Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 05 February 2021
Sec. Perception Science

Activation of Prefrontal Cortex in Process of Oral and Finger Shape Discrimination: fNIRS Study

\r\nNoriyuki Narita*Noriyuki Narita1*Kazunobu KamiyaKazunobu Kamiya2Sunao IwakiSunao Iwaki3Tomohiro IshiiTomohiro Ishii2Hiroshi EndoHiroshi Endo4Michiharu ShimosakaMichiharu Shimosaka5Takeshi UchidaTakeshi Uchida6Ikuo KantakeIkuo Kantake6Koh ShibutaniKoh Shibutani5
  • 1Research Institute of Oral Science, Nihon University School of Dentistry at Matsudo, Matsudo, Japan
  • 2Department of Removable Prosthodontics, Nihon University School of Dentistry at Matsudo, Matsudo, Japan
  • 3Mental and Physical Functions Modeling Group, Human Informatics and Interaction Research Institute, National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology, Tsukuba, Japan
  • 4Physical Fitness Technology Group, Human Informatics and Interaction Research Institute, National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology, Tsukuba, Japan
  • 5Department of Anesthesiology, Nihon University School of Dentistry at Matsudo, Matsudo, Japan
  • 6Dental Support Co., Ltd., Chiba, Japan

Background: The differences in the brain activities of the insular and the visual association cortices have been reported between oral and manual stereognosis. However, these results were not conclusive because of the inherent differences in the task performance-related motor sequence conditions. We hypothesized that the involvement of the prefrontal cortex may be different between finger and oral shape discrimination. This study was conducted to clarify temporal changes in prefrontal activities occurring in the processes of oral and finger tactual shape discrimination using prefrontal functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS).

Methods: Six healthy right-handed males [aged 30.8 ± 8.2 years (mean ± SD)] were enrolled. Measurements of prefrontal activities were performed using a 22-channel fNIRS device (ETG-100, Hitachi Medical Co., Chiba, Japan) during experimental blocks that included resting state (REST), nonsense shape discrimination (SHAM), and shape discrimination (SHAPE).

Results: No significant difference was presented with regard to the number of correct answers during trials between oral and finger SHAPE discrimination. Additionally, a statistical difference for the prefrontal fNIRS activity between oral and finger shape discrimination was noted in CH 1. Finger SHAPE, as compared with SHAM, presented a temporally shifting onset and burst in the prefrontal activities from the frontopolar area (FPA) to the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). In contrast, oral SHAPE as compared with SHAM was shown to be temporally overlapped in the onset and burst of the prefrontal activities in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC)/FPA/OFC.

Conclusion: The prefrontal activities temporally shifting from the FPA to the OFC during SHAPE as compared with SHAM may suggest the segregated serial prefrontal processing from the manipulation of a target image to the decision making during the process of finger shape discrimination. In contrast, the temporally overlapped prefrontal activities of the DLPFC/FPA/OFC in the oral SHAPE block may suggest the parallel procession of the repetitive involvement of generation, manipulation, and decision making in order to form a reliable representation of target objects.

Introduction

Irving (1968) noted that stereognosis can be defined as the ability to recognize objects using only tactile (somatic) sensation and that it particularly has been applied to the finger and mouth.

The neurophysiological mechanisms for finger stereognosis have been investigated intensively, in which the participation of fronto-parietal networks was observed in the process of haptic shape recognition (Shomstein, 2012; Katsuki and Constantinidis, 2014; Sathian, 2016; Xu et al., 2020). These results suggest that a top-down bias attention may be advantageous for familiar percepts in haptic shape recognition, whereas a bottom-up bias attention may be more reliable under circumstances with unfamiliar percepts (Sathian, 2016; Xu et al., 2020) or during unskilled task performance (Lee et al., 2007; Jackson and Raymond, 2008; Seidler et al., 2012). In contrast, the neural mechanisms for oral stereognosis have not been thoroughly studied despite its importance in cognitive aging confirmed in many clinical and behavioral studies, where denture wearing in the elderly population has been shown to be effective in improving oral stereognosis (Müller et al., 1995; Jacobs et al., 1998; Ikebe et al., 2007; Meenakshi et al., 2014; Singh and Mattoo, 2014; Fukutake et al., 2019).

One of the few studies on brain activities during oral stereognosis employed functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to report that activities in the prefrontal cortex decreases with age (Kawagishi et al., 2014); however, it is still not clear whether the prefrontal activities during oral stereognosis differ from those during finger stereognosis. Another neuroimaging study by Fujii et al. (2011) compared differences in the brain activities between finger and oral stereognosis directly by using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), which presented the predominant activities in the insular and the visual association cortices during oral stereognosis; however, the results are not conclusive probably because the effects from the differences in the motor task sequences between the oral and finger shape discrimination tasks were not controlled adequately.

We previously clarified the cross-modal associations between oral somatosensory and visual cortex activities during oral shape discrimination where the effects of motor sequence during the task performance were compensated by the SHAM condition, under which participants mimic only the mouth movement without a target object (Kagawa et al., 2014). Taking into account the effects of motor sequences specific to each target task condition by using the SHAM condition for each task performance, detailed differences in the brain activities between finger and oral shape discrimination tasks should be revealed more precisely.

In the present study, we compare the brain responses to oral and finger shape discrimination conditions as characterized by the differences in the fNIRS signals during the target and SHAM task performances for each discrimination condition (Reed et al., 2004; Kagawa et al., 2014). Specifically, we focus on the fNIRS signals measured in the prefrontal cortices, which represent the manipulation of mental representation of the target (target image) and reference (reference image) objects (Cohen-Kdoshay and Meiran, 2009; Hering et al., 2018; Emch et al., 2019).

Materials and Methods

Participants

Six healthy right-handed males [aged 30.8 ± 8.2 years (mean ± SD)] were enrolled in this study. All subjects were mentally healthy, as indicated by a score lower than 7 on the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (Zigmond and Snaith, 1983). The sample size for the two-way repeated measures ANOVA in this study was determined using the GPower 3 software package (noncommercial program downloaded from the University of Dusseldorf, Germany) (Faul et al., 2007), which established parameters with a significance level of 0.05, statistical power of 0.80, and effect size of 0.25 (medium effect). Each provided written informed consent for participation in this study, which was approved by the Ethics Committee of Nihon University School of Dentistry at Matsudo (EC 19-14-015-1).

Experimental Procedures

Experiments were performed under the same conditions used in our previous study (Kagawa et al., 2014). Briefly, each of the six subjects was comfortably seated in a quiet room and a screen was positioned around them so as to block extra visual information from entering their field of view. The experimental blocks required six test pieces for discrimination, which were made of autopolymer resin (UNIFAST3, GC, Japan). Six test pieces were differently shaped, including a circle, ellipse, square, rectangle, triangle, and a semicircle, and each shape was tested once for each test type (oral and finger), in a random order (Kaiba et al., 2006) (Figure 1A).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental aspects of finger and oral shape discrimination using prefrontal functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). (A) Schematic diagrams and photographs of six types of test pieces: circle, ellipse, square, rectangle, triangle, and semicircle. (B) Positioning of the prefrontal fNIRS probes and corresponding channel numbers. (C) Timeline of a single session composed of six trials of shape discrimination in 8 min. (D) Location of the prefrontal fNIRS channels. Orange and red indicate the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC); yellow, the frontopolar area (FPA); green, triangular part of the inferior frontal gyrus (TP); blue, the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC); and purple, the inferior prefrontal gyrus (IFG). Each circle corresponds to a channel number. Pie chart shows percentage in cortical areas.

Prior to the experimental blocks, the subjects received explanations regarding the procedures, including the six differently shaped test pieces and a schematic diagram, and rehearsed one block without any test pieces used (Kaiba et al., 2006).

During the pre-trial period, the investigator approached the subject from the rear so as not to enter their field of view and then instructed them to open their mouth in order to place a test piece with tweezers on the tongue or open their hands upward to place the piece on the anterior belly of the index finger. One of six test pieces was placed in the mouth preceding oral shape discrimination (O-SHAPE) or on the tip of the index finger of the right hand preceding finger shape discrimination (F-SHAPE). The subject was asked to quietly wait for verbal cues during the pre-trial and pre-task periods, after which they began to manipulate the test piece in their mouth without biting or in their hands using the index, middle, and ring fingers during the 10-s task period. After 10 s, they were given a verbal cue to stop the shape exploration and return to a quiet condition for the post-task period. The test piece was removed from the mouth or fingers during the inter-trial period of 20 s by means of tweezers. Following that, the subject was asked to point to one of the images on the schematic diagram that they thought had been explored during the task trial.

Oral shape discrimination with no test pieces (O-SHAM), or finger shape discrimination with no test pieces (F-SHAM), and Rest (REST) blocks were also conducted in this study. During the SHAM blocks, the subject was instructed to mimic their movements, with no test piece placed in the mouth or on the fingers, and then point toward the schematic diagram after the experimenter had imitated removing the test piece. During the REST block, there was no task performance and the subject was instructed to remain quiet. The O-SHAPE, F-SHAPE, O-SHAM, F-SHAM, and REST blocks were performed in random order in every subject. REST blocks were used to take account of brain activity fluctuations, with the findings obtained during that time related to brain responses specific to the SHAM and SHAPE blocks evaluated by comparing the fNIRS signals measured during each of those blocks with the signals recorded during the REST. Each block lasted for 8 min and the intervals between blocks were set at 5 min; thus, the total duration for five experimental blocks in one session was 60 min for each subject.

Measurements of Prefrontal fNIRS Activities

Prefrontal fNIRS activities were measured according to the methods used in our previous study (Kamiya et al., 2016). Briefly, prefrontal activity was assessed during the pre-task, task, and post-task periods using a 22-channel fNIRS device (ETG-100, Hitachi Medical Co., Chiba, Japan) that utilizes two wavelengths of near-infrared light (780 and 830 nm) (Narita et al., 2009). Probes were fitted with 3 × 5 thermoplastic shells and placed on the prefrontal region, with the bottom lines of the fNIRS probes set according to FP1 and FP2, with referral to the international 10-20 system (Klem et al., 1999) (Figure 1B). fNIRS was used to determine relative changes in the concentrations of oxygenated hemoglobin ([oxy-Hb]), deoxygenated-hemoglobin ([deoxy-Hb]), and total hemoglobin. A change in [oxy-Hb] was used as an indicator of change in regional cerebral blood volume because it is more sensitive than [deoxy-Hb] as a parameter for measuring blood flow changes associated with brain activation (Hoshi et al., 2001) and has a stronger correlation with blood oxygenation level-dependent signals measured by fMRI (Strangman et al., 2002). The sampling interval was 0.1 s. During the measurements, the subjects were instructed to open their eyes and gaze at a point forward. Each trial was repeated six times, and the data obtained were averaged using the “integral mode” of the ETG-100 software package for the F-SHAPE, O-SHAPE, F-SHAM, O-SHAM, and REST blocks (Figure 1C). Also, the linear fitting algorithm (Naseer and Hong, 2015) was used for baseline correction (Ehlis et al., 2007). A moving average with a window width of 5 s was used to remove physiological noise such as cardiac artifacts (Yang et al., 2020) and the short-term motion artifacts (Suto et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2013) in the fNIRS signals.

Location of the Prefrontal fNIRS Channel

The three-dimensional locations of each probe and the landmark position (NZ, Iz, AI, A2, Cz) on the scalp of each participant were recorded by utilizing a 3D magnetic space digitizer (3SPACE ISOTRACK2, Polhemus, United States). Furthermore, estimation of the corresponding location of each channel in the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space (Collins et al., 1994; Brett et al., 2002) was obtained using a probabilistic registration method (Okamoto and Dan, 2005; Singh et al., 2005), with anatomical localization corresponding to the probe position identified using Platform for Optical Topography Analysis Tools (POTATo, Hitachi, Japan) with reference to Automated Anatomical Labeling (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002; Tsuzuki and Dan, 2014) (Figure 1D).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Grand average waveforms and topographical maps of the prefrontal functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) activities. Shown on the left are the grand averages in the six subjects’ waveforms of oxygenated hemoglobin concentration ([oxy-Hb], red line) and deoxygenated hemoglobin concentration ([deoxy-Hb], blue line) for each of the 22 measurement channels during the REST, SHAM, and SHAPE blocks of finger and oral shape discriminations. The x-axis indicates time (in seconds) and the y-axis indicates hemodynamic changes in [oxy-Hb]. Vertical lines at 20 and 30 s indicate the onset and offset of the 10-s task period. Shown on the right are the topographical maps of the changes in [oxy-Hb] during the 10-s period preceding the task, the 10-s task period, and the 10-s period following the task period during the REST, SHAM, and SHAPE blocks of finger and oral manipulations. Changes in [oxy-Hb] during the task periods were not clearly demonstrated during the REST and SHAM blocks (A,B,D), whereas it was clearly increased during the F-SHAPE and O-SHAPE (C,E).

Data and Statistical Analyses

A Wilcoxon signed-rank test was used to compare the averaged number of correct answers of the six shaped test pieces between the F-SHAPE and O-SHAPE blocks (Jacobs et al., 1997; Mantecchini et al., 1998). Two-way repeated measures ANOVA and multiple comparisons using a Bonferroni t-test were also applied for the time course of the averaged data for [oxy-Hb] for every 1 s during the pre-task, task, and post-task periods to compare between the SHAPE and REST, SHAM and REST, and SHAPE and SHAM blocks. The statistical software package SigmaPlot 12.5 (Systat Software Inc., AC, United States) was used for all analyses, and p-values less than 0.05 were considered to indicate a significant difference.

Results

Behavioral Performance

Number of Correct Answers of F-SHAPE and O-SHAPE

The number of correct answers averaged over six subjects (mean ± SD) during the F-SHAPE blocks were 4.5 ± 1.0, while those during the O-SHAPE were 4.7 ± 1.0. There was no significant difference (Wilcoxon signed-rank test: Z = 0.408, p = 0.818) shown between the numbers of correct answers by the six subjects.

fNIRS Activities

Grand Averaged Waveforms and Topographical Maps of Prefrontal fNIRS Activities

The grand averaged waveforms in the six subjects for changes in [oxy-Hb] and [deoxy-Hb] during the REST, F-SHAM, F-SHAPE, O-SHAM, and O-SHAPE blocks are shown on the left side of Figure 2, while the [oxy-Hb] maps for changes in [oxy-Hb] in the pre-task, task, and post-task periods during the REST, F-SHAM, F-SHAPE, O-SHAM, and O-SHAPE blocks are presented on the right side of Figure 2. During REST, there were no apparent changes in [oxy-Hb] with regard to prefrontal fNIRS activities (Figure 2A). O-SHAM and F-SHAM showed slight increases in [oxy-Hb] during the task periods (Figures 2B,D), while there were clear increases in [oxy-Hb] during both the task and post-task periods during the O-SHAPE and F-SHAPE blocks (Figures 2C,E).

Comparison Between F-SHAPE and O-SHAPE

In the comparison between F-SHAPE and O-SHAPE, the values for [oxy-Hb] for F-SHAPE significantly (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05, F = 1.755, p = 0.003, power of performed test = 0.852) increased as compared to O-SHAPE at fNIRS channel (CH) 1 which corresponds to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC).

Comparison Between F-SHAPE and REST

Temporal Differences in Prefrontal fNIRS Activities

Statistically significant (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) increases in prefrontal fNIRS activities were shown by the values obtained for [oxy-Hb] in the DLPFC (CH 1–4), DLPFC/frontopolar area (FPA) (CH 6–8, 13, 18), FPA (CH 11, 12, 16), triangular part of the inferior frontal gyrus (TP)/DLPFC (CH 5, 9), FPA/OFC (CH 15, 17), OFC/inferior prefrontal gyrus (IPG)/DLPFC (CH 19), OFC (CH 20, 21), and OFC/IPG/FPA (CH 22) (Figure 3A).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Prefrontal functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) activities in SHAPE as compared to REST. (A) Temporal differences and sequential pattern of the prefrontal fNIRS activities in F-SHAPE as compared to REST. Most channels presented significant (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) prefrontal activation in F-SHAPE as compared to REST. A sequential pattern of significant (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) prefrontal activations also presented temporally overlapped features in the onset and duration of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC)/frontopolar area (FPA)/orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) activities preceding, during, and following the task periods in F-SHAPE as compared to REST. (B) Temporal differences and sequential pattern of the prefrontal fNIRS activities in O-SHAPE as compared to REST. Most channels presented significant (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) prefrontal activation in O-SHAPE as compared to REST. A sequential pattern of significant (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) prefrontal activations also presented temporally overlapped onset and duration of the DLPFC/FPA/OFC activities preceding, during, and following the task periods in O-SHAPE as compared to REST.

Sequential Patterns of Prefrontal fNIRS Activities

As compared to REST, the sequential pattern of prefrontal fNIRS activities demonstrated significantly (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) temporally overlapped features in the onset and duration of various areas of the DLPFC/FPA/OFC activities during F-SHAPE (Figure 3A).

Comparison Between O-SHAPE and REST

Temporal Differences in Prefrontal fNIRS Activities

Statistically significant (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) increases in [oxy-Hb] were noted in the DLPFC (CH 2–4), DLPFC/FPA (CH 7, 8, 13), FPA (CH 11, 12, 16), TP/DLPFC (CH 5, 9), FPA/OFC (CH 15, 17), OFC/IPG/DLPFC (CH 19), OFC (CH 20, 21), and OFC/IPG/FPA (CH 22) (Figure 3B).

Sequential Patterns of Prefrontal fNIRS Activities

As compared to REST, O-SHAPE significantly (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) showed temporally overlapped features with regard to the onset and duration of prefrontal activities in the DLPFC, FPA, and OFC (Figure 3B).

Comparison Between F-SHAM and REST

Temporal Differences in Prefrontal fNIRS Activities

Statistically significant (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) increases in [oxy-Hb] were noted in the DLPFC (CH 1–4), DLPFC/FPA (CH 7, 8, 13, 18), BA/DLPFC (CH 9), FPA (CH 11, 12, 16), FPA/OFC (CH 17), OFC/IPG/DLPFC (CH 19), and OFC (CH 20, 21) (Figure 4A).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Prefrontal functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) activities in SHAM as compared to REST. (A) Temporal differences and sequential pattern of the prefrontal fNIRS activities in F-SHAM as compared REST. Most channels presented significant (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) prefrontal activation in F-SHAM as compared to REST. A sequential pattern of significant (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) prefrontal activations presented temporally overlapped features with regard to the onset and duration of the DLPFC/FPA/OFC activities between F-SHAM and REST. (B) Temporal differences and sequential pattern of the prefrontal fNIRS activities in O-SHAM as compared to REST. Several channels presented significant (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) prefrontal activations in the DLPFC and FPA in O-SHAM as compared to REST.

Sequential Patterns of Prefrontal fNIRS Activities

As compared to REST, F-SHAM significantly (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) showed temporally overlapped features with regard to the onset and duration of prefrontal fNIRS activities in the DLPFC, FPA, and OFC (Figure 4A).

Comparison Between O-SHAM and REST

Temporal Differences in Prefrontal fNIRS Activities

Statistically significant (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) increases in [oxy-Hb] were noted in the DLPFC (CH 1), FPA (CH 11, 12, 16), and DLPFC/FPA (CH 14) (Figure 4B). A weak association of prefrontal activities in the DLPFC and FPA was seen in O-SHAM as compared to REST.

Sequential Patterns in Prefrontal fNIRS Activities

As compared to REST, O-SHAM showed significant (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) prefrontal activations in several channels in the DLPFC and FPA, and those activities diminished early during the task phase (Figure 4B).

Comparison Between F-SHAPE and SHAM

Temporal Differences in Prefrontal fNIRS Activities

Statistically significant (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) increases were seen regarding the activities for [oxy-Hb] in the DLPFC/FPA (CH 7), FPA (CH 11, 12, 16), FPA/OFC (CH 15, 17), OFC/IPG/DLPFC (CH 19), and OFC (CH 20, 21) (Figure 5A).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Prefrontal functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) activities in SHAPE as compared to SHAM. (A) Temporal differences and sequential pattern of prefrontal fNIRS activities in F-SHAPE as compared to F-SHAM. Prefrontal activations presented significant (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) in the frontopolar area (FPA) and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) during F-SHAPE as compared to F-SHAM. A sequential pattern of significantly shifting features in the onset and burst was noted from the FPA to the OFC during F-SHAPE as compared to F-SHAM. (B) Temporal differences and sequential pattern of the prefrontal fNIRS activities in O-SHAPE as compared to O-SHAM. Most channels presented significant (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) prefrontal activation in O-SHAPE as compared to O-SHAM. A sequential pattern of significant (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) prefrontal activation resulted in temporally overlapped features in the onset and burst of the DLPFC/FPA/OFC activities during O-SHAPE as compared to O-SHAM.

Sequential Patterns of Prefrontal fNIRS Activities

As compared to SHAM, F-SHAPE findings showed that the onset and duration of prefrontal cortex activities were significantly (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) shifted in the different prefrontal areas of the FPA and OFC. (Figure 5A). Anatomically isolated FPA activities in CH 11 (R-FPA), CH 12 (L-FPA), and CH 16 (M-FPA) preceded task onset and continued beyond the task period, whereas OFC activities in CH 20 (R-OFC) and CH 21 (L-OFC) were initiated during the middle of the task phase and lasted beyond the end of the FPA activities in the post-task period. FPA activities in the pre-task and task periods preceded the onset of OFC activities, and OFC activities in the post-task period followed the offset of FPA activities. Thus, a comparison of F-SHAPE and F-SHAM presented temporally shifting features in the onset and duration from the FPA to the OFC (Figure 5A).

Comparison Between O-SHAPE and SHAM

Temporal Differences in Prefrontal fNIRS Activities

When O-SHAPE was compared with O-SHAM, statistically (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) significant increased [oxy-Hb] were shown in the DLPFC (CH 4), TP/DLPFC (CH 5 and 9), FPA (CH 7, 11, 12, and 16), DLPFC/FPA (CH 8, 13, and 14), FPA/OFC (CH 15 and 17), OFC/IPG/DLPFC (CH 19), OFC (CH 20 and 21), and OFC/IPG/FPA (CH 22) (Figure 5B).

Sequential Patterns of Prefrontal fNIRS Activities

As compared to O-SHAM, O-SHAPE significantly (two-way repeated measures ANOVA and Bonferroni t-test: p < 0.05) showed temporally overlapped features with regard to the onset and duration of prefrontal activities in the DLPFC, FPA, and OFC (Figure 5B).

Discussion

The results of the present study suggest no significant difference with regard to the number of correct answers during trials between oral and finger SHAPE discrimination as compared to REST. Additionally, a statistical difference for the prefrontal fNIRS activity between oral and finger shape discrimination was noted only in CH 1 of the DLPFC. A previous study also found no significant differences for prefrontal activities between finger and oral shape discrimination using fMRI (Fujii et al., 2011).

In the current study, we conducted randomly ordered experimental blocks of REST, nonsense SHAM, and SHAPE with six test pieces, following instructions given for the experimental procedures and a block rehearsal without any test pieces, the same as in previous studies by Reed et al. (2004) and Kagawa et al. (2014). SHAM blocks were used to compensate the differences in prefrontal activities between oral and fingers motor task sequences during shape discrimination. The differences in the prefrontal cortex activities between the SHAPE and SHAM blocks should represent the process of target image generation and manipulation in both the finger and oral shape discrimination tasks.

As results, this study presented the significant prefrontal activities in large numbers of channels between the finger SHAM and REST blocks, whereas significant prefrontal activities were only noted in a few channels between the oral SHAM and REST blocks (Figures 4A,B). Because the influences from the motor task sequences were not controlled by the use of SHAM blocks in a previous study (Fujii et al., 2011), significant differences in the prefrontal cortex activities were not observed between oral and finger shape discrimination. Further, a comparison between the finger SHAPE and SHAM showed the distinctive prefrontal activities temporally shifting from the FPA to the OFC (Figure 5A), whereas the temporally overlapped activities in the DLPFC/FPA/OFC were presented between the oral SHAPE and SHAM blocks (Figure 5B).

In consideration of the temporal hierarchical cognition process in the prefrontal cortex (Christoff and Gabrieli, 2000; Jeon, 2014; Rahnev, 2017; Gurtubay-Antolin et al., 2018), the prefrontal activities shifting from the FPA to the OFC may indicate the segregated serial prefrontal processing of tactual shape manipulation and decision making in finger shape discrimination (Daw et al., 2006; Wallis, 2007; Raposo et al., 2011; Euston et al., 2012; Laureiro-Martínez et al., 2014). In contrast, no significant difference regarding the activation of the DLPFC was found between the SHAPE and SHAM blocks in finger shape discrimination (Figures 5A,B). It has been reported that the DLPFC may be involved in monitoring and manipulating externally generated information (Christoff and Gabrieli, 2000). It is therefore assumed that the no significant difference for DLPFC activation between the finger SHAPE and SHAM blocks suggests that tactual finger shape discrimination does not require intensive DLPFC participation in generating a target image.

In contrast, as compared with the nonsense SHAM block, temporally overlapped prefrontal activities were presented in the DLPFC/FPA/OFC during the oral SHAPE block. It has been reported that the FPA is involved in the monitoring and manipulation of internal generated information, whereas the OFC is involved in the internally driven decision making (Christoff and Gabrieli, 2000). Thus, temporally overlapped prefrontal activities in the DLPFC/FPA/OFC during oral shape discrimination suggest the parallel and repetitive involvement of generation, manipulation, and decision making in order to form a reliable representation of a target object.

Conclusion

The present results showed no significant difference with regard to the number of correct answers during trials between oral and finger SHAPE discrimination. Additionally, no statistical difference for prefrontal fNIRS activity between oral and finger shape discrimination was noted, except in CH 1. Prefrontal fNIRS activities during finger and oral shape discrimination were measured during rest, nonsense sham, and shape discrimination performed by healthy subjects. The findings indicated shifting prefrontal activities from the FPA to the OFC during finger SHAPE as compared with SHAM blocks, which suggests segregated serial processing from the manipulation of a target image to the decision making during finger shape discrimination. In contrast, the overlapped prefrontal activities in the DLPFC/FPA/OFC during oral SHAPE blocks may suggest the paralleled prefrontal processing of the repetitive involvement of generation, manipulation, and decision making in order to form a reliable representation of target objects. This study presented the characterized prefrontal activities during tactual oral and finger discrimination task performance in healthy subjects.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee of Nihon University School of Dentistry at Matsudo (EC 19-14-015-1). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author Contributions

NN designed the study and wrote the manuscript. KK collected experimental data. KK and TI analyzed the obtained data. SI, HE, MS, TU, IK, and KS contributed to the interpretation of the significance of the obtained data. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the Dental Support Co., Ltd. and a JSPS KAKENHI (Grant No. 25463029).

Conflict of Interest

TU and IK are employed by Dental Support Co., Ltd.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Shingo Kawasaki and Takumi Inakazu for the excellent technical assistance with the topographical maps.

References

Brett, M., Johnsrude, I. S., and Owen, A. M. (2002). The problem of functional localization in the human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 243–249. doi: 10.1038/nrn756

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christoff, K., and Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2000). The frontopolar cortex and human cognition: Evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex. Psychobiology 28, 168–186.

Google Scholar

Cohen-Kdoshay, O., and Meiran, N. (2009). The representation of instructions operates like a prepared reflex: flanker compatibility effects found in first trial following S–R instructions. Exp. Psychol. 56, 128–133. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.2.128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, D. L., Neelin, P., Peters, T. M., and Evans, A. C. (1994). Automatic 3D intersubject registration of MR volumetric data in standardized Talairach space. J. Comput. Assist. Tomogr. 18, 192–205.

Google Scholar

Daw, N. D., O’Doherty, J. P., Dayan, P., Seymour, B., and Dolan, R. J. (2006). Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans. Nature 441, 876–879. doi: 10.1038/nature04766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehlis, A. C., Herrmann, M. J., Plichta, M. M., and Fallgatter, A. J. (2007). Cortical activation during two verbal fluency tasks in schizophrenic patients and healthy controls as assessed by multi-channel near-infrared spectroscopy. Psychiatry Res. 156, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emch, M., von Bastian, C. C., and Koch, K. (2019). Neural correlates of verbal working memory: an fMRI meta-analysis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:180. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Euston, D. R., Gruber, A. J., and McNaughton, B. L. (2012). The role of medial prefrontal cortex in memory and decision making. Neuron. 76, 1057–1070. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujii, R., Takahashi, T., Toyomura, A., Miyamoto, T., Ueno, T., and Yokoyama, A. (2011). Comparison of cerebral activation involved in oral and manual stereognosis. J. Clin. Neurosci. 18, 1520–1523. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2011.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fukutake, M., Ogawa, T., Ikebe, K., Mihara, Y., Inomata, C., Takeshita, H., et al. (2019). Impact of cognitive function on oral perception in independently living older people. Clin. Oral Investig. 23, 267–271. doi: 10.1007/s00784-018-2433-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gurtubay-Antolin, A., León-Cabrera, P., and Rodríguez-Fornells, A. (2018). Neural evidence of hierarchical cognitive control during haptic processing: an fMRI study. eNeuro 5:e0295-18. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0295-18.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hering, A., Rautenberg, M., von Bloh, P., Schnitzspahn, K., Ballhausen, N., Ihle, A., et al. (2018). Examining the role of rehearsal in old-old adults’ working memory. Eur. J. Ageing 16, 63–71. doi: 10.1007/s10433-018-0461-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoshi, Y., Kobayashi, N., and Tamura, M. (2001). Interpretation of near-infrared spectroscopy signals: a study with a newly developed perfused rat brain model. J. Appl. Physiol. 90, 1657–1662. doi: 10.1152/jappl.2001.90.5.1657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ikebe, K., Amemiya, M., Morii, K., Matsuda, K., Furuya-Yoshinaka, M., and Nokubi, T. (2007). Comparison of oral stereognosis in relation to age and the use of complete dentures. J. Oral Rehabil. 34, 345–350. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2842.2007.01687.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Irving, J. B. (1968). Stereognosis. Res. Med. 6, 25–27.

Google Scholar

Jackson, M. C., and Raymond, J. E. (2008). Familiarity enhances visual working memory for faces. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 34, 556–568. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.3.556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobs, R., Bou Serhal, C., and van Steenberghe, D. (1997). The stereognostic ability of natural dentitions versus implant-supported fixed prostheses or overdentures. Clin. Oral Investig. 1, 89–94. doi: 10.1007/s007840050017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobs, R., Bou Serhal, C., and van Steenberghe, D. (1998). Oral stereognosis: a review of the literature. Clin. Oral Investig. 2, 3–10. doi: 10.1007/s007840050035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeon, H. A. (2014). Hierarchical processing in the prefrontal cortex in a variety of cognitive domains. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:223. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kagawa, T., Narita, N., Iwaki, S., Kawasaki, S., Kamiya, K., and Minakuchi, S. (2014). Does shape discrimination by the mouth activate the parietal and occipital lobes? – near-infrared spectroscopy study. PLoS One 9:e108685. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0108685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaiba, Y., Hirano, S., and Hayakawa, I. (2006). Palatal coverage disturbance in masticatory function. J. Med. Dent. Sci. 53, 1–6.

Google Scholar

Kamiya, K., Narita, N., and Iwaki, S. (2016). Improved prefrontal activity and chewing performance as function of wearing denture in partially edentulous elderly individuals: functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. PLoS One 11:e0158070. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0158070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katsuki, F., and Constantinidis, C. (2014). Bottom-up and top-down attention: different processes and overlapping neural systems. Neuroscientist 20, 509–521. doi: 10.1177/1073858413514136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawagishi, S., Tanaka, T., Yoshino, K., and Shimodozono, M. (2014). Brain activity during stereognostic discrimination using the tongue measured by functional near-infrared spectroscopy. J. Aging Sci. 2, 128. doi: 10.4172/2329-8847.1000128

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klem, G. H., Lüders, H. O., Jasper, H. H., and Elger, C. (1999). The ten-twenty electrode system of the International federation. The international federation of clinical neurophysiology. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. Suppl. 52, 3–6.

Google Scholar

Laureiro-Martínez, D., Canessa, N., Brusoni, S., Zollo, M., Hare, T., Alemanno, F., et al. (2014). Frontopolar cortex and decision-making efficiency: comparing brain activity of experts with different professional background during an exploration-exploitation task. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:927. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00927

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, Y.-s, Lu, M.-j, and Ko, H. (2007). Effects of skill training on working memory capacity. Learn. Instruct. 17, 336–344. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2007.02.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mantecchini, G., Bassi, F., Pera, P., and Preti, G. (1998). Oral stereognosis in edentulous subjects rehabilitated with complete removable dentures. J. Oral Rehabil. 25, 185–189. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2842.1998.00221.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meenakshi, S., Gujjari, A. K., Thippeswamy, H. N., and Raghunath, N. (2014). Evaluation of oral stereognostic ability after rehabilitating patients with complete dentures: in vivo study. J. Indian Prosthodont. Soc. 14, 363–368. doi: 10.1007/s13191-013-0333-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, F., Link, I., Fuhr, K., and Utz, K. H. (1995). Studies on adaptation to complete dentures. Part II: oral stereognosis and tactile sensibility. J. Oral Rehabil. 22, 759–767. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2842.1995.tb00220.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Narita, N., Kamiya, K., Yamamura, K., Kawasaki, S., Matsumoto, T., and Tanaka, N. (2009). Chewing-related prefrontal cortex activation while wearing partial denture prosthesis: pilot study. J. Prosthodont. Res. 53, 126–135.

Google Scholar

Naseer, N., and Hong, K. S. (2015). fNIRS-based brain-computer interfaces: a review. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 28:3. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okamoto, M., and Dan, I. (2005). Automated cortical projection of head-surface locations for transcranial functional brain mapping. Neuroimage. 26, 18–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rahnev, D. (2017). Top-down control of perceptual decision making by the prefrontal cortex. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 26, 1–6. doi: 10.1177/0963721417709807

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raposo, A., Vicens, L., Clithero, J. A., Dobbins, I. G., and Huettel, S. A. (2011). Contributions of frontopolar cortex to judgments about self, others and relations. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 6, 260–269. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reed, C. L., Shoham, S., and Halgren, E. (2004). Neural substrates of tactile object recognition: an fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 21, 236–246. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sathian, K. (2016). Analysis of haptic information in the cerebral cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 116, 1795–1806. doi: 10.1152/jn.00546.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seidler, R. D., Bo, J., and Anguera, J. A. (2012). Neurocognitive contributions to motor skill learning: the role of working memory. J. Mot. Behav. 44, 445–453. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2012.672348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shomstein, S. (2012). Cognitive functions of the posterior parietal cortex: top-down and bottom-up attentional control. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:38. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, A. K., Okamoto, M., Dan, H., Jurcak, V., and Dan, I. (2005). Spatial registration of multichannel multi-subject fNIRS data to MNI space without MRI. Neuroimage 27, 842–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, V., and Mattoo, K. A. (2014). Oral stereognosis pattern in patients with dental prosthesis in the elderly population. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Physiol. 1, 211–215.

Google Scholar

Strangman, G., Culver, J. P., Thompson, J. H., and Boas, D. A. (2002). A quantitative comparison of simultaneous BOLD fMRI and NIRS recordings during functional brain activation. Neuroimage 17, 719–731.

Google Scholar

Suto, T., Fukuda, M., Ito, M., Uehara, T., and Mikuni, M. (2004). Multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy in depression and schizophrenia: cognitive brain activation study. Biol. Psychiatry 55, 501–511. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsuzuki, D., and Dan, I. (2014). Spatial registration for functional near-infrared spectroscopy: from channel position on the scalp to cortical location in individual and group analyses. Neuroimage 85, 92–103. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., et al. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallis, J. D. (2007). Orbitofrontal cortex and its contribution to decision-making. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 30, 31–56. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, W., Qiu, C., Ota, T., Sawada, M., Kishimoto, N., and Kishimoto, T. (2013). Effects of TAI CHI exercise on attention in healthy elderly subjects as measured by near-infrared spectroscopy during the stroop task. J. Nara Med. Assoc. 64, 79–86.

Google Scholar

Xu, X., Hanganu-Opatz, I. L., and Bieler, M. (2020). Cross-talk of low-level sensory and high-level cognitive processing: development, mechanisms, and relevance for cross-modal abilities of the brain. Front. Neurorobot. 14:7. doi: 10.3389/fnbot.2020.00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, D., Huang, R., Yoo, S. H., Shin, M. J., Yoon, J. A., Shin, Y.-I., et al. (2020). Detection of mild cognitive impairment using convolutional neural network: temporal-feature maps of functional near-Infrared Spectroscopy. Front. Aging Neurosci. 12:141. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.00141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zigmond, A. S., and Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 67, 361–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: prefrontal cortex, parallel activity, serial activity, oral, finger, shape discrimination

Citation: Narita N, Kamiya K, Iwaki S, Ishii T, Endo H, Shimosaka M, Uchida T, Kantake I and Shibutani K (2021) Activation of Prefrontal Cortex in Process of Oral and Finger Shape Discrimination: fNIRS Study. Front. Neurosci. 15:588593. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.588593

Received: 03 August 2020; Accepted: 04 January 2021;
Published: 05 February 2021.

Edited by:

Kazutaka Takahashi, University of Chicago, United States

Reviewed by:

Keum-Shik Hong, Pusan National University, South Korea
Melanie K. Fleming, University of Oxford, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2021 Narita, Kamiya, Iwaki, Ishii, Endo, Shimosaka, Uchida, Kantake and Shibutani. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Noriyuki Narita, narita.noriyuki@nihon-u.ac.jp

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.