Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 17 October 2012
Sec. Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Forethought as an evolutionary doorway to emotions and consciousness View all 27 articles

Evolution of Social Predictive Brains?

  • 1Research Department of Cognitive Neuropsychiatry and Preventative Medicine, LWL University Hospital Bochum, Bochum, Germany
  • 2International Graduate School of Neuroscience, Ruhr University Bochum, Bochum, Germany

A commentary on

Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science
by Clark, A. (in press). Behav. Brain Sci.

Clark’s (in press) paper gives us an adept introduction into the potential for the concept of the predictive brain to provide us with an integrative and unifying framework to explain cognition. Indeed, this raises an abundance of new questions for possible future work to address. It also highlights the necessity for much gap-filling that will arise through the inevitable application of these principles to different aspects of cognition, as the breadth of the promise of the predictive brain in explaining neural and psychological phenomena becomes wider. Clark touches on some possible expansions of the predictive brain into the realm of social neuroscience. In this commentary, the relatively new hypothesis of the “social predictive brain” is presented. We argue that there is already a large body of empirical and theoretical neuroscientific work from neurophysiological, behavioral, and computational perspectives that provide substantial evidence for a fundamental role for predictive mechanisms in the processing of social information and in social interaction. Further to this, we propose that the evolution of social cognitive processes, or in other words, the evolution of the social brain, has been built upon the increasing complexity of the predictive mechanisms of perception, action, and learning, which Clark outlines in his paper.

To successfully interact with others, we must be able to interpret and understand others’ behavior, and this often involves predicting their cognitive and emotional mental states. It is likely that this is largely formed from our own perceptual and motor experiences and facilitated by the expectations acquired from previous social experience. In recent years, more empirical work has confirmed the existence of shared neural representations in social interaction, in that similar patterns of neural activity are recruited during one’s own experience and when observing others. In particular, the processing involved in the observation of others’ actions (Gallese et al., 1996), rewards (Ma et al., 2011), errors (van Schie et al., 2004), pain (Cheng et al., 2008), and fear (Mineka and Cook, 1993) is conceived through predictive mechanisms in both first-person and third-person experience. It is thought that these shared neural representations may form the neural basis for understanding the mental states of others. Predictive coding models of the mirror neuron system (Kilner et al., 2007) and the MOSAIC model of social interaction (Wolpert et al., 2003) are pertinent examples of the central role of predictive mechanisms and Bayesian inference in social interactive processes. Behrens et al. (2008) proposes the presence of a “social prediction error” during social valuation, and Heyes (2011) compellingly argues that social learning involves the same mechanisms as non-social learning, which may also apply to social decision-making (e.g., Yoshida et al., 2010). Expectancy violations in social contexts may be represented by a similar prediction error as that coding for the discrepancy between one’s own expectation and experience, such as in social deviance and the breaking of social norms (Harris and Fiske, 2010; Kim et al., 2011). Other evidence that suggests a fundamental role for predictive mechanisms in social interactive cognitive processes comes from work demonstrating a direct modulatory effect of social context and social information on low level perceptual and sensorimotor processing. For example, when participants were engaged in a social interaction, this improved visual discrimination and detection of biological motion (Neri et al., 2006; Manera et al., 2011). This “inverse relationship” between bottom-up perceptual input and top-down social information implies the presence of “social” forward (internally generated) models acting as top-down priors, which may be competing with other internal models for overall control, comparable to forward models of action (Wolpert and Miall, 1996).

In accordance with this body of work, it would be legitimate to argue that either the evolution of at least some social cognitive processes such as “theory of mind” emerged through “cooptation” of predictive mechanisms or vice versa, as selection pressures on sophisticated abilities to predict and manipulate the behavior of con-specifics increased in early hominid environments that became more heavily reliant on social cooperation. The modulation of predictive mechanisms by social context could be explained by a prioritized attentional orientation toward social information (Driver et al., 1999), which would likely have had some adaptive evolutionary benefits. The increasing complexity of the social environment in primates and humans may have consequently had an impact on the development of fundamental predictive mechanisms, and therefore recruiting previously non-social cognitions and neural structures for social predictive functions. This notion could also be supported by the substantial overlap seen between brain areas implicated in the “predictive brain” (Bubic et al., 2010) and the “social brain” (Abu-Akel and Shamay-Tsoory, 2011). These suggestions are compatible with the social brain hypothesis (Brothers, 1990; Dunbar, 1998). Indeed, Sallet et al. (2011) have recently shown that social network size and social status is correlated with gray matter volume in the superior temporal cortex and rostral prefrontal cortex in macaques. As an alternative account, which is at least equally complimentary to our proposal of the predictive social brain (Brown and Brüne, 2012), Barton (2012) convincingly demonstrates the benefit of using cerebellar neuron number as a more appropriate index of cognitive evolution, and consequently the evolution of social intelligence, as opposed to the more commonly used index of neocortex volume and neuron number. He also points to the work identifying the cerebellum as the source of predictive internal (forward) models of action (Wolpert et al., 1998), which have been extended to action understanding and action prediction in social interaction (Wolpert et al., 2003). One hypothesis of the evolution of the predictive social brain would be that the brain areas involved in predictive processing of social information are also shaped by the complexity of the social environment.

As the validity of the Bayesian/predictive brain hypothesis further gets put to the test in different realms of cognitive science, we will begin to reveal the ability to generalize these principles to other cognitive and psychological phenomena. However, it may be the case that we have already made substantial headway in confirming the potential breadth of applying such principles, at least in social neuroscience (Brown and Brüne, 2012). Therefore, it would be of value to take stock of what existing evidence from social neuroscience lends compatibility with the concept of the predictive brain, and how the implications from this work might impact on current standpoints on the evolution of the social brain.

References

Abu-Akel, A., and Shamay-Tsoory, S. (2011). Neuroanatomical and neurochemical bases of theory of mind. Neuropsychologia 49, 2971–2984.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barton, R. (2012). Embodied cognitive evolution and the cerebellum. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 367, 2097–2107.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Behrens, T. E., Hunt, L. T., Woolrich, M. W., and Rushworth, M. F. (2008). Associative learning of social value. Nature 456, 245–249.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brothers, L. (1990). The social brain: a project for integrating primate behaviour and neurophysiology in a new domain. Concepts Neurosci. 1, 27–251.

Brown, E. C., and Brüne, M. (2012). The role of prediction in social neuroscience. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:147. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00147

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bubic, A., von Cramon, D. Y., and Schubotz, R. I. (2010). Prediction, cognition and the brain. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:25. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00025

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cheng, Y., Yang, C. Y., Lin, C. P., Lee, P. L., and Decety, J. (2008). The perception of pain in others suppresses somatosensory oscillations: a magnetoencephalography study. Neuroimage 40, 1833–1840.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Clark, A. (in press). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behav. Brain Sci.

Driver, J., Davis, G., Ricciardelli, P., Kidd, P., Maxwell, E., and Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Gaze perception triggers reflexive visuospatial orienting. Vis. Cogn. 6, 509–540.

CrossRef Full Text

Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evol. Anthrop. 6, 178–190.

CrossRef Full Text

Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., and Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain 119(Pt 2), 593–609.

CrossRef Full Text

Harris, L. T., and Fiske, S. T. (2010). Neural regions that underlie reinforcement learning are also active for social expectancy violations. Soc. Neurosci. 5, 76–91.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Heyes, C. (2011). What’s social about social learning? J. Comp. Psychol. 126, 193–202.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kilner, J. M., Friston, K. J., and Frith, C. D. (2007). The mirror-neuron system: a Bayesian perspective. Neuroreport 18, 619–623.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kim, B. R., Liss, A., Rao, M., Singer, Z., and Compton, R. J. (2011). Social deviance activates the brain’s error-monitoring system. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 12, 65–73.

CrossRef Full Text

Ma, Q., Shen, Q., Xu, Q., Li, D., Shu, L., and Weber, B. (2011). Empathic responses to others’ gains and losses: an electrophysiological investigation. Neuroimage 54, 2472–2480.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Manera, V., Becchio, C., Schouten, B., Bara, B. G., and Verfaillie, K. (2011). Communicative interactions improve visual detection of biological motion. PLoS ONE 6, e14594. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014594

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mineka, S., and Cook, M. (1993). Mechanisms involved in the observational conditioning of fear. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 122, 23–38.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Neri, P., Luu, J. Y., and Levi, D. M. (2006). Meaningful interactions can enhance visual discrimination of human agents. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1186–1192.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sallet, J., Mars, R. B., Noonan, M. P., Andersson, J. L., O’Reilly, J. X., Jbabdi, S., et al. (2011). Social network size affects neural circuits in macaques. Science 334, 697–700.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

van Schie, H. T., Mars, R. B., Coles, M. G., and Bekkering, H. (2004). Modulation of activity in medial frontal and motor cortices during error observation. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 549–554.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wolpert, D. M., Doya, K., and Kawato, M. (2003). A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 358, 593–602.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wolpert, D. M., and Miall, R. C. (1996). Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Netw. 9, 1265–1279.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wolpert, D. M., Miall, R. C., and Kawato, M. (1998). Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 2, 338–347.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yoshida, W., Seymour, B., Friston, K. J., and Dolan, R. J. (2010). Neural mechanisms of belief inference during cooperative games. J. Neurosci. 30, 10744–10751.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Citation: Brown EC and Brüne M (2012) Evolution of social predictive brains? Front. Psychology 3:414. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00414

Received: 03 September 2012; Accepted: 30 September 2012;
Published online: 17 October 2012.

Edited by:

Axel Cleeremans, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Belgium

Reviewed by:

Shimon Edelman, Cornell University, USA

Copyright: © 2012 Brown and Brüne. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: elliot.c.brown@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.