Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 07 March 2019
Sec. Eating Behavior

Food-Pics_Extended—An Image Database for Experimental Research on Eating and Appetite: Additional Images, Normative Ratings and an Updated Review

  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria
  • 2Centre for Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria
  • 3Department of Psychology and Education, Free University of Berlin, Berlin, Germany
  • 4Institute of Psychology, University of Münster, Münster, Germany
  • 5Research Center Jülich, Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-3), Cognitive Neuroscience, Jülich, Germany

Our current environment is characterized by the omnipresence of food cues. The taste and smell of real foods—but also graphical depictions of appetizing foods—can guide our eating behavior, for example, by eliciting food craving and anticipatory cephalic phase responses. To facilitate research into this so-called cue reactivity, several groups have compiled standardized food image sets. Yet, selecting the best subset of images for a specific research question can be difficult as images and image sets vary along several dimensions. In the present report, we review the strengths and weaknesses of popular food image sets to guide researchers during stimulus selection. Furthermore, we present a recent extension of our previously published database food-pics, which comprises an additional 328 food images from different countries to increase cross-cultural applicability. This food-pics_extended stimulus database, thus, encompasses and replaces food-pics. Normative data from a predominantly German-speaking sample are again presented as well as updated calculations of image characteristics.

Introduction

Our current environment is characterized by frequent cues of highly palatable foods. Many researchers partially attribute rising obesity rates and problems in eating-related self-regulation to this factor (e.g., Davis et al., 2011). Today’s foods—processed as well as unprocessed—have reached a level of refinement that appeals strongly to our senses: visual, gustatory, olfactory, and oro-sensory food properties interact in creating hedonic pleasure. Pervasive advertisement penetrates real and virtual lives and constantly taxes self-regulation.

Research uses food images as experimental stimuli in a range of different paradigms. The food-viewing paradigm attempts to simulate environmental conditions in a controlled laboratory environment. Passive picture viewing is seen as a preparatory or anticipatory stage in food intake: natural eating settings often start with exposure to a food’s visual appearance along with its smell. Such preparatory stages are of interest to research as anticipatory cephalic phase responses might underlie conditioned food cravings (Berthoud and Morrison, 2008; Dagher, 2012). Passive picture viewing is not the only way in which food images are used: Pavlovian or operant conditioning setups pair foods with neutral images and thereby tap into learning (e.g., Blechert et al., 2016;Wardle et al., 2018), memory setups tap into retention (Meule et al., 2012), and lateral or non-foveal presentations investigate spatial attention (Castellanos et al., 2009). Research has repeatedly demonstrated that food images capture attention (Nummenmaa et al., 2011; Cunningham and Egeth, 2018), are prioritized during neural processing (Toepel et al., 2009; Meule et al., 2013), and consistently activate brain areas associated with reward, salience, and cognitive control (Dagher, 2012; Tang et al., 2012; Spence et al., 2016). These reward-related neural responses can be enhanced by both the presentation of energy-dense food (Killgore et al., 2003; Schur et al., 2009) and by manipulations of hunger (Uher et al., 2006; Fuhrer et al., 2008; Siep et al., 2009) or cravings (Pelchat et al., 2004). Furthermore, individuals with obesity (compared to healthy weight controls) show increased activation in reward-related brain regions induced by particularly energy-dense cues (Pursey et al., 2014). In their meta-analysis, Boswell and Kober (2016) showed that food cue reactivity and craving predicted eating and weight gain, and that the effect sizes of this prediction were similar for visual food cues and real food exposure (and stronger than those predicted by olfactory cues). This is an impressive demonstration of the power of visual food cues on appetitive responding and health. More recently, research has used food images to change associated evaluations and response tendencies, such as in motor response inhibition trainings (Stice et al., 2016; Jones et al., 2018).

The predominance of picture viewing in experimental research has brought about the need for adequate stimulus material. While earlier research had used food images from cookbooks, unspecified internet searches, or other databases such as the International Affective Picture System (IAPS), it soon became clear that these images yielded both insufficient images quality (e.g., poor resolution or contrast) and limited variance (e.g., in food categories, portion sizes, or viewing angles). As a result, considerable effort has been invested in the development of standardized, high quality, and open source materials. Several sets of pictures have been published recently, providing researchers with more options. Based on the IAPS, Miccoli et al. (2014) published the open library of affective foods (OLAF) with a particular focus of naturalistic settings. The macronutrient picture system (MAPS) is a relatively small set but provides detailed macronutrient composition for each food image (King et al., 2018). Larger image sets were presented by Foroni et al. (2013) [FoodCast Research Image Database (FRIDa)] and Charbonnier et al., 2016 [Food4Health (F4H); 2016], along with ratings from larger samples on various subjective properties such as energy density. Finally, food-pics (Blechert et al., 2014) was introduced by our group and includes a large number of images along with normative ratings and computational measures of image characteristics. However, requests from food-pics users to include further items motivated the search for additional images. For example, several food items popular in France, the United Kingdom, Austria, Germany, the Middle East, and Asia, a wider range of baked goods (e.g., different kinds of dark bread), a wider range of portion sizes for fruits and vegetables (including single foods and sliced fruits), as well as drinks were added. Also, improvements to the indices of image characteristics were made.

Existing image sets vary on several dimensions that might be of relevance to researchers looking for experimental stimuli. Hence, in addition to describing the extension of the food-pics database (aim i), the present report reviews other food image datasets popular in experimental research (aim ii), and assesses their strengths and weaknesses in order to guide researchers during the selection of the optimal database (aim iii). Toward this end, we pay particular attention to properties such as database size and intercultural applicability, the existence of normative data and computational measures of image characteristics, and the range and coverage of various food types and settings (single foods, solid foods vs. drinks, main meals vs. snacks, naturalistic vs. highly controlled settings). We review image sets that were freely available and established for the purpose of experimental picture-viewing paradigms in humans. Image sets established for the development and training of automatic recognition algorithms [e.g., Pittsburgh Fast-Food Image Dataset, Chen et al., 2009; University of Catania (UNICT) Food Dataset 889, Farinella et al., 2015; the ChineseFoodNet, Chen et al., 2017] are not reviewed, as they serve a different purpose. More generally, the present report aims to facilitate comparability and replicability of food-related research on the level of experimental stimuli.

Materials and Methods

Food-Pics_Extended

Stimuli

The extended food-pics database added 328 food images to the original 568 images (for details see Blechert et al., 2014). Images were provided by several researchers using food-pics1. Categories of foods include sweet (e.g., banana split), savory (e.g., ravioli), processed (e.g., fried chicken), and whole (e.g., orange) foods as well as beverages (e.g., milk). Several single images of individually presented foods were added to allow for a relatively precise estimation of nutritional composition and calorie content compared to foods that consist of several components. As in the original dataset, images comprised both single items (e.g., 1 blackberry) and numerous items (e.g., 11 blackberries) as well as meals (e.g., salmon and spinach). The same non-food items as previously described by Blechert et al. (2014) were included in the extended database for obtaining comparable normative ratings. For standardization, all images were edited onto a white background and homogenized according to viewing distance (≈80 cm), angle, and simple figure-ground composition. Plates and bowls were shown when necessary (e.g., ice cream sundae), though most foods could be presented without (e.g., fruits).

Image Characteristics

Image properties characterizing the images’ physical properties were computed using customized MATLAB scripts (The Mathworks, Inc. Natick, United States), which can be downloaded from the food-pics website2. A full description of image characteristic analysis is provided in the original report (Blechert et al., 2014). In brief, size was quantified as the proportion of non-white pixels. Color properties were quantified as the contribution of red, green, and blue color channels to the non-white pixels. Within-object contrast was quantified as the standard deviation of luminance values across non-white pixels. To describe how much an object stands out from the white background, we quantified its intensity as the mean of the pixel-wise luminance difference to the white background. Note that this property was previously referred to as “brightness,” but was renamed to intensity (i.e., inversed brightness). As intensity depends on both the luminance and number of non-white pixels (i.e., object size), we also provide a normalized intensity measure that is size-independent. To describe the spatial variations of luminance, we calculated the spatial frequency content with a bi-dimensional fast Fourier transform and a subsequent radial average of the two-dimensional power spectra. Thus, the median power quantifies variations in pixel luminance at different spatial scales, independent of their location in the image. In addition, complexity of an image was defined by the number or proportion (normalized complexity) of pixels representing contour outlines, as determined by a Canny edge detection algorithm (Canny, 1986) with adjusted parameters.

Macronutrients

Caloric information was estimated by students of nutritional science using the database https://fddb.info. Each food was given a kcal/100 g and total kcal value for the depicted portion. Ratings were pooled across two to five raters.

Normative Ratings

Participants

Participants (n = 245) completed an anonymous online survey to provide normative data for the additional food-pics images (21.2% male, mean age = 31.4 years, 87.3% German; see Table 1 for detailed participant demographics). Participants were recruited through different university mailing lists; thus, the sample comprised students and employees alike. Participants who rated less than three food images were excluded from the analyses. The survey was available between December 2016 and February 2017. Participants were offered participation in a raffle for 5 × 30 Euros. The ethics board of the University of Salzburg approved this study.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Subject demographics.

Online Survey

Participants provided demographic information on their age, gender, height, weight, occupation, and nationality as well as on eating habits (omnivore/vegetarian/vegan, dieting, or not dieting; see Table 1) before they rated the pictures. Each participant viewed and rated a random selection of 40 foods out of the 328 new food images, as participants could not have reliably rated all 328 images. Participants further rated five food images from the old food-pics database and a random selection of eight non-food images out of all 315 non-foods to check for comparability of the old and the new rating sample. Participants were given a detailed explanation of each of the scales and shown an example rating for all scales. Familiarity (German: “Bekanntheit”) was defined as whether the participant recognized the object or not. Recognizability (German: “Erkennbarkeit”) was defined as whether the object was easy or difficult to identify. Complexity (German: “Komplexität”) was characterized by “many components or details,” and “many colors/edges/pieces.” Valence (German: “Valenz”) was characterized by how negatively or positively the participant viewed the object; that is, whether they found it was repulsive or attractive. Arousal (German: “Erregung”) was characterized by how much the object aroused an emotional reaction in the participant. Palatability (German: “Schmackhaftigkeit”) was characterized by how delicious the participant found the depicted food in general, regardless of whether they wanted to eat it in the moment or not. Desire to eat (German: “Verlangen”) was characterized by how much the participant would like to eat the depicted food if it were available at that moment. Each image was displayed individually and participants were asked to rate each aspect of the depicted food. Response options for familiarity and recognizability were dichotomous (yes/no) and visual analog scales (VAS; solid horizontal bars approximately 8 cm long) with anchors on either extreme were used for ratings of complexity (“very little” to “very high”), valence (“very negative” to “very positive”), arousal (“not at all” to “extremely”), palatability (“not at all” to “extremely”), and desire to eat (“not at all” to “extremely”). Responses were provided via mouse click and ranged from 0 (leftmost extreme) to 100 (rightmost extreme); the value was not shown to participants.

Results

Normative Ratings

Each food image was rated by 14 to 47 participants (M = 28.21 images, SD = 5.26).

Interrater Reliability

Intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC) were then calculated using SPSS statistics (Version 24; IBM Corp.) based on a mean rating (k = 2), consistency, two-way random-effects model to compare normative ratings from the food-pics sample with normative ratings of the food-pics_extended sample. For food images, reliability was good for recognizability, familiarity, complexity, palatability, valence, and arousal (ICC = 0.870, 0.810, 0.801, 0.772, 0.834, and 0.820, respectively), and moderate for craving (ICC = 0.658). For non-food images, reliability was good for recognizability, familiarity, complexity, and arousal (ICC = 0.815, 0.850, 0.791, and 0.756, respectively), and moderate for valence (ICC = 0.671).

Food-pics_extended replaces food-pics, and images and metadata are available at http://food-pics.sbg.ac.at. Users of both food-pics and food-pics_extended are asked to cite the present report describing food-pics_extended.

Overview Over Selected Food Image Databases

To guide researchers in selecting images according to their needs, we have compiled a table describing all of the above-mentioned datasets (see Table 2). Regarding set size, the following ordering emerged: food-pics_extended, food-pics, F4H, FRIDa, MaPS, OLAF, IAPS_foods. Normative ratings were available for all datasets, with the most ratings per image available for IAPS_foods, followed by F4H, food-pics, food-pics_extended, FRIDa/MaPS, and OLAF. Image characteristics were available for food-pics, food-pics_extended and FRIDa. Energy density is available for all datasets but OLAF and IAPS_foods.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Overview of stimulus sets.

Discussion

The present report presents the food-pics_extended image dataset, an addition to the food-pics stimulus set that added 328 images to the original 568 images (the extended set, thus, replaces food-pics and contains a total of 896 images). In the following, we describe food-pics_extended (aim i), characterize ours and each of the major food image sets with a focus on advantages and limitations (aim ii), and finally present a guideline for choosing between sets by ranking sets on various dimensions (aim iii).

Regarding aim i, food-pics_extended enlarges and complements the food-pics database (Blechert et al., 2014): besides the mere addition of images, we amended the normative data in a way that allowed compatibility with the normative data of food-pics. Our agreement/consistency data indicate that this process was successful: normative ratings by the new raters were largely comparable to those of food-pics as evidenced by good interrater agreement for a subset of images presented to both subject pools. This suggests that researchers can use images and normative data from both image sets and, thus, food-pics_extended subsumes and replaces food-pics, so users of “old” and “new” images should refer to food-pics_extended. Some caution should be given for craving ratings, for which agreement indices were lower, and which are known to be very state-dependent and fluctuate (Shiffman, 2000). Images in food-pics and food-pics_extended were selected under the following principles: (A) all foods were set on a white background, mostly without context (plates are shown where necessary), (B) high recognizability for most images (though for some foods in food-pics_extended, recognition might depend on cultural knowledge; Jensen et al., 2016), (C) high image quality and esthetic appeal. Single foods as well as full meals and different combinations of single foods are included. Normative ratings are available from several large samples (German-speaking and North American). Thus, researchers interested in investigating certain subpopulations (e.g., older US females), can extract the respective normative ratings from the database and use them to select images accordingly (e.g., on high vs. low palatability, given high recognizability). Food-pics_extended comes with 315 non-food control images that can be matched in terms of physical stimulus properties to the food images on ratings of valence and arousal as well as on image characteristics.

Regarding aim ii, in reviewing established image databases, it became clear that while IAPS (Lang et al., 2008) has been of undebated importance for standardizing stimuli across laboratories, it is very limited in the food context. Its focus lies on images that vary strongly in valence and arousal. Its advantages include the inclusion of a large database of valence and arousal ratings and its extensive use in the literature. Users aiming to include non-food IAPS images in their study should thus opt for these images or for OLAF for reasons of comparability of the normative ratings. Yet, these advantages are offset by several shortcomings: food images are few in number (48) and images are embedded in varying and complex backgrounds that might influence the neural response as a result of their overall image complexity. Furthermore, rating data do not include important information such as palatability ratings or data on calorie density.

An approach similar to that of the IAPS was taken by the authors of the OLAF (Miccoli et al., 2014). Explicitly referring to the IAPS database, the authors provide 96 images that parallel the complex and contextualized character of the IAPS: images are taken “on eye level,” full meals are shown with an overrepresentation of high-energy and highly palatable foods, images are meant to particularly appeal to the observers’ affective response, and normative data are given in relation to other categories of the IAPS (negative, neutral, and positive IAPS). As a result of the naturalistic, contextualized setup, it is difficult to control aspects of the food (macronutrient content, energy density), its components (only parts of the foods visible), and constituents (several main and side dishes, gravy, toppings, etc.). Normative data (valence, arousal, dominance, craving) from a large group of Spanish children and adults are available, resulting in 18 ratings per image.

FRIDa from Foroni et al. (2013) was the largest set at the time of publication with 582 food images representing mostly Western foods, with a slight bias toward Mediterranean foods. It was the first set for which quantitative measures of image characteristics (i.e., size, mean brightness, and high spatial frequency power) were available. Such image characteristics are known to influence behavioral response times and performance (Felipe et al., 1993; Mace et al., 2005; VanRullen, 2006; O’Donell et al., 2010) as well as neurophysiological responses (Pourtois et al., 2005; Schadow et al., 2007; Kovalenko et al., 2012). Therefore, providing information about image characteristics is important because they represent a potential confound for the comparison between groups of images, such as high caloric vs. low caloric food. They were also the first to include spoiled or rotten foods, allowing interesting comparisons within the food category but with varying valence/edibility (Becker et al., 2016). Their inclusion of natural and artificial non-foods further allow for interesting food/non-food contrasts. Clear advantages are set size, comparison categories, and rating data on “degree of food transformation,” “distance from edibility,” and calories, which are not available in any other image set. Disadvantages include the strict omission of plates (even for soups), which created edge artifacts for some images, limitation of their normative data to relatively few ratings (5–14 ratings) per image from respondents predominantly from Italy.

The database F4H by Charbonnier et al. (2016) includes 370 images by the time of this writing. It was the first image set to publish a standardized image protocol that would allow the community to extend the image set with comparable parameters. It focuses on individual foods (mostly between one to ∼30 pieces of one food on a plate) and on standardized presentation. This allowed the authors to provide exact estimates of calorie density along with the subjective ratings of participants. This standardized character, however, decreases the esthetic appeal and decontextualizes foods, which are often consumed in meals and compositions. F4H also includes 41 non-food images without any ratings. Food images represent foods from different Western countries. Strengths also include normative data from children and adults from seven European countries on healthiness, calories, and similarity of images with real food. The high level of control over food content allows for precise calculations of macronutrients for studies focusing on this aspect (however, no such data other than subjective calorie content are included). Limitations include the aforementioned de-contextualization, lack of image characteristics, a relatively small set of unrated non-food images, and the focus on European foods and European normative data.

Macronutrient picture system (King et al., 2018) is a rather small image set (144 images) with a specialized purpose: neurocognitive research on the neural representations of different macronutrients. Thus, foods are relatively homogenous (but extreme) with regard to fat, sugar, and protein content. Advantages include the presentation of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data that show neural activation patterns for foods varying in macronutrient composition (sugar, fat, protein) and correspondence of image content with items in a food preference questionnaire (Geiselman et al., 1998) allowing the parallel investigation of habitual food consumption and neural correlates.

Regarding aim iii and a guideline for choosing between sets, our review illustrates that each of the presented databases has advantages but also limitations. Thus, each ranking of image sets has to been done in the light of the specific research question. One important attribute of any database is the number and variety of available images, because this affects many different research questions. For various reasons, such as variety of diets and culture, it seems important to not constrain image choice within a given set. Small sets run the risk of omitting typical and frequently consumed foods in a given geographical area (e.g., dark bread in Central Europe, rice dishes in Asia) or retraining variability within a given food category (e.g., salty snacks). Researchers interested in a large number of foods and/or different cultures may decide for one of the larger sets, such as food-pics_extended, F4H or FRIDa. A variety of items allows one not only to tap into a wide range of foods and potentially a wide range of cultures but also to match image subsets on other aspects. For instance, one may be interested in calorie density as an independent variable, but want to match stimulus groups on image characteristics (e.g., colors) and degree of processing, while keeping palatability comparable. This would require complex matching operations as these variables are sometimes correlated (Foroni et al., 2013; Blechert et al., 2014).

Almost equally important for a range of research questions is the amount of normative data provided. It requires extensive normative data to ensure reliable palatability matching from a population resembling the intended study sample. Accordingly, the size of image database is also related to another relevant choice dimension, namely, cross-cultural validity/applicability and availability of normative data. With regard to size and cross-cultural applicability, F4H and food-pics_extended would be the ideal sets, while researchers with a focus on Mediterranean diets and samples may also use FRIDa and OLAF. In the realm of neuroimaging, and particularly in electroencephalography (EEG) or magnetoencephalography (MEG) research and reaction time-based studies, researchers may consider recognizability and physical image characteristics such as complexity, brightness, and other attributes that might affect brain responses. For those, the choice might be between FRIDa and food-pics_extended. Regarding research focused on macronutrient content, MAPS and food-pics_extended would be recommendable. Drinks or food packaging are only available in food-pics_extended and food-pics. Researchers aiming to extend the data bases with images from their own labs may opt for open-ended stimulus sets such as F4H.

Certain limitations need to be kept in mind. First, our review was selective and might have overlooked some image sets. However, we aimed to review the most popular, free databases focusing on human appetite studies. Second, regarding food-pics_extended, even though we intended to include Middle Eastern and Asian foods, there is still ways to go to include typical foods from all major areas of the world. Also, drinks are underrepresented but may be important. Macronutrients are available for a subset of food-pics_extended (images of the former food-pics). In fact, pointing to the usefulness of such information for all images, recent research shows that both high fat and high carbohydrate content is more reinforcing than equicaloric foods with either high fat or high carbohydrate content (Difeliceantonio et al., 2018). Adaptations for different age groups may also be worthwhile. For example, a subset of food-pics_extended (images of the former food-pics) has been examined in US adolescents aged 12–17, where an average of 75% of foods were recognized. There are no data yet available for younger participants. This indicates that while certainly enough images are available with good recognizability, there is still room for improvement for some foods. Cultural differences were also documented for food-pics ratings in Portugal (Prada et al., 2017), pointing to the need for further validation. Due to elevated public awareness of the issue of nutritional health, normative ratings may have to be updated periodically. For example, more recent samples gave higher valence ratings for low calorie foods than the original food-pics sample, tentatively pointing in that direction (although confounded with cultural differences, see Prada et al., 2017). Future research might extend normative data, image breadth, and include 3D images for virtual reality and more high-resolution images in various formats. Importantly, the normative ratings for food-pics_extended were obtained from a relatively homogenous sample of predominantly female, German-speaking, and educated individuals in their 30 s. A representative database would require inclusion of other age groups (particularly younger aged youth and children), less educated groups, more males, and importantly, participants from other geographical regions.

Data Availability

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: http://food-pics.sbg.ac.at.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the ethics board of the University of Salzburg with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the ethics board of the University of Salzburg.

Author Contributions

JB wrote the introduction, parts of the methods, and discussion. AL and SP compiled the images and the data base. KO and SP conducted the questionnaire study and ran the statistical analysis. NAB optimized the image characteristic scripts. All authors edited and approved the last version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation Program (ERC-StG-2014 639445 NewEat).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Sarah Schmid for her work on the calorie data in the database and for contributions to the manuscript.

Footnotes

  1. ^ Many thanks to our food-pics_extended contributors:
    Loïc P. Heurley, Université Paris Ouest Nanterre La Défense, Nanterre, France
    Jang-Han Lee, Chung-Ang University, Seoul, Korea; Gal Sheppes, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
    Loukia Tzavella, Cardiff University, Wales, United Kingdom; Olga Pollatos, Universität Ulm, Ulm, Germany
    Vaibhav Tyagi, Plymouth University, Plymouth, United Kingdom
  2. ^ http://food-pics.sbg.ac.at

References

Becker, C. A., Flaisch, T., Renner, B., and Schupp, H. T. (2016). Neural correlates of the perception of spoiled food stimuli. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:302. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berthoud, H. R., and Morrison, C. (2008). The brain, appetite, and obesity. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 55–92. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093551

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blechert, J., Meule, A., Busch, N. A., and Ohla, K. (2014). Food-pics: an image database for experimental research on eating and appetite. Front. Psychol. 5:617. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blechert, J., Testa, G., Georgii, C., Klimesch, W., and Wilhelm, F. H. (2016). The Pavlovian craver: neural and experiential correlates of single trial naturalistic food conditioning in humans. Physiol. Behav. 58, 18–25. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.02.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boswell, R. G., and Kober, H. (2016). Food cue reactivity and craving predict eating and weight gain: a meta-analytic review. Obes. Rev. 17, 159–177. doi: 10.1111/obr.12354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canny, J. (1986). A computational approach to edge detection. IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. 8, 679–698. doi: 10.1109/TPAMI.1986.4767851

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellanos, E. H., Charboneau, E., Dietrich, M. S., Park, S., Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., et al. (2009). Obese adults have visual attention bias for food cue images: evidence for altered reward system function. Int. J. Obesity 33, 1063–1073. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2009.138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charbonnier, L., Van Meer, F., Van Der Laan, L. N., Viergever, M. A., and Smeets, P. A. M. (2016). Standardized food images: a photographing protocol and image database. Appetite 96, 166–173. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.08.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, M., Dhingra, K., Wu, W., Yang, L., Sukthankar, R., and Yang, J. (2009). “PFID: Pittsburgh fast-food image dataset,” in Proceedings for the 2009 16th IEEE International Conference on Image Processing (ICIP), (Piscataway, NJ: IEEE), 289–292. doi: 10.1109/ICIP.2009.5413511

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, X., Zhou, H., and Diao, L. (2017). ChineseFoodNet: a large-scale image dataset for Chinese food recognition. arXiv [Preprint]. arXiv:1705.02743.

Google Scholar

Cunningham, C. A., and Egeth, H. E. (2018). The capture of attention by entirely irrelevant pictures of calorie-dense foods. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 25, 586–595. doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1375-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dagher, A. (2012). Functional brain imaging of appetite. Trends Endocrinol. Metab. 23, 250–260. doi: 10.1016/j.tem.2012.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, C., Curtis, C., Levitan, R. D., Carter, J. C., Kaplan, A. S., and Kennedy, J. L. (2011). Evidence that ‘food addiction’ is a valid phenotype of obesity. Appetite 57, 711–717. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Difeliceantonio, A. G., Coppin, G., Rigoux, L., Edwin Thanarajah, S., Dagher, A., Tittgemeyer, M., et al. (2018). Supra-additive effects of combining fat and carbohydrate on food reward. Cell Metab. 28, 33–44.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2018.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farinella, G. M., Allegra, D., Stanco, F., and Battiato, S. (2015). “On the exploitation of one class classification to distinguish food vs non-food images,” in Proceedings of the International Conference on Image Analysis and Processing, (Berlin: Springer), 375–383. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-23222-5_46

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Felipe, A., Buades, M. J., and Artigas, J. M. (1993). Influence of the contrast sensitivity function on the reaction-time. Vis. Res. 33, 2461–2466. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90126-H

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foroni, F., Pergola, G., Argiris, G., and Rumiati, R. I. (2013). The foodcast research image database (FRIDa). Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:51. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuhrer, D., Zysset, S., and Stumvoll, M. (2008). Brain activity in hunger and satiety: an exploratory visually stimulated FMRI study. Obesity 16, 945–950. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.33

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geiselman, P. J., Anderson, A. M., Dowdy, M. L., West, D. B., Redmann, S. M., and Smith, S. R. (1998). Reliability and validity of a macronutrient self-selection paradigm and a food preference questionnaire. Physiol. Behav. 63, 919–928. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(97)00542-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, C. D., Duraccio, K. M., Barnett, K. A., and Stevens, K. S. (2016). Appropriateness of the food-pics image database for experimental eating and appetite research with adolescents. Eat. Behav. 23, 195–199. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, A., Hardman, C. A., Lawrence, N., and Field, M. (2018). Cognitive training as a potential treatment for overweight and obesity: a critical review of the evidence. Appetite 124, 50–67. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.05.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Killgore, W. D. S., Young, A. D., Femia, L. A., Bogorodzki, P., Rogowska, J., and Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2003). Cortical and limbic activation during viewing of high- versus low-calorie foods. Neuroimage 19, 1381–1394. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00191-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, J. L., Fearnbach, S. N., Ramakrishnapillai, S., Shankpal, P., Geiselman, P. J., Martin, C. K., et al. (2018). Perceptual characterization of the macronutrient picture system (MaPS) for food image fMRI. Front. Psychol. 9:17. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kovalenko, L. Y., Chaumon, M., and Busch, N. A. (2012). A Pool of Pairs of Related Objects (POPORO) for investigating visual semantic integration: behavioral and electrophysiological validation. Brain Topogr. 25, 272–284. doi: 10.1007/s10548-011-0216-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., and Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective Ratings of Pictures and Instruction Manual. Technical Report A-8. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.

Google Scholar

Mace, M. J. M., Thorpe, S. J., and Fabre-Thorpe, M. (2005). Rapid categorization of achromatic natural scenes: how robust at very low contrasts? Eur. J. Neurosci. 21, 2007–2018. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04029.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meule, A., Kübler, A., and Blechert, J. (2013). Time course of electrocortical food-cue responses during cognitive regulation of craving. Front. Psychol. 4:669. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meule, A., Skirde, A. K., Freund, R., Vogele, C., and Kubler, A. (2012). High-calorie food-cues impair working memory performance in high and low food cravers. Appetite 59, 264–269. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miccoli, L., Delgado, R., Rodríguez-Ruiz, S., Guerra, P., García-Mármol, E., and Fernández-Santaella, M. C. (2014). Meet OLAF, a good friend of the IAPS! The Open Library of Affective Foods: a tool to investigate the emotional impact of food in adolescents. Plos One 9:e114515. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0114515

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nummenmaa, L., Hietanen, J. K., Calvo, M. G., and Hyönä, J. (2011). Food catches the eye but not for everyone: a BMI–contingent attentional bias in rapid detection of nutriments. Plos One 6:e19215. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Donell, B. M., Barraza, J. F., and Colombo, E. M. (2010). The effect of chromatic and luminance information on reaction times. Visual Neurosci. 27, 119–129. doi: 10.1017/S0952523810000143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pelchat, M. L., Johnson, A., Chan, R., Valdez, J., and Ragland, J. D. (2004). Images of desire: food-craving activation during fMRI. Neuroimage 23, 1486–1493. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pourtois, G., Dan, E. S., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., and Vuilleumier, P. (2005). Enhanced extrastriate visual response to bandpass spatial frequency filtered fearful faces: time course and topographic evoked-potentials mapping. Hum. Brain Mapp. 26, 65–79. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prada, M., Rodrigues, D., Garrido, M. V., and Lopes, J. (2017). Food-pics-PT: portuguese validation of food images in 10 subjective evaluative dimensions. Food Qual. Prefer. 61, 15–25. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.04.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pursey, K. M., Stanwell, P., Callister, R. J., Brain, K., Collins, C. E., and Burrows, T. L. (2014). Neural responses to visual food cues according to weight status: a systematic review of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Front. Nutr. 1:7. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2014.00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schadow, J., Lenz, D., Thaerig, S., Busch, N. A., Frund, I., Rieger, J. W., et al. (2007). Stimulus intensity affects early sensory processing: visual contrast modulates evoked gamma-band activity in human EEG. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 66, 28–36. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schur, E. A., Kleinhans, N. M., Goldberg, J., Buchwald, D., Schwartz, M. W., and Maravilla, K. (2009). Activation in brain energy regulation and reward centers by food cues varies with choice of visual stimulus. Int. J. Obes. 33, 653–661. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2009.56

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shiffman, S. (2000). Comments on craving. Addiction 95, S171–S175. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.95.8s2.6.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siep, N., Roefs, A., Roebroeck, A., Havermans, R., Bonte, M. L., and Jansen, A. (2009). Hunger is the best spice: an fMRI study of the effects of attention, hunger and calorie content on food reward processing in the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Behav. Brain Res. 198, 149–158. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.10.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spence, C., Okajima, K., Cheok, A. D., Petit, O., and Michel, C. (2016). Eating with our eyes: from visual hunger to digital satiation. Brain Cogn. 110, 53–63. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2015.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stice, E., Lawrence, N. S., Kemps, E., and Veling, H. (2016). Training motor responses to food: a novel treatment for obesity targeting implicit processes. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 49, 16–27. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, D. W., Fellows, L. K., Small, D. M., and Dagher, A. (2012). Food and drug cues activate similar brain regions: a meta-analysis of functional MRI studies. Physiol. Behav. 106, 317–324. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toepel, U., Knebel, J. F., Hudry, J., Le Coutre, J., and Murray, M. M. (2009). The brain tracks the energetic value in food images. Neuroimage 44, 967–974. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uher, R., Treasure, J., Heining, M., Brammer, M. J., and Campbell, I. C. (2006). Cerebral processing of food-related stimuli: effects of fasting and gender. Behav. Brain Res. 169, 111–119. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

VanRullen, R. (2006). On second glance: still no high-level pop-out effect for faces. Vis. Res. 46, 3017–3027. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wardle, M. C., Lopez-Gamundi, P., and Flagel, S. B. (2018). Measuring appetitive conditioned responses in humans. Physiol. Behav. 188, 140–150. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: experimental research, image database, eating behavior, food stimuli, cue reactivity

Citation: Blechert J, Lender A, Polk S, Busch NA and Ohla K (2019) Food-Pics_Extended—An Image Database for Experimental Research on Eating and Appetite: Additional Images, Normative Ratings and an Updated Review. Front. Psychol. 10:307. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00307

Received: 07 November 2018; Accepted: 31 January 2019;
Published: 07 March 2019.

Edited by:

Astrid Müller, Hannover Medical School, Germany

Reviewed by:

Lisa Schäfer, Integriertes Forschungs- und Behandlungszentrum (IFB) AdipositasErkrankungen, Germany
Silke M. Mueller, University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany

Copyright © 2019 Blechert, Lender, Polk, Busch and Ohla. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jens Blechert, jens.blechert@sbg.ac.at

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.