Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 22 September 2015
Sec. Brain Imaging and Stimulation
Volume 9 - 2015 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00486

Multi-modal representation of effector modality in frontal cortex during rule switching

  • 1School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, UK
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Bournemouth, Bournemouth, UK
  • 3Exeter MR Research Centre, University of Exeter, Exeter, UK

We report a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study which investigated whether brain areas involved in updating task rules within the frontal lobe of the cerebral cortex show activity related to the modality of motor response used in the task. Participants performed a rule switching task using different effector modalities. In some blocks participants responded with left/right button presses, whilst in other blocks left/right saccades were required. The color of a Cue event instructed a left or right response based upon a rule, followed by a Feedback which indicated whether the rule was to stay the same or “Flip” on the next trial. The findings revealed variation in the locus of activity within the ventrolateral frontal cortex dependent upon effector modality. Other frontal areas showed no significant difference in activity between response epochs but changed their pattern of connectivity with posterior cortical areas dependent upon response. Multivariate analysis revealed that the pattern of activity evoked by Flip rule Feedbacks within an apparently supra modal frontal region (dorsolateral frontal cortex) discriminated between response epochs. The results are consistent with the existence of multi-modal representations of stimulus-response (SR) rules within the frontal cerebral cortex.

Introduction

Performing many complex tasks involves the execution of actions based upon arbitrary mappings between stimulus and response. Previous patient and neuroimaging investigations have suggested that the lateral and medial frontal regions of the human cerebral cortex might be important in maintaining arbitrary stimulus-response (SR) rules and resolving response conflict in rule based tasks (Milner, 1963; Asaad et al., 1998; White and Wise, 1999; MacDonald et al., 2000; Passingham et al., 2000; Swainson et al., 2003; Boettinger and D’Esposito, 2005; Hampshire and Owen, 2006; Parris et al., 2007; Petrides, 2008; Wallis, 2008; Chouinard and Goodale, 2009). A key motivation of the current study was to investigate whether frontal regions anterior to the motor cortex show regional variation in activity dependent upon motor response modality (eye or hand) during a SR rule switching task. The alternative is that frontal regions encode information entirely supra-modally i.e., the concept of left or right, as opposed to the movement of a particular muscle group or effector organ to the left or right. If this were the case then activity should be independent of response modality.

A common assumption is that cognitive representations of task rules are maintained within the frontal cerebral cortex which modifies specific SR associations via top-down influences on sub-cortical and posterior cortical structures (Desimone and Duncan, 1995; Gehring and Knight, 2000; Miller and Cohen, 2001; Parris et al., 2007). This account predicts that given a task in which the same rule can be applied across more than one motor response modality frontal regions should show different patterns of connectivity with posterior cortical areas containing modality specific representations (e.g., for an eye movement or a hand movement). If cognitive representations of rules were truly supra-modal and distinct from representations of stimulus and response then they would not be able to exert top down control of action in this manner. Switching between different response modes given this type of representation would require an implausible state of neural “functional pleomorphism” in which individual neurons would need to actively modify the physical structure of their axonal network within a timeframe of seconds to select one type of response or another (Nachev et al., 2008).

At the same time a defining feature of human cognition is the ability to form abstract concepts and generalise across specific stimuli and contexts in a supra-modal manner (e.g., Deacon, 1997; Kosslyn et al., 2006). An important question therefore is how such representation is possible given the constraints of neural architecture described above. One possibility would be that even apparently supra-modal brain areas might possess modality specific tuning at a finer spatial scale such that sub regions or populations of neurons could selectively influence one or another mode of responding whilst activity across the entire population of neurons represents a general rule concept independent of specific responses to be executed. We term such a form of representation as “multi-modal” rather than truly supra-modal. If the spatial organization of such regions was consistent across participants at the local scale then functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) should reveal their existence within the frontal cortex. However, if response specific sub-regions were idiosyncratically spatially distributed across participants, conventional univariate fMRI analysis averaging across individual participants may not be able to detect them. Another approach to delineate such an organization would be to examine the pattern of voxels activating in individual participants in different tasks. Woolgar et al. (2011) applied multi-voxel pattern analysis (MVPA) to fMRI data acquired whilst participants switched between different task rules and SR mappings linking four locations on a screen with index and middle finger key presses of the left and right hand. Whilst univariate analysis revealed no voxels with significant activity related to particular rules or SR associations, MVPA showed task rule related activity within the “multiple demands” cognitive network of the dorsal and ventral lateral frontal (Duncan and Owen, 2000).

In an earlier fMRI study, we examined brain activity during a color-response rule switching task (Parris et al., 2007). In this task a colored “Cue” presented at fixation instructed a left or right button press dependent upon an arbitrary rule (e.g., blue = left; yellow = right). The rule linking color and response can change dependent upon a “Feedback” signal which instructs participants to “Hold” or “Flip” the rule on the next trial. By jittering the timing of Cue, Response and Feedback events within a trial we were able to resolve BOLD responses to discrete events representing the response preparation and execution period and activity related to Flip/Hold rule Feedbacks. It was found that activity within dorsal and ventrolateral frontal areas, along with the medial frontal cortex was correlated with feedbacks that signaled the requirement to flip the rule on the next trial. Interestingly, a control condition revealed that lateral frontal regions activated equally strongly to infrequent/salient Feedback events which did not signal the need to update rules, whereas only the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and basal ganglia responded selectively to the requirement to update SR mappings. It was concluded that lateral frontal regions are engaged in an evaluative process which determines whether current representations of rules need to be maintained or modified in the light of salient stimulus events, whereas medial frontal cortex implements updating of actual SR mappings.

We have also carried out studies of patients with neurological damage using the same SR rule switching task with either saccades or manual button press responses (Hodgson et al., 2007, 2013; Huddy et al., 2011). These suggest that some frontal regions mediating cognitive control operations might be segregated according to modality of response. Husain et al. (2003) tested a patient who had a highly localized lesion of the supplementary eye fields on both a manual and saccadic version of the color-response rule switching task. The patient was found to be selectively impaired in an oculomotor but not a manual version of the task such that they made increased errors following a rule change for saccades but not manual responses (Husain et al., 2003; Parton et al., 2007). Other patient work has shown that lesions to the ventrolateral frontal cortex bilaterally lead to significant increases in errors in a saccade version of the rule switching task (Hodgson et al., 2007).

Although a number of previous studies have investigated cognitive control mechanisms across sensory modalities (e.g., Roberts and Hall, 2008) few previous fMRI studies have compared activity between different motor modalities during cognitive tasks. These have revealed some evidence for the existence of sub-regions within the frontal cortex anterior to primary motor cortex organized by motor effector modality. For example, Leung and Cai (2007) compared two versions of a stop signal task for which participants could respond with either a left/right manual button press or a saccade. These authors found that Stop signal trials recruited the ventrolateral frontal cortex bilaterally under both conditions. However, saccade trials were associated with a more dorsal and anterior locus of activity whilst manual response trials recruited more ventral and posterior parts of the inferior frontal gyrus. Connolly et al. (2000) also compared stimulus directed and “anti” responding for saccades and manual pointing movements. They report that regions traditionally conceived of as oculomotor (e.g., FEF) are also activated during the programming of manual pointing responses. Whilst no regions were found to be selectively activated during anti-saccades relative to anti-pointing, some regions of the ventrolateral frontal cortex did show activity specific to manual pointing which was not observed during eye movement response blocks.

Although previous studies have examined different response modalities during cognitive tasks, no earlier work has compared activity between eye and hand movements during closely matched rule/task switching procedures. In our rule switching task described above (e.g., Parris et al., 2007), participants must intermittently update task rules in response to a Feedback signal. This affords participants the opportunity to reconfigure SR mappings in advance of the next response cue onset and at least some of the activity seen in response to “Flip” rule feedbacks may reflect this reconfiguration process. However, without comparison of activity across different response modes it is not possible to discriminate between activity related to reconfiguration of SR mappings and updating of supra-modal representations of rules. Differences in activity in response to “Flip” feedbacks between Eye and Hand movement versions of the task are likely to be related to reconfiguration of response specific task mappings in advance of the next trial. Moreover, such advanced reconfiguration of SR mappings might also be reflected in changes in the pattern of connectivity between frontal and posterior areas.

The study reported here used a color-response rule switching task closely based upon that used in our previous work (Hodgson et al., 2007; Parris et al., 2007). As in earlier studies the rule linking a colored cue and a left/right response could reverse several times during the course of the test, allowing us to examine event evoked activity during rule updating as well as response execution. Unlike our previous fMRI study each subject performed two versions of the task in alternating epochs within a single scanning run. In half the epochs participants responded with a left or right button press, whilst in the other epochs they executed saccades to the left or right. Data were analyzed at the group level to examine which regions activated to different task events in either a supra-modal or a response specific manner. Of particular interest was whether activity specific to “Flip” rule Feedback events was modulated by motor response (even though no motor response had to be executed). As it is possible that there might be no consistent pattern of spatial localization by response across participants we also explored individual participant data for evidence of response mode specific activity using multivoxel pattern analysis (Schrouff et al., 2013). Finally, we also examined changes in connectivity of frontal regions with response mode using psycho-physiological interaction (PPI) analysis.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Fifteen healthy right handed participants ranging in age from 18–36 years participated in the experiment. The study was approved by the School of Psychology Ethics Committee, University of Exeter and the Management Board of the Exeter Magnetic Resonance Research Centre, University of Exeter, UK. All participants gave written informed consent to participate in the study.

Task and Procedure

Visual stimuli were presented on a back projection screen positioned at the foot end of the MRI scanner and viewed via a mirror mounted on the subject head coil. The screen subtended 16 degrees of visual arc to the subject lying prone in the scanner. Button press responses and manual reaction times were measured using two fiber optic button boxes held in the participants’ right and left hands. Eye movements were recorded using an Applied Science Labs 504 eye tracker with long range optics, mounted 1.5 m from the head end of the scanner bore, acquiring an eye image via a mirror fitted to the head coil. Eye position was sampled at 60 Hz and analyzed using custom written LabView software which allowed extraction of saccadic onset times, target-response latencies, movement direction and amplitude. Saccades were defined as periods for which eye velocity exceeded 30°/s for three consecutive samples and exceeded a displacement criterion of one degree of visual arc from the eye starting position.

A schematic of the task is shown in Figure 1. Each trial in the task began with the presentation of a blue or yellow colored circle (the response contingent Cue event). Subjects made a left or right behavioral response dependent upon the color of the cue and the current rule linking color with direction. For “Eye” (saccade) blocks participants were instructed to fixate one of two peripheral response locations (marked by two onscreen response boxes located approximately five degrees to the left or right of the central location and displayed during Eye epochs only). When the cue stimulus was extinguished they then had to make a return saccade to fixate the central location. The word “hold” or “flip” was then presented at fixation to indicate to the participant whether to hold or reverse the current rule on the following trial (Feedback event). In order to resolve the fMRI response to these discrete events within a trial i.e., Cue and Feedback, the period between each event and trial was varied randomly between 2200, 4400 and 10200 ms such that inter-event intervals did not correspond to multiples of the TR period. Varying the period between individual events in this manner within as well as between trials ensures that discrete events can be resolved within a trial (Parris et al., 2007).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic of rule switching task. Participants switch between two rules linking a centrally presented colored Cue with a motor response to the left or right. The participant either holds or reverses the rule linking cue and response on the next trial based on the Feedback event following response execution (“Hold”/“Flip”). Each participant completed four blocks, two blocks each for which either Manual press button responses (“Hand epochs”) or saccades were made to the left or right (“Eye epochs”).

Each block consisted of 21 trials and each subject completed four blocks in one continuous scanning run, with two blocks requiring responses to be made with a manual press button response (Hand blocks) and two blocks required a saccadic response to be made to a response box located to the left or right of the central location (Eye blocks). Eye and hand blocks were presented alternately with half the subjects completing the task in the order Eye-Hand-Eye-Hand and the other group of subjects in the order Hand-Eye-Hand-Eye. Each task block was preceded by an instruction screen which told subjects to either “Respond with Eyes” or “Respond with Hands” as well as showing the rule to be used on the first trial (e.g., blue = respond left; yellow = respond right) which was displayed for 10 s, followed by a variable delay period before presentation of the first colored cue stimulus. Participants completed one practice block of each version of the task outside the scanner just prior to the scanning session.

fMRI Data Acquisition

Scanning was performed on a 1.5 T Philips Gyroscan magnet at the Peninsula MR Research centre, University of Exeter, UK. A T2*-weighted echoplanar sequence was used (TR = 3000 ms, TE = 50 ms, flip angle 90°, 32 transverse slices, 3.6 × 3.6 × 4 mm, ascending acquisition). Four hundred and sixty five image volumes were acquired in one continuous scanning run. An additional five “dummy” scans were performed at the start of each block prior to the start of the stimulus sequence.

fMRI analysis

Univariate Analysis

Data were analyzed using SPM8 software. The images were realigned, unwarped to remove variance caused by movement-by-field-inhomogeneity interactions, normalized to a standard EPI template, and smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 6 mm full-width at half maximum. Inter-event periods were selected such that the response Cue and Feedback event sequences were temporally de-correlated with each other and the TR period (3000 ms TR relative to 2200, 4400 and 10200 ms inter-event period see task and procedure above), This design allowed unique regressors to be derived for each event type by convolving the event onset times with a canonical hemodynamic response function. The eight different event types modeled at the individual subject level were:

Response Cue event onsets: Hold rule-Hand; Flip rule-Hand; Hold rule-Eye; Flip rule-Eye; Feedback event onsets: Hold rule-Hand; Flip rule-Hand; Hold rule-Eye; Flip rule-Eye.

Response error events were also modeled for participants who made such errors. Separate regressors were included in the first level model corresponding to the timing of Hand response errors and Eye Response errors. Trials on which errors occurred were not represented in the regressors corresponding to the main event and trial types listed above.

A second level (random effects) factorial statistical model was applied to the one sample t-tests derived from each of the regressors described above for each individual subject’s data. A three-way analysis of variance included three factors with two levels each: event type (Cue vs. Feedback); Trial type (Hold vs. Flip rule) and Response type (Hand vs. Eye). All factors and levels were assumed to be non-independent (repeated measures) and the statistics and degrees of freedom used for inference were calculated accordingly. Two approaches were taken in the comparison of activation by response. Direct statistical contrasts between conditions of interest were first carried out (two sample t-tests testing the main effects of Response and F-tests for interaction effects of Trial type with Response). In addition, an exclusive masking approach was also taken to explore response mode specific activity in which activity for one response type was masked with the equivalent contrast for the other response using a very low statistical threshold (p < 0.05 uncorrected) to derive the exclusive mask (e.g., Vilberg and Rugg, 2008). We reasoned that this exclusive masking approach would be more likely to identify regions which show significant activation differences in some participants but which do not consistently generalise across participants, whereas direct statistical contrasts would only reveal areas which showed highly consistent response specific activations across participants.

For key comparisons of interest, we also applied a number of additional inclusive and exclusive masking and region of interest (ROI) analyses derived from other relevant contrasts (e.g., Flip vs. Hold Feedback event contrast masked by the main effect of Cue event activity for Eye epochs) or a priori hypotheses arising from previous fMRI studies (e.g., Leung and Cai, 2007; Parris et al., 2007). In the case of all contrasts utilizing a mask or ROI a Small Volume Correction (SVC) was applied to determine which voxels survived correction for multiple statistical comparisons. Anatomical ROIs were generated using the Wake Forest University (WFU) Pickatlas toolbox (Maldjian et al., 2003).

The conjunction analyses across Eye and Hand epochs tested the conjunction null hypothesis (see Friston et al., 2005; Nichols et al., 2005 for discussion of global vs. conjunction null hypothesis testing).

In all cases only those activations which passed a significance criterion of p < 0.05 Family Wise Error (FWE) corrected for multiple comparisons are reported below and the results of all other statistical contrasts carried out at the individual or group level revealed no significantly activated voxels based upon this criteria. All coordinates given in tables are Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates. Anatomical labels and estimated Brodmann area numbers were generated by converting MNI coordinates to Talaraich and interrogation of the Talaraich Daemon database tool (Lancaster et al., 2000).

Psycho-Physiological Interaction Analysis

In order to assess the hypothesised biasing influence of frontal areas on posterior brain regions during updating of SR rules, a PPI analysis was carried out (Friston et al., 1997). PPI analyses assess whether the correlation in activity between two distant brain areas differs dependent upon psychological context—in other words, whether there is an interaction between a psychological variable and the functional coupling between two brain areas. In this case the “psychological” variable was whether a saccade or a press button response had to be made during a given epoch and the physiological variable was the BOLD signal in a given spherical volume of interest (VOI) or “seed” region. Two seed VOIs in the frontal cortex were used in the analysis reported below based upon the first level conjunction analysis for regions activated equally by Flip rule Feedbacks during both hand and eye epochs.

Multi-Voxel Pattern Analysis

Multivariate analysis of the data was carried out to investigate whether differences between response epochs were apparent in the pattern of activity across voxels rather than within individual clusters of voxels. The Pattern Recognition Neuroimaging Toolbox (PRoNTo; Schrouff et al., 2013) was used to implement a binary classifier based upon the support vector machine approach (Cristianini and Shawe-Taylor, 2000) using realigned, unwarped, normalized, unsmoothed data from each participant (use of spatially smoothed data not being recommended with MVPA/PRoNTo). In simple terms, the classifier algorithm works out the best array of weightings to apply to voxels within the specified mask in order to discriminate between two experimental variables (In the present case whether the motor response to be executed in response to the next Cue event was to be an Eye or Hand movement). Activity elicited by Flip feedback events was analyzed in this way to determine whether the overall pattern of activating voxels differed systematically between the two response modality conditions. In order to avoid the classifier algorithm “over fitting” the data PRoNTO utilizes a procedure in which the classifier is tested for discrimination accuracy on a subset of the total scan run which was not used during the “learning” phase, a so-called “leave-one-block-out” approach to cross validation within a single dataset.

Results

Behavioral Data

None of the participants made any errors in which the wrong type of response was initiated in a given epoch (i.e., an eye movement when a manual response was required and vice versa). Directional errors in eye and hand responding were also very rare. A total of five such errors were made in Eye response blocks across all subjects and six manual response errors equating to <4% errors overall. For saccadic, but not manual responding, “corrected” errors were observed on some trials in which a saccade was initially directed in the wrong direction but the movement was quickly corrected such that the eye was directed to the correct response box. A total of only 10 such corrected errors were observed across all participants. Means and standard errors of response times across participants for eye and hand blocks were 865 ± 41 ms and 774 ± 46 ms respectively. A two-way ANOVA with response type and trial type (Hold/Flip rule), revealed no significant effect of response type, trial type or interaction between response and trial type on reaction times.

fMRI analysis

Main Effect of Trial Type (Hold vs. Flip Rule)

No voxels were significantly activated for the comparison of Trial Type (Hold > Flip or Flip > Hold) for Cue events. The comparison of Flip compared to Hold rule trial Feedback events highlighted superior bilateral parietal cortex, the anterior cingulate, left middle frontal gyrus, left inferior frontal gyrus and bi-lateral cerebellum (Table 1). The reverse contrast (Hold > Flip) revealed no significantly activated voxels.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Coordinates of peak activations clusters for the contrast of “Flip” vs. “Hold” trial Feedback events.

Main Effect of Response Type (Hand vs. Eye)

Areas which showed more or less activity during the two response mode epochs (Eye/Hand) were compared via both direct statistical contrasts and the exclusive masking technique for both Cue and Feedback elicited event activations.

Direct statistical contrasts for Feedback and Cue evoked activity (independent of Trial type) revealed no differences between Eye and Hand blocks. Cue locked activity in Eye blocks masked by Hand blocks showed activity in the left frontal eye fields (FEF), medial parietal and occipital cortex along with the Middle temporal and parahippocampal gyrus (Table 2; Figure 2). The reverse comparison of Cue related activity in Hand epochs exclusively masked by Eye epochs highlighted the bilateral post-central gyrus, cerebellum and middle occipital gyrus (Table 2; Figure 2). For Feedback evoked activations exclusive masking analysis showed Hand epoch activity exclusive to the right post-central gyrus and left inferior frontal gyrus. No activity evoked by Feedback events was found to be exclusive to Eye blocks.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Cue event activations which were greater for Eye vs. Hand or Hand vs. Eye response epochs.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Rendered surface activation views of event locked activity for main effect of Cue and contrast of Flip minus Hold Feedback events which was either exclusive to Hand or Eye response periods or overlapping between the two response modes (conjunction analysis). The exclusive masking approach was used to compare Eye and Hand activations. See Table 2 for activation coordinates, anatomical labels and Brodmann Area classification. Green circle shows dorsolateral frontal cortex locus used as the basis of the Psycho-physiological interaction analysis (PPI) and MVPA (BA 9/46). Blue circle indicates region activated by Cue events corresponding to the location of human frontal eye fields (FEF; BA6). Activations are shown thresholded at p < 0.05 FWE corrected.

Interactions Between Trial Type and Response Type

A key motivating question for the study was whether the location of regions showing an enhanced response to Flip relative to Hold rule Feedback events varied dependent upon response type. Of particular interest is whether the increase in activity observed in some areas during Flip relative to Hold Feedback events was modulated by motor response (even though no motor response was actually executed contingent upon this signal to implement a new rule on the next trial). Activations of this type would be indicative of rule updating and cognitive control processes which were specific to particular modalities of response.

No voxels were found whose activity depended upon the interaction effect between Response and Trial type, even when an inclusive mask corresponding to the main effect of Flip minus Hold rule Feedbacks and an appropriate SVC was applied. Using the exclusive masking approach, in which the contrast of Flip minus Hold trial feedback was exclusively masked with regions showing Eye/Hand response (i.e., Cue event) locked activity, a number of regions were found to be significantly activated suggesting that these response execution related regions were also activate during rule updating (Figure 2; Table 3A). Previous research had identified areas of the ventrolateral frontal cortex as showing response specific activity during cognitive tasks including stop signal and anti saccades/anti pointing tasks. In order to test whether a similar organization could be observed during rule switching, an ROI encompassing the bilateral ventrolateral frontal cortex (combination of BA45, BA47, BA44 as defined within the WFU Pickatlas toolbox) and associated SVC was applied to the data. This revealed that Hand response epochs were associated with a more ventral and posterior locus of activity, whereas for the reverse comparison of Eye epochs exclusively masked with Hand epochs, a more dorsal and anterior locus confined to the left hemisphere was highlighted (Table 3B; Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Sub regions of the ventrolateral frontal cortex which were found to show activity which varied with response modality during processing of Flip rule feedback events.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. (A) Coordinates of peak activations for Flip vs. Hold rule Feedbacks within regions which showed significant Cue response related activity specific to either Eye or Hand epochs (p < 0.05, whole brain FWE corrected). (B) Coordinates of activation within the ventrolateral frontal cortex which showed response mode exclusive activity during processing of rule change feedback events. (C) Psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analysis for lateral frontal cortex seed region connectivity with posterior cortical areas during processing of rule change feedback events in Hand versus Eye blocks.

Conjunction Analysis (Common Activations Between Eye and Hand Epochs)

Areas which showed completely overlapping event elicited activity during the two response modes (Eye/Hand) were examined for each relevant event type contrast using the conjunction null hypothesis test option in SPM8.

This analysis for Cue locked activity alone revealed predominantly left superior parietal cortex activity, bilateral cerebellar and dorsolateral frontal (BA6) activity in the region of the FEF (Mort et al., 2003; Figure 2).

The conjunction analysis for the contrast between Flip vs. Hold Feedback events (i.e., rule updating vs. non-rule updating events) for Hand and Eye epochs showed activity in the medial frontal cortex including activation peaks within the dorsal anterior cingulate gyrus, superior frontal gyrus, left inferior frontal cortex/frontal operculum (BA47), bilateral inferior parietal cortex and left dorsolateral frontal cortex (BA9/BA46; Figure 2).

Psycho-Physiological Interaction Analysis

In order to assess the hypothesised biasing influence of prefrontal cortical areas on other brain regions during updating of rules and how this might vary with response modality, a PPI analysis was carried out (Friston et al., 1997). Two analyses were carried out for each participant using spherical VOIs as seed regions (radius 15 mm) centered around the peak activation coordinates within the left dorsolateral frontal cortex (BA9: −46, 34, 26 MNI space) and dorsal anterior cingulate gyrus (BA32: −4, 20, 46) as highlighted by the conjunction analysis above (Figure 2). An ROI and corresponding SVC, comprising the entire parietal and occipital lobes were applied to the results combined with a FWE correction for multiple comparisons.

This analysis revealed a large cluster of voxels in the right posterior parietal cortex which showed enhanced connectivity with the lateral frontal seed region during Hand compared to Eye epochs. For the reverse comparison, a cluster of voxels in the lateral occipital cortex showed significantly enhanced covariance of activity during eye relative to hand epochs (Figure 4A; Table 3C). No significantly activated voxels were apparent from the equivalent analysis using the anterior cingulate gyrus as the seed region.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A) Sectional view of activity in dorsolateral frontal cortex ROI during Flip rule feedback events (left panel) used as a seed region in a PPI analysis, alongside rendered view of posterior cortical areas showing enhanced covariance with the seed region in Hand (middle) or Eye (right) response epochs. (B) Examples of confusion matrices and classifier function plots illustrating typical discrimination performance of a voxel pattern analysis classifier discriminating between Flip feedback event related activity in either Eye or Hand response epochs in the same left dorsolateral frontal ROI.

Multi-Voxel Pattern Analysis

Finally, we applied pattern analysis/classifier algorithms to the data set to assess whether the overall pattern of activity across voxels was different for feedback events which occurred during Eye or Hand epochs (even though the occurrence of a Feedback event was not a direct signal for execution of a motor response). In particular, we wanted to apply this technique to regions which the univariate analysis indicated were supra-modal in nature (such that no voxels showed significant variation in activity dependent upon response epoch) but which nevertheless showed evidence for changes in connectivity with posterior regions during rule updating.

A binary support vector machine classifier was applied to Flip feedback event related activity in voxels extracted from the same left dorsolateral frontal ROI used as a seed region in the PPI analysis (see above). Using this approach it was found that the machine classifier was able to correctly identify whether activity evoked by Feedback events occurred in Hand or Eye epochs. This was found to be the case for all participants tested, with an overall mean accuracy across subjects of 85% (ranging from 75–98 between participants) for Hand and 84% (62–98) for Eye response epochs (Figure 4B).

Discussion

A key motivation of this study was to examine whether regions of the frontal cerebral cortex previously shown to be active in a SR rule switching task might show regional variation in activation patterns dependent upon effector modality (saccades vs. manual button pressing). As would be expected, activity dependent upon the type of response (Eye/Hand) to be executed time-locked to response Cue events was found within the inferior parietal cortex, lateral occipital cortex, post-central gyrus and FEF (Table 2). Interestingly however, some of these eye/hand dependent regions were also active during processing of rule Feedback events, for which a motor response was not required but cognitive updating of SR rules was (Table 3). Consistent with earlier fMRI studies, eye/hand specific sub regions were apparent within the ventrolateral frontal cortex (Table 3B). Further analysis also suggested that even apparently supra-modal regions within the lateral frontal cortex changed their pattern of connectivity with posterior areas during rule updating (PPI analysis) and showed variation in the overall pattern of voxel activity dependent upon response modality (PRoNTo/MVPA analysis).

Our results support previous findings which suggest that sub-regions of the ventrolateral frontal cortex support cognitive operations for particular motor domains specifically the inferior frontal gyrus pars triangularis and orbitalis. A more dorsal and anterior locus of activity was found for saccade responses, whilst manual response trials recruited ventral and posterior parts of the inferior frontal gyrus bilaterally. The inferior frontal cortex has been suggested as an important substrate of cognitive inhibitory control across different modalities (Aron et al., 2014) as well as forming part of a general multiple cognitive demands fronto-parietal network (Duncan and Owen, 2000). In a previous fMRI study using a manual response version of the rule switching task we found the region to be active even in a control task where “flip” feedbacks did not require a rule reversal (Parris et al., 2007). From this it was concluded that the role of this region was related to processing and evaluating salient stimulus events rather than updating SR rules or implementing cognitive inhibitory control (see also Aron et al., 2014; Erika-Florence et al., 2014). However, the current results suggest that this region does have an underlying motoric organization and function.

The conjunction analysis highlighted the dorsolateral region of the frontal cortex as being equally activated during rule updating for both eye and hand versions of the task. A suggested function of the dorsolateral frontal cortex is that it applies appropriate biasing input onto posterior structures to ensure that behavior adaptively conforms to current goals (Desimone and Duncan, 1995; MacDonald et al., 2000; Miller and Cohen, 2001). To assess evidence for such changes in connectivity between epochs a PPI analysis was carried out. This revealed clusters of voxels within the occipital and parietal cortex whose activity covariance with the dorsolateral frontal cortex was significantly modulated by response type (Table 3; Figure 4). This result prompts the question as to how truly supra-modal neural representations could influence posterior cortical areas in a modality specific manner without modifying their pattern of anterior to posterior synaptic connections over a very short time scale (Nachev et al., 2008).

One way of answering this question is offered by recent work which has suggested that standard univariate fMRI analysis lacks the resolution required to detect neuronal organization by rule SR associations, whereas multivariate techniques such as MVPA are better able to discriminate between different task rule related activity (e.g., Woolgar et al., 2011). Consistent with this, an MVPA analysis within the same left lateral frontal ROI used in the PPI analysis was able to discriminate between Hand and Eye response blocks with a high degree of accuracy. This is consistent with the existence of distributed multi modal representations of motor response type even within regions which at the voxel cluster level activate equally during Eye and Hand movements. It should be noted however that other authors have argued that the MVPA comparisons across task conditions may be susceptible to unknown confounds which vary across participants, such as task difficulty and response times (Todd et al., 2013). Although we believe the overall level of task/rule complexity is closely matched between the two conditions we acknowledge that MVPA discrimination based on factors other than response modality related tuning cannot be entirely excluded.

The conjunction analysis for Cue locked activity revealed a number of areas which activated equally during response programming and execution irrespective of the modality of the response to be executed. These included areas of the bilateral superior parietal cortex, the ventrolateral occipital cortex and lateral cerebellum (Figure 2). It is likely that these regions represent aspects of responding to the Cue stimulus which did not relate directly to the execution of a response. For example, processing the color of the cue and selection of the associated spatial concept i.e., left or right, independent of response mode. It is interesting that activity within the region assumed to be the location of the FEF (Mort et al., 2003) was observed in both hand and eye epochs (Figure 2). None of our participants made any errors in which the wrong type (rather than the wrong direction) of response was executed (e.g., an overt saccade rather than a button press in manual response epochs). Other neuroimaging studies which have studied cognitive task execution with hand and eye movements have also reported FEF activity during manual as well as saccade response blocks (Connolly et al., 2000). One interpretation of this finding is that oculomotor centers play a role in orienting of spatial attention and manual response programming and that the oculomotor system is active during orienting of attention irrespective of the overt response mode required by the task (Rizzolatti et al., 1994).

Summary and Conclusions

During a SR rule switching task the lateral frontal cortex was found to show regional variation in activity dependent upon the type of motor response to be executed (Hand or Eye), even during events that signaled a change in rule rather than being associated with an actual motor response to be executed. Closer examination of fMRI data shows that even within frontal areas which univariate analysis suggested were equally activated during rule switching with manual or saccade responses, differences were apparent between Hand and Eye response epochs in terms of their connectivity with posterior areas as well as the overall pattern of voxels activated within the region. We suggest that higher order representations of task rules are underpinned by neural representations which represent effector modality. Such a distributed form of representation is best termed multi modal rather than supra-modal and may not always be apparent when standard univariate statistical analysis techniques are applied to fMRI data.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by Wellcome Trust Project Grant Number: 072082/Z/03/Z and Wellcome Trust Flexible Travel Award (Sabbatical) Grant Number 086013AIA.

References

Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., and Poldrack, R. A. (2014). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 177–185. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asaad, W. F., Rainer, G., and Miller, E. K. (1998). Neural activity in the primate prefrontal cortex during associative learning. Neuron 21, 1399–1407. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80658-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boettinger, C. A., and D’Esposito, M. T. (2005). Frontal networks for learning and executing arbitrary stimulus-response associations. J. Neurosci. 25, 2723–2732. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3697-04.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chouinard, P. A., and Goodale, M. A. (2009). FMRI adaptation during performance of learned arbitrary visuomotor conditional associations. Neuroimage 48, 696–706. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Connolly, J. D., Goodale, M. A., DeSouza, J. F. X., Menon, R. S., and Vilis, T. (2000). A comparison of frontoparietal fMRI activation during anti-saccades and anti-pointing. J. Neurophysiol. 84, 1645–1655.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Cristianini, N., and Shawe-Taylor, J. (2000). An Introduction to Support Vector Machines: And Other Kernel-Based Learning Methods. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Deacon, T. (1997). The Symbolic Species. London: Penguin.

Google Scholar

Desimone, R., and Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual-attention. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 193–222. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.18.1.193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, J., and Owen, A. M. (2000). Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci. 23, 475–483. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01633-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erika-Florence, M., Leech, R., and Hampshire, A. (2014). A functional network perspective on response inhibition and attentional control. Nat. Commun. 5:4073. doi: 10.1038/ncomms5073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J., Buechel, C., Fink, G. R., Morris, J., Rolls, E., and Dolan, R. J. (1997). Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. Neuroimage 6, 218–229. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J., Penny, W. D., and Glaser, D. E. (2005). Conjunction revisited. Neuroimage 25, 661–667. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gehring, W. J., and Knight, R. T. (2000). Prefrontal-cingulate interactions in action monitoring. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 516–520. doi: 10.1038/74899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampshire, A., and Owen, A. M. (2006). Fractionating attentional control using event-related fMRI. Cereb. Cortex 16, 1679–1689. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodgson, T., Chamberlain, M., Parris, B., James, M., Gutowski, N., Husain, M., et al. (2007). The role of the ventrolateral frontal cortex in inhibitory oculomotor control. Brain 130, 1525–1537. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodgson, T. L., Sumner, P., Molyva, M., Sheridan, R., and Kennard, C. (2013). Learning and switching between stimulus-saccade associations in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 51, 1350–1360. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.03.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huddy, V. C., Hodgson, T. L., Ron, M. A., Barnes, T. R. E., and Joyce, E. M. (2011). Abnormal negative feedback processing in first episode schizophrenia: evidence from an oculomotor rule switching task. Psychol. Med. 41, 1805–1814. doi: 10.1017/s0033291710002527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Husain, M., Parton, A., Hodgson, T., Mort, D., and Rees, G. (2003). Self-control during response conflict by human supplementary eye field. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 117–118. doi: 10.1038/nn1005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., and Ganis, G. (2006). The Case for Mental Imagery. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Lancaster, J. L., Woldorff, M. G., Parsons, L. M., Liotti, M., Freitas, C. S., Rainey, L., et al. (2000). Automated talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Hum. Brain Mapp. 10, 120–131. doi: 10.1002/1097-0193(200007)10:3<120::aid-hbm30>3.0.co;2-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leung, H. C., and Cai, W. D. (2007). Common and differential ventrolateral prefrontal activity during inhibition of hand and eye movements. J. Neurosci. 27, 9893–9900. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2837-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDonald, A. W., Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A., and Carter, C. S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science 288, 1835–1838. doi: 10.1126/science.288.5472.1835

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maldjian, J. A., Laurienti, P. J., Kraft, R. A., and Burdette, J. H. (2003). An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. Neuroimage 19, 1233–1239. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00169-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, E. K., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milner, B. (1963). Effects of different brain lesions on card sorting—role of frontal lobes. Arch. Neurol. 9, 90–100. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1963.00460070100010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mort, D. J., Perry, R. J., Mannan, S. K., Hodgson, T. L., Anderson, E., Quest, R., et al. (2003). Differential cortical activation during voluntary and reflexive saccades in man. Neuroimage 18, 231–246. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)00028-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nachev, P., Kennard, C., and Husain, M. (2008). Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 856–869. doi: 10.1038/nrn2478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nichols, T. E., Brett, M., Andersson, J., Wager, T., and Poline, J. B. (2005). Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic. Neuroimage 25, 653–660. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parris, B. A., Thai, N. J., Benattayallah, A., Summers, I. R., and Hodgson, T. L. (2007). The role of the lateral prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate in stimulus-response association reversals. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 13–24. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parton, A., Nachev, P., Hodgson, T. L., Mort, D., Thomas, D., Ordidge, R., et al. (2007). Role of the human supplementary eye field in the control of saccadic eye movements. Neuropsychologia 45, 997–1008. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Passingham, R. E., Toni, I., and Rushworth, M. F. S. (2000). Specialisation within the prefrontal cortex: the ventral prefrontal cortex and associative learning. Exp. Brain Res. 133, 103–113. doi: 10.1007/s002210000405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, M. (2008). “Selecting between competing responses based on conditional rules,” in Neuroscience of Rule-Guided Behaviour, eds S. A. Bunge and J. D. Wallis (New York: Oxford University Press), 3–21.

Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., and Sheliga, B. M. (1994). “Space and selective attention,” in Attention and Performance XV, eds C. Umiltá and M. Moscovitch (Cambridge, MA: MIT press), 231–265.

Google Scholar

Roberts, K. L., and Hall, D. A. (2008). Examining a supramodal network for conflict processing: a systematic review and novel functional magnetic resonance imaging data for related visual and auditory stroop tasks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 1063–1078. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schrouff, J., Rosa, M. J., Rondina, J. M., Marquand, A. F., Chu, C., Ashburner, J., et al. (2013). PRoNTo: pattern recognition for neuroimaging toolbox. Neuroinformatics 11, 319–337. doi: 10.1007/s12021-013-9178-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swainson, R., Cunnington, R., Jackson, G. M., Rorden, C., Morris, P. G., and Jackson, S. R. (2003). Cognitive control mechanisms revealed by ERP and fMRI: evidence from repeated task-switching. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 785–799. doi: 10.1162/089892903322370717

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todd, M. T., Nystrom, L. E., and Cohen, J. D. (2013). Confounds in multivariate pattern analysis: theory and rule representation case study. Neuroimage 77, 157–165. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vilberg, K. L., and Rugg, D. (2008). Memory retrieval and the parietal cortex: a review of evidence from a dual-process perspective. Neuropsychologia 46, 1787–1799. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallis, J. D. (2008). “Single neuron activity underlying behaviour-guiding rules,” in Neuroscience of Rule-Guided Behaviour, eds S. A. Bunge and J. D. Wallis (New York: Oxford University Press), 23–45.

White, I. M., and Wise, S. P. (1999). Rule-dependent neuronal activity in the prefrontal cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 126, 315–335. doi: 10.1007/s002210050740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woolgar, A., Thompson, R., Bor, D., and Duncan, J. (2011). Multi-voxel coding of stimuli, rules and responses in human frontoparietal cortex. Neuroimage 56, 744–752. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive, executive, fMRI, manual, prefrontal, saccades

Citation: Hodgson TL, Parris BA, Benattayallah A and Summers IR (2015) Multi-modal representation of effector modality in frontal cortex during rule switching. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:486. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00486

Received: 29 May 2014; Accepted: 21 August 2015;
Published: 22 September 2015.

Edited by:

Srikantan S. Nagarajan, University of California, San Francisco, USA

Reviewed by:

Leighton B. Hinkley, University of California, San Francisco, USA
Laura Anne Libby, University of California, Davis, USA

Copyright © 2015 Hodgson, Parris, Benattayallah and Summers. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Timothy L. Hodgson, School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Lincoln LN6 7TS, UK, tlhodgson@lincoln.ac.uk

Download