΔR2 = 0.045, F(4, 222) = 2.65, p = 0.034], HPTol°C [ΔR2 = 0.068, F(3, 103) = 2.75, p = 0.047], and ASintensity [ΔR2 = 0.095, F(3, 74) = 2.76, p = 0.048] (Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Racialized identity significantly (p < 0.05) moderated the relationship between (A) cold pain threshold (CPThsec), (B) after sensations 30 s after cold stimulus cessation (ASintensity), and (C) heat pain tolerance (HPTol°C). The raw, untransformed values are presented in the figure.

There was no main effect of racialized group on lifetime ostracism experiences [F(6, 500) = 1.153, p = 0.330]. However, there was a pattern of more lifetime experiences of ostracism among participants in racialized groups (participants identifying as Asian or Asian American: M = 18.11, SD = 8.45; those identifying as groups that weren't listed: M = 17.25, SD = 6.08; Black or African American: M = 16.95, SD = 9.75; Multiracial: M = 16.39, SD = 8.84; Native American or Alaskan Native: M = 16.25. SD = 14.57; Hispanic or Latinx American M = 15.63, SD = 8.69; White or European American: M = 14.85, SD = 7.64). Among participants that identified as White, greater ostracism experiences were associated with lower CPThsec [r(137) = −0.198, p = 0.020] and CPTh°C [r(42) = 0.416, p = 0.006]. For Latinx participants, greater ostracism experiences were associated with lower CPIntmax [r(19) = −0.575, p = 0.010] and higher HPTol°C [r(35) = 0.366, p = 0.031]. There was also a trend for greater lifetime ostracism experiences to be associated with lower ASintensity [r(19) = −0.448, p = 0.055] within Latinx participants, but this trend did not reach statistical significance. Ostracism experiences were not associated with any laboratory pain measures for participants identifying as Asian or as Multiracial.

4. Discussion

In the current study, we demonstrate for the first time that greater lifetime experiences of ostracism are associated with laboratory pain, and specifically with lower CPTolsec. This is consistent with previous work that has demonstrated links between acute laboratory episodes of ostracism on cold pain (23). However, no other simple associations between lifetime ostracism and laboratory pain were observed. Indeed, while a strength of this study was the inclusion of dynamic measures, only the static measure of cold pain tolerance was significantly associated with lifetime ostracism experiences within whole group analyses. Although null findings for other measures may reflect several underling mechanisms (as discussed below), taken together with the extant literature, the current findings suggest it is possible that static measures of cold pain are the most sensitive to experiences of ostracism. Some studies have demonstrated a link between feelings of exclusion and cold sensations [e.g., experimental ostracism is associated with perceiving room temperatures as colder as well as with lower body temperatures (34, 35) drinking cold water in the laboratory was associated with lower levels of belongingness (36)]. Further work is needed to confirm whether static measures of cold pain are more sensitive to the variability of ostracism experiences.

Importantly, when sample diversity was considered as a relevant moderator, a more nuanced pattern of relationship between lifetime ostracism experiences and laboratory pain emerged. Specifically, planned analyses examining this relationship within racialized groups indicates that patterns between lifetime experiences of ostracism and acute pain sensitivity are obscured when racialized social oppression and privilege is not considered (i.e., when racialized identity is not considered in analyses). Notably, this is despite the lack of statistically significant differences in lifetime ostracism between racialized groups in this sample. Within participants that self-identified as White, greater lifetime ostracism experiences were associated with lower CPThsec and CPTh°C. Within Latinx participants, greater lifetime experiences of ostracism were associated with lower CPIntmax and higher HPTol°C. It is important to consider that there may be differential patterns in social indicators of pain among racialized groups—highlighting the importance of analyzing within groups to understand potential nuance in experiences of pain inequity. Furthermore, ostracism experiences of racialized groups are largely underrepresented within the literature. The current measures used to assess ostracism experiences may not fully capture ostracism experiences that are unique to racialized groups—who systematically experience greater ostracism across levels (e.g., interpersonally, structurally, culturally). In the current study, the differences in lifetime experiences of ostracism between racialized groups were not statistically significant. While the measure we used to assess lifetime experiences of ostracism has been validated within a racialized group (28, 29), it could also be missing important nuance in the ostracism experienced by different racialized groups or ostracism experiences occurring at different levels. More research is needed that examines the ostracism experiences of individuals within racialized groups, and how this may create or maintain pain inequities.

Further, our diversity approach revealed differential patterns of relationships between male and female participants. Lifetime experiences of ostracism were significantly associated with lower CPThsec and CPTolsec only within male participants. We did not hypothesize differential patterns between male and female participants. However, this could point to potential differences between males and females in other factors that may mitigate the effects of ostracism such as social support and support seeking after experiences of ostracism. Social support is known to buffer the effects of life stressors on an individual's well-being and health including pain outcomes (3740). Indeed, prior research suggests that receiving social support from a close other after being ostracized reduces negative emotions and brain activity associated with being excluded (41, 42). Although there is no evidence to suggest sex differences in responses to ostracism, there is evidence to support that men may be less likely to seek help or support from others when experiencing distress due to masculine gender roles of self-reliance and stoicism (4346). Therefore, it is possible that sex differences in social support and support seeking may be an important factor that alters the relationship between ostracism experiences and pain sensitivity. Future research may consider how different responses to ostracism or other social modulators may influence the relationship between lifetime ostracism experiences and pain. Additionally, it is likely that stigmatized gender identities are associated with greater ostracism experiences and should be followed up in future work.

Importantly, post-hoc analyses revealed an unexpected pattern within sampling subgroups (convenience vs. community sample). Greater lifetime ostracism experiences were associated with lower HPTol°C within the community sample but were not associated among the convenience sample. There were also trends for greater lifetime ostracism experiences to be associated with less MTS and CPM within the community sample compared to the convenience sample. A majority of the participants that completed the studies included in the current analysis were recruited on-campus using convenience sampling and were often students or other individuals affiliated with the university. Because of this, the studies that used convenience sampling consisted of primarily young, healthy, and college-educated adults who tend to report lower levels of ostracism. Our community sample, which excluded anyone affiliated with university, was relatively older and tended to report higher levels of lifetime ostracism experiences on average compared to the convenience sample. These trends may suggest a differential pattern among community samples who are likely to encounter more ostracism within their lifetime or perhaps more severe incidences of ostracism. This is important to consider because most of psychology research is conducted in undergraduate students using convenience sampling which limits its generalizability to other populations. Even within pain research, particularly research within non-clinical or chronic pain samples, a considerable amount is conducted with convenience samples with limited recruitment strategies and sampling within local communities. As a result, current models of social indicators of pain and pain inequity may be missing important nuance in the experiences of individuals that are often inadvertently excluded from participating in research.

There are several limitations that should be considered when interpreting the current findings. First, we report all comparisons within this exploratory study which increases the possibility for Type 1 error. Future research is needed to confirm and clarify the relationships between lifetime ostracism experiences and cold pain sensitivity. We also report non-significant results to spur future research and to support meta-analyses. However, these should be interpreted with caution. Second, due of differences in the studies sampled as well as the inclusion criteria for the present analysis, there were discrepancies in sample sizes across inferential analyses. This may impact the current findings, given that inferential analyses for certain laboratory pain measures had smaller sample sizes and less power to detect relationships. Third, most participants scored below the midpoint of the lifetime ostracism measure which could limit our ability to make inferences on those that experience moderate to high levels of ostracism. Previous research has shown that chronic pain is prevalent within groups that experience high levels of exclusion and marginalization such as homeless populations (4749). However, further research is needed to explore the relationship between ostracism and pain sensitivity within individuals that have experienced moderate to high levels of ostracism. Fourth, while this study is novel in that it includes a measure of accumulated lifetime ostracism experiences, this measure is unable to assess the severity of ostracism experiences. Prior research suggests that the severity of ostracism experiences may show different patterns on acute pain sensitivity, in that severe experiences of ostracism may be associated with hyposensitivity to pain stimuli (26). This measure also does not assess ostracism experiences that are related to an individual's identity which is likely an important aspect of ostracism experienced by individuals with stigmatized or minoritized identities. In one study, Goodwin and colleagues (50) show that individuals that experienced ostracism related to their racialized identity had slower recovery of basic needs than individuals where ostracism was not related to racialized identity. Greater experiences of racialized exclusion or rejections have also shown to be associated with greater post-traumatic stress and depression symptoms (51). However, more research is needed to assess the severity of ostracism experiences, as well as the attributions of ostracism to examine how these variables may influence the pain experiences of minoritized communities. Fifth, while the experience of acute laboratory pain is clinically relevant (52), it is important for future studies to examine the relationship between real-lived experiences of ostracism among individuals with chronic pain. Though loneliness and social isolation are known to enhance pain in this population, it is also expected that people with chronic pain experience more overt experiences of ostracism due to the frequent stigmatization of people with chronic pain conditions—especially for pain conditions with no biological or specified cause (5356). Future work is needed to evaluate the independent effects of ostracism in the context of chronic and persistent clinical pain. Lastly, it is important to note that there were many null findings in the current study. It is possible that some of these findings represent small effects, true null findings, or that there are differential patterns related to the diversity of lived experiences across groups. Our moderation results indicate that differential patterns among groups are at least partially present.

Traditional approaches to understanding pain disparities typically control for demographic factors and identities that confound pain experiences due to social indicators. This approach tells us little about the reasons as to why and how pain inequities exist. By controlling for diverse identities, this approach discards the meaningful variance in experiences of social inequity (the root cause of pain disparities) among racialized and other marginalized identities that influence pain physiology. In the current study, we provide a new potential approach that retains and explores the diversity of our sample to understand one potential mechanism—lifetime ostracism experiences—by which social inequity may contribute to pain inequities within marginalized groups. Instead of using diverse identities as control variables, we conducted within-group analyses to examine whether there were different patterns between lifetime ostracism experiences and acute sensitivity to laboratory pain across age and within sex and racialized groups. Future work is needed to confirm and extend the current findings by including and representing non-convenience populations—particularly groups that experience greater levels of ostracism or marginalization to assess patterns on lived pain experiences. As noted above, future research in this area may specifically seek to (1) confirm the specificity of the relationship between ostracism and cold-pain and further probe mechanistic explanations for this relationship, (2) examine specific racialized ostracism experiences and associations with pain using focused within-group approaches, (3) examine the ostracism-pain relationship among stigmatized gender groups, and (4) expand the development and validation of social indicators of pain using non-convenience samples. Additional research is also needed to assess other dimensions of social injustice across levels (e.g., interpersonal, structural, cultural) and how they create and maintain pain inequities (15).

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Texas A&M University IRB. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

VM and AC-S conceived the idea and provided resources. KW, BB, and NN collected data. KW curated data, developed the analysis plan and conducted all data analyses, and wrote the complete original draft as part of her master's thesis. VM provided supervision during each stage of the process. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

Research funding was provided by the American Pain Society’s Future Leaders in Pain Research Award to VM, as well as the Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences at Texas A&M University. Additional research support was provided by the National Science Foundation under NSF Cooperative Agreement (No. HRD-1723255). NN was supported by NSF Graduate Research Fellowship (DGE: 1746932) and the Doctoral Diversity Excellence Fellowship from Texas A&M University during her work on this project.

Acknowledgments

We thank Matthew Vess and Theresa Morris for helpful feedback on this manuscript, Katherine Jaffe for assistance with citation management, Humza Ahmed for technical support, and the following research assistants for their valuable contributions to building community partnerships, participant recruitment, and data collection: Mollie Adams, Marguerite Anderson, Magdalena Banda, Lane Bannwart, Montae Bermudez, Kyisha Butcher, Jane EM Carter, Leslie Chaffin, Matthew Cline, Madison Cortez, Alida Demeter, Paul Forks, Xochitl Fuentes, Meredith Gary, Nevita George, Meron Haley, Melinda Hale, Jordan Hayes, Travis Hyatt, Catherine Kuykendall, Hanggi Lee, Hannah Hutchins Lewer, Jamie Loasby, Megan Marsico, Mitra Nair, Avery Nennmann, Sarah Peoples, James Connor Price, Nathan Ramsey, Ria Rao, Darby Salge, Cassidy Seale, Rahul Sirigiri, Evelyn Stewart-Johnson, Jennifer Van Horn, Sanya Varma, and Fallon Wenck.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Williams DR, Lawrence JA, Davis BA. Racism and health: evidence and needed research. Annu Rev Public Health. (2019) 40(1):105–25. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-040218-043750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Krieger N. Measures of racism, sexism, heterosexism, and gender binarism for health equity research: from structural injustice to embodied harm-an ecosocial analysis. Annu Rev Public Health. (2020) 41:37–62. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-040119-094017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Kurzban R, Leary MR. Evolutionary origins of stigmatization: the functions of social exclusion. Psychol Bull. (2001) 127(2):187–208. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Wesselmann ED, DeSouza ER, AuBuchon S, Bebel C, Parris L. Investigating microaggressions against transgender individuals as a form of social exclusion. Psychol Sex Orientat Gend Divers. (2021):1–12. doi: 10.1037/sgd0000513

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Bowes L, Joinson C, Wolke D, Lewis G. Peer victimisation during adolescence and its impact on depression in early adulthood: prospective cohort study in the United Kingdom. Br Med J. (2015) 350. doi: 10.1136/bmj.h2469

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Stapinski LA, Bowes L, Wolke D, Pearson RM, Mahedy L, Button KS, et al. Peer victimization during adolescence and risk for anxiety disorders in adulthood: a prospective cohort study. Depress Anxiety. (2014) 31(7):574–82. doi: 10.1002/da.22270

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Zadro L, Williams KD, Richardson R. How low can you go? Ostracism by a computer is sufficient to lower self-reported levels of belonging, control, self-esteem, and meaningful existence. J Exp Soc Psychol. (2004) 40(4):560–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2003.11.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Brown EG, Gallagher S, Creaven AM. Loneliness and acute stress reactivity: a systematic review of psychophysiological studies. Psychophysiology. (2018) 55(5):e13031. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Baker M, Harris T, Stephenson D. Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2015) 10(2):227–37. doi: 10.1177/1745691614568352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Leigh-Hunt N, Bagguley D, Bash K, Turner V, Turnbull S, Valtorta N, et al. An overview of systematic reviews on the public health consequences of social isolation and loneliness. Public Health. (2017) 152:157–71. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2017.07.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Valtorta NK, Kanaan M, Gilbody S, Ronzi S, Hanratty B. Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for coronary heart disease and stroke: systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal observational studies. Heart. (2016) 102(13):1009–16. doi: 10.1136/heartjnl-2015-308790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Craig KD, MacKenzie NE. What is pain: are cognitive and social features core components? Paediatr Neonatal Pain. (2021) 3(3):106–18. doi: 10.1002/pne2.12046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Mogil JS. Social modulation of and by pain in humans and rodents. Pain. (2015) 156:56, S35–S41. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Letzen JE, Mathur VA, Janevic MR, Burton MD, Hood AM, Morais CA, et al. Confronting racism in all forms of pain research: reframing study designs. J Pain. (2022) 23(6):893–912. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2022.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Mathur VA, Trost Z, Ezenwa MO, Sturgeon JA, Hood AM. Mechanisms of injustice: what we (don’t) know about racialized disparities in pain. Pain. (2022) 163(6):999–1005. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Booker SQ, Bartley EJ, Powell-Roach K, Palit S, Morais C, Thompson OJ, et al. The imperative for racial equality in pain science: a way forward. J Pain. (2021) 22(12):1578–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2021.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Canaipa R, Castro-Caldas A, Moreira JM, Pimentel-Santos F, Branco JC, Treister R. Impaired pain modulation in fibromyalgia patients in response to social distress manipulation. Clin J Pain. (2017) 33(7):611–9. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0000000000000447

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Jaremka LM, Fagundes CP, Glaser R, Bennett JM, Malarkey WB, Kiecolt-Glaser JK. Loneliness predicts pain, depression, and fatigue: understanding the role of immune dysregulation. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2013) 38(8):1310–7. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.11.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Karayannis NV, Baumann I, Sturgeon JA, Melloh M, Mackey SC. The impact of social isolation on pain interference: a longitudinal study. Ann Behav Med. (2019) 53(1):65–74. doi: 10.1093/abm/kay017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Petitte T, Mallow JA, Barnes ER, Petrone A, Barr T, Theeke LA. A systematic review of loneliness and common chronic physical conditions in adults. Open Psychol J. (2015) 8(Suppl 2):113–32. doi: 10.2174/1874350101508010113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Tse M, Leung R, Ho S. Pain and psychological well-being of older persons living in nursing homes: an exploratory study in planning patient-centred intervention. J Adv Nurs. (2012) 68(2):312–21. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2011.05738.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Wolf LD, Davis MC. Loneliness, daily pain, and perceptions of interpersonal events in adults with fibromyalgia. Health Psychol. (2015) 33(9):929–37. doi: 10.1037/hea0000059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Pieritz K, Schäfer SJ, Strahler J, Rief W, Euteneuer F. Chronic stress moderates the impact of social exclusion on pain tolerance: an experimental investigation. J Pain Res. (2017) 10:1155–62. doi: 10.2147/JPR.S129872

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Antico L, Guyon A, Mohamed ZK, Corradi-Dell’Acqua C. Beyond unpleasantness. Social exclusion affects the experience of pain, but not of equally-unpleasant disgust. Cognition. (2018) 181:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. DeWall CN, Baumeister RF. Alone but feeling no pain: effects of social exclusion on physical pain tolerance and pain threshold, affective forecasting, and interpersonal empathy. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2006) 91(1):1–15. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Bernstein MJ, Claypool HM. Social exclusion and pain sensitivity: why exclusion sometimes hurts and sometimes numbs. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. (2012) 38(2):185–96. doi: 10.1177/0146167211422449

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Walsh KT, Boring BL, Nanavaty N, Guzman H, Mathur VA. Sociocultural context and pre-clinical pain facilitation: multiple dimensions of racialized discrimination experienced by latinx Americans are associated with enhanced temporal summation of pain. J Pain. (2022) 23(11):1885–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2022.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Gilman R, Carter-Sowell A, DeWall CN, Adams RE, Carboni I. Validation of the ostracism experience scale for adolescents. Psychol Assess. (2013) 25(2):319–30. doi: 10.1037/a0030913

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Carter-Sowell AR. Salting a wound, building a callous, or throwing in the towel? The measurement and effects of chronic ostracism experiences [Doctoral dissertation]. West Lafayette, Indiana: Purdue University (2010).

30. Mathur VA, Kiley KB, Haywood Jr C, Bediako SM, Lanzkron S, Carroll CP, et al. Multiple levels of suffering: discrimination in health-care settings is associated with enhanced laboratory pain sensitivity in sickle cell disease. Clin J Pain. (2016) 32(12):1076–85. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0000000000000361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Mathur VA, Kiley KB, Carroll CP, Edwards RR, Lanzkron S, Haythornthwaite JA, et al. Disease-related, nondisease-related, and situational catastrophizing in sickle cell disease and its relationship with pain. J Pain. (2016) 17(11):1227–36. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2016.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Sauder DC, DeMars CE. An updated recommendation for multiple comparisons. Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci. (2019) 2(1):26–44. doi: 10.1177/2515245918808784

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Pek J, Wong O, Wong ACM. How to address non-normality: a taxonomy of approaches, reviewed, and illustrated. Front Psychol. (2018) 9:2104. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Ijzerman H, Gallucci M, Pouw WTJL, Weiβgerber SC, Van Doesum NJ, Williams KD. Cold-blooded loneliness: social exclusion leads to lower skin temperatures. Acta Psychol. (2012) 140(3):283–8. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.05.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Zhong C-B, Leonardelli GJ. Cold and lonely: does social exclusion literally feel cold? Psychol Sci. (2008) 19(9):838–42. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02165.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Chen Z, Poon K-T, DeWall CN. Cold thermal temperature threatens belonging: the moderating role of perceived social support. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. (2015) 6(4):439–46. doi: 10.1177/1948550614562843

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Chen Y, Feeley TH. Social support, social strain, loneliness, and well-being among older adults: an analysis of the health and retirement study. J Soc Pers Relat. (2014) 31(2):141–61. doi: 10.1177/0265407513488728

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Hughes S, Jaremka LM, Alfano CM, Glaser R, Povoski SP, Lipari AM, et al. Social support predicts inflammation, pain, and depressive symptoms: longitudinal relationships among breast cancer survivors. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2014) 42:38–44. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Richmond NL, Meyer ML, Hollowell AG, Isenberg EE, Domeier RM, Swor RA, et al. Social support and pain outcomes after trauma exposure among older adults. Clin J Pain. (2018) 34(4):366–74. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0000000000000545

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Uchino BN. Social support and health: a review of physiological processes potentially underlying links to disease outcomes. J Behav Med. (2006) 29(4):377–87. doi: 10.1007/s10865-006-9056-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Morese R, Lamm C, Bosco FM, Valentini MC, Silani G. Social support modulates the neural correlates underlying social exclusion. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2019) 14(6):633–43. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Teng F, Chen Z. Does social support reduce distress caused by ostracism? It depends on the level of one’s self-esteem. J Exp Soc Psychol. (2012) 48(5):1192–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.03.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Cleary A. Death rather than disclosure: struggling to be a real man. Irish J Sociol. (2005) 14(2):155–76. doi: 10.1177/079160350501400209

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. McKenzie SK, Collings S, Jenkin G, River J. Masculinity, social connectedness, and mental health: men’s diverse patterns of practice. Am J Mens Health. (2018) 12(5):1247–61. doi: 10.1177/1557988318772732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Oliffe JL, Robertson S, Kelly MT, Roy P, Ogrodniczuk JS. Connecting masculinity and depression among international Male university students. Qual Health Res. (2010) 20(7):987–98. doi: 10.1177/1049732310365700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Seidler ZE, Dawes AJ, Rice SM, Oliffe JL, Dhillon HM. The role of masculinity in men’s help-seeking for depression: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev. (2016) 49:106–18. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Hwang SW, Wilkins E, Chambers C, Estrabillo E, Berends J, MacDonald A. Chronic pain among homeless persons: characteristics, treatment, and barriers to management. BMC Fam Pract. (2011) 12(1):1–9. doi: 10.1186/1471-2296-12-73

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Landefeld JC, Miaskowski C, Tieu L, Ponath C, Lee CT, Guzman D, et al. Characteristics and factors associated with pain in older homeless individuals: results from the HOPE HOME study. J Pain. (2017) 18(9):1036–45. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2017.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Vogel M, Choi F, Westenberg JN, Cabanis M, Nikoo N, Nikoo M, et al. Chronic pain among individuals experiencing homelessness and its interdependence with opioid and other substance use and mental illness. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2022) 19(5). doi: 10.3390/ijerph19052685

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Goodwin SA, Williams KD, Carter-Sowell AR. The psychological sting of stigma: the costs of attributing ostracism to racism. J Exp Soc Psychol. (2010) 46(4):612–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.02.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Chin D, Loeb TB, Zhang M, Liu H, Cooley-Strickland M, Wyatt GE. Racial/ethnic discrimination: dimensions and relation to mental health symptoms in a marginalized urban American population. Am J Orthopsychiatry. (2020) 350:h2469. doi: 10.1037/ort0000481

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Edwards RR, Sarlani E, Wesselmann U, Fillingim RB. Quantitative assessment of experimental pain perception: multiple domains of clinical relevance. Pain. (2005) 114(3):315–9. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2005.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Åsbring P, Närvänen A-L. Women’s experiences of stigma in relation to chronic fatigue syndrome and fibromyalgia. Qual Health Res. (2002) 12(2):148–60. doi: 10.1177/104973230201200202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Gardner JR, Sandhu G. The stigma and enigma of chronic non-malignant back pain (CNMBP) treated with long term opioids (LTO). Contemp Nurse. (1997) 6(2):61–6. doi: 10.5172/conu.1997.6.2.61

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Looper KJ, Kirmayer LJ. Perceived stigma in functional somatic syndromes and comparable medical conditions. J Psychosom Res. (2004) 57(4):373–8. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(04)00447-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Slade SC, Molloy E, Keating JL. Stigma experienced by people with nonspecific chronic low back pain: a qualitative study. Pain Med. (2009) 10(1):143–54. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4637.2008.00540.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: inequity, disparity, quantitative sensory testing, social exclusion, social indicators

Citation: Walsh KT, Boring BL, Nanavaty N, Carter-Sowell AR and Mathur VA (2022) Lifetime ostracism experiences and mechanisms of pain. Front. Pain Res. 3:1037472. doi: 10.3389/fpain.2022.1037472

Received: 5 September 2022; Accepted: 8 November 2022;
Published: 16 December 2022.

Edited by:

Carolina Roza, University of Alcalá, Spain

Reviewed by:

Ming Zhang, Chinese Academy of Sciences (CAS), China
Margaret Dunham, Edinburgh Napier University, United Kingdom

© 2022 Walsh, Boring, Nanavaty, Carter-Sowell and Mathur. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Vani A. Mathur vmathur@tamu.edu

Specialty Section: This article was submitted to Pain Research Methods, a section of the journal Frontiers in Pain Research

Download