Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Endocrinol., 14 April 2014
Sec. Pediatric Endocrinology
This article is part of the Research Topic Assessing Prenatal and Neonatal Gonadal Steroid Exposure for Studies of Human Development: Methodological and Theoretical Challenges. View all 10 articles

Mechanisms of Prenatal Programing: Identifying and Distinguishing the Impact of Steroid Hormones

  • 1Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, NY, USA
  • 2Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, NY, USA

Developmental programing is gaining considerable leverage as a conceptual framework for understanding individual variability in human behavioral and somatic health. The current mini-review examines some of the key conceptual and methodological challenges for developmental programing research focused on fetal sex steroid exposure and physical, behavioral, physiological, and health outcomes. Specifically, we consider the bases for focusing on sex steroids, methods for assessing prenatal steroid hormone exposure, confounding factors, and the most relevant postnatal outcomes. We conclude with a brief consideration, based on current knowledge, of the applications of the existing findings for further research and practice.

The developmental origins of health and disease hypothesis and the programing mechanisms that underlie it are a major focus of current basic science and clinical health research. A common theme is that stress (or in humans, anxiety) experienced by the mother is transmitted to the fetus and the ensuing developmental changes may have long-lasting effects on offspring biology and behavior. The hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis – and particularly glucocorticoid exposure – has attracted attention as a likely mediating mechanism (13). However, it may be worthwhile to look beyond the HPA axis. In this mini-review, we consider the role that sex steroids may play in prenatal programing and identify strategies for managing some of the methodological challenges that have arisen.

Programing Effects from Prenatal Stress/Anxiety: Sex Steroids

It is natural that HPA axis-mediated programing mechanisms have attracted substantial research attention given the dominant role of prenatal stress as a “risk phenotype” in the animal and, more recently, human research. The mediating role of glucocorticoids in programing fetal physiology may extend beyond prenatal psychosocial stress to also include exposure to other types of stressors, such as nutritional deprivation. Moreover, there are equally compelling reasons why an exclusive focus on a glucocorticoid-mediated mechanism will be inadequate and consideration of complementary mechanisms, notably sex steroid hormones, could be informative. For example, the HPA and hypothalamic–pituitary–gonadal (HPG) axes show considerable overlap and interaction (4); just as stress may affect sex steroid hormone production, so sex steroids may act on the stress response system.

One way of evaluating the likely importance of sex steroids in developmental programing is to examine sex differences in the associations between prenatal stress and outcomes in offspring. For instance, in animal models, male and female offsprings often show different patterns of developmental programing (5). In some cases, only one sex appears to be affected by prenatal stress (6), but in other research, the sexes show opposite directions of effect. For example, in a rat model, prenatal restraint stress was associated with increased anxiety in males but decreased anxiety in females compared to controls (7).

Whether or not there are sex differences in human studies of the effects of prenatal stress/anxiety on child outcomes is far from clear. Some examples have been reported for behavioral and cognitive development (8, 9) and the evidence for cardiovascular outcomes is strong (10). However, for several of the most widely researched stress-related outcomes, such as behavioral and emotional problems and HPA axis function, few, if any, robust and consistent sex differences have been found (11, 12). This inconsistency across human studies raises several critical issues. First, there is no reason to expect that responses to prenatal stress will differ by sex across all traits, so carefully selecting only those phenotypes of most relevance is important. Second, it is plausible that focusing on HPA axis-mediated mechanisms, to the exclusion of other mediators, may have limited our ability to detect and understand sex differences.

Two lines of study provide evidence of sex steroid involvement in stress-related developmental programing. In animal models, prenatal stress is associated with changes in anogenital distance (AGD), a marker of prenatal androgen exposure; these associations have now been noted in humans (1315). Critically, the direction of effects differs by sex: prenatal stressed males show demasculinization and females show masculinization of AGD. Interestingly, in contrast to the literature on prenatal nutritional stress, in which males tend to be more affected postnatally (16), the effects of psychosocial stress on reproductive development appear to be stronger in females (13, 17). Although research on prenatal stress and reproductive health and development in humans is limited, the fact that AGD appears to be stable over time and has been linked to adult reproductive outcomes (18, 19) suggests the potential importance of this line of research. A related line of work suggests prenatal programing from sex steroids from testosterone in amniotic fluid on autistic behaviors (20, 21) as well as temperament and play behavior (9, 22) in childhood. These findings point to the need for further clinical research on developmental programing of sex steroids and their effects on human health and development.

Methodological Challenges for Investigating Prenatal Programing Effects of Sex Steroids in Human Studies

There are several methodological challenges for future research on the possible programing role of prenatal sex steroid exposure; we consider several in this mini-review.

Assessing Fetal Exposure

The first, most basic question is how to assess fetal exposure to sex steroid hormones. Several studies have inferred fetal exposure to sex steroids from prenatal maternal distress (13, 23), although direct evidence of an association has not been found, e.g., Ref. (24). Directly measuring fetal exposure to sex steroids remains a major challenge. In the past, many studies were able to measure hormones in amniotic fluid when there was clinical indication; however, amniocentesis is losing favor as a clinical diagnostic tool and is unlikely to be a viable measurement strategy moving forward. Less direct measures of fetal exposure may be obtained from the mother; saliva, serum, and hair have been suggested as potential candidates. Circulating maternal hormone levels may represent production by the fetus, placenta, and the mother herself, with the relative proportions differing by hormone and stage of gestation (2527). Circulating maternal testosterone levels could be a useful index, but it is likely that most fetal androgens are aromatized by the placenta before reaching maternal circulation (28). Finally, if circulating maternal sex steroids are correlated with fetal exposure, it still remains unclear which maternal medium is most appropriate for measurement, given that there may be inconsistencies across media (29). In any event, there is good reason to suspect that peripheral maternal levels of sex steroids may have minimal influence on fetal exposure. One potentially valuable strategy to index fetal exposure is to examine placental gene expression (see below).

Indexing fetal exposure to sex steroids in utero is an essential but difficult task for research. The availability of non-intrusive and reliable estimates of fetal exposure has proved difficult and is a central challenge to overcome in future research.

Distinguishing between Correlated Steroid Hormone Exposures

Distinguishing the impact of sex steroid hormones from other sources of programing is another major challenge for research. For example, cortisol and testosterone are positively correlated in fetal blood (30) and in amniotic fluid (31). Given that, it may be difficult to separate programing effects attributable to sex steroids from those attributable to glucocorticoids. One approach in past research is to examine natural experiments in which a medical condition, such as congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH), alters the fetal hormonal milieu in a well-characterized manner. Findings from studies of girls with CAH, for example, have helped to establish that prenatal exposure to sex steroids may program postnatal outcomes ranging from play behavior, to motor development, to personality (3234). Interestingly, females with non-classical CAH have “female-typical” genitalia at birth (with evidence of impaired fertility later in life) (35), however, this does not rule out the possibility of subtly masculinized AGD, which would not be evident to a casual observer.

On the other hand, it is unclear if the findings can be generalized to non-clinical populations with fetal hormonal exposures within the normal range of variation. Other means of differentiating between glucocorticoid and sex steroid-based programing within healthy populations are needed. It is an interesting possibility that the developmental programing studies associated with maternal prenatal distress may have over-attributed effects to stress hormones because they have (largely) ignored sex steroids.

Role of Placental Structure and Function

A novel and potentially promising approach to indexing fetal steroid hormone exposure is to examine placental gene expression and epigenetic changes. As the main maternal–fetal interface, the placenta is of inarguable importance for understanding developmental programing. To date, research has been influenced by a glucocorticoid-mediated model. For instance, there has been great interest in the effects of stress on placental production of 11-β-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase 2 (11BHSD2), an enzyme which shields the fetus from maternal cortisol by converting it to inactive cortisone (36). However, the placenta’s endocrine production and regulation clearly extend far beyond 11BHSD2; prenatal distress may affect other placental steroid hormone pathways, impacting sex steroidogenesis and activity. At the same time, remarkably few studies on developmental programing have looked forward from placental structure and function to clinical phenotypes in the offspring; exploratory work on this subject is needed.

There are obviously substantial hurdles to assessing placental gene expression; it is a methodology that poses significant collection, cost, and laboratory demands. In healthy pregnancies, moreover, we are limited to looking at placental morphology and physiology at birth rather than at critical periods earlier in gestation. Nevertheless, examining placental variation, particularly in steroidogenesis pathway activity, in relation to prenatal exposures and postnatal outcomes requires attention given the (other) inherent challenges of estimating fetal exposure to sex steroids in a non-intrusive, reliable way.

Identifying Relevant Postnatal Phenotypes

A final challenge to be considered is the selection of relevant postnatal phenotypes. Traits with notable sex differences may be the most fruitful starting points. Autism spectrum disorders and associated traits, for instance, differ quite notably in prevalence and presentation between the sexes (37). Play behavior is another strong candidate given the extensive evidence that from early childhood onward, males and females show clear preferences for sex-typical toys (38). Both autism and sexually dimorphic play behaviors have been associated with exposure to prenatal stress (17, 23), although further work is clearly needed. Other phenotypes that do not show consistent sex differences, such as temperament, may be less relevant to consider in this context.

Levels of circulating sex hormones are extremely low from shortly after birth until puberty. Nevertheless, even in infancy there appears to be sex differences in neurodevelopmental traits (39), suggesting that there may be prenatal, organizational effects of exposure of sex steroids. Other sex differences in development (in brain development, for instance), emerge later in childhood, but prior to the peripubertal increase in sex hormones and may plausibly be the product of in utero sex steroid programing. Studies showing sex differences in infancy are interesting because there are minimally detectable levels of sex hormones in circulation; that means that these infant sex differences may be induced by prenatal sex steroid exposure. There are other well-known differences between the sexes that are evident early in development, including physical growth and brain development, e.g., Ref. (40). A challenge for future research is to examine if these – and perhaps other – early-emerging sex differences in biology and behavior can be attributable in part to prenatal programing of sex steroid exposure.

Conclusion

Research on human health and development is just beginning to translate the animal work on developmental programing effects of sex steroids. Further studies are needed to substantiate this emerging line of investigation and to provide a broader biological context in which to interpret the sizable research based on developmental programing associated with prenatal stress and HPA axis mechanisms, a literature which has begun to influence practice and policy, e.g., Ref. (41). Progress in the area of research will require the consideration and surmounting of several methodological challenges, which we have highlighted.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors’ work is supported by grant MH 073019, MH 073842, MH 097293, K12 ES019852-01, and P30 ES001247.

References

1. Gluckman PD, Hanson MA, Beedle AS, Raubenheimer D. Fetal and neonatal pathways to obesity. Front Horm Res (2008) 36:61–72. doi:10.1159/000115337

CrossRef Full Text

2. Glover V, O’Connor TG, O’Donnell K. Prenatal stress and the programming of the HPA axis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2010) 35:17–22. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

3. Cottrell EC, Seckl JR. Prenatal stress, glucocorticoids and the programming of adult disease. Front Behav Neurosci (2009) 3:19. doi:10.3389/neuro.08.019.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

4. Lovejoy DA, Barsyte-Lovejoy D. Systems approaches to genomic and epigenetic inter-regulation of peptide hormones in stress and reproduction. Prog Biophys Mol Biol (2013) 113(3):375–86. doi:10.1016/j.pbiomolbio.2013.02.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

5. Bale TL. Sex differences in prenatal epigenetic programming of stress pathways. Stress (2011) 14:348–56. doi:10.3109/10253890.2011.586447

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

6. Howerton CL, Morgan CP, Fischer DB, Bale TL. O-GlcNAc transferase (OGT) as a placental biomarker of maternal stress and reprogramming of CNS gene transcription in development. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2013) 110:5169–74. doi:10.1073/pnas.1300065110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

7. Zuena AR, Mairesse J, Casolini P, Cinque C, Alemà GS, Morley-Fletcher S, et al. Prenatal restraint stress generates two distinct behavioral and neurochemical profiles in male and female rats. PLoS One (2008) 3:e2170. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0002170

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

8. Sandman CA, Glynn LM, Davis EP. Is there a viability-vulnerability tradeoff? Sex differences in fetal programming. J Psychosom Res (2013) 75:327–35. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.07.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

9. Bergman K, Glover V, Sarkar P, Abbott DH, O’Connor TG. In utero cortisol and testosterone exposure and fear reactivity in infancy. Horm Behav (2010) 57:306–12. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.12.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

10. Grigore D, Ojeda NB, Alexander BT. Sex differences in the fetal programming of hypertension. Gend Med (2008) 5(Suppl A):S121–32. doi:10.1016/j.genm.2008.03.012

CrossRef Full Text

11. O’Donnell KJ, Glover V, Barker ED, O’Connor TG. The persisting effect of maternal mood in pregnancy on childhood psychopathology. Dev Psychoppathol (2014) (in press).

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

12. O’Donnell KJ, Glover V, Jenkins J, Browne D, Ben-Shlomo Y, Golding J, et al. Prenatal maternal mood is associated with altered diurnal cortisol in adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2013) 38:1630–8. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.01.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

13. Barrett ES, Parlett LE, Sathyanarayana S, Liu F, Redmon JB, Wang C, et al. Prenatal exposure to stressful life events is associated with masculinized anogenital distance (AGD) in female infants. Physiol Behav (2013) 11(4–115):14–20. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.03.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

14. vom Saal FS, Quadagno DM, Even MD, Keisler LW, Keisler DH, Khan S. Paradoxical effects of maternal stress on fetal steroids and postnatal reproductive traits in female mice from different intrauterine positions. Biol Reprod (1990) 43:751–61. doi:10.1095/biolreprod43.5.751

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

15. Ward IL, Weisz J. Maternal stress alters plasma testosterone in fetal males. Science (1980) 207:328–9. doi:10.1126/science.7188648

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

16. Begum G, Davies A, Stevens A, Oliver M, Jaquiery A, Challis J, et al. Maternal undernutrition programs tissue-specific epigenetic changes in the glucocorticoid receptor in adult offspring. Endocrinology (2013) 154:4560–9. doi:10.1210/en.2013-1693

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

17. Barrett ES, Redmon JB, Wang C, Sparks A, Swan SH. Exposure to prenatal life events stress is associated with masculinized play behavior in girls. Neurotoxicology (2014) 41C:20–7. doi:10.1016/j.neuro.2013.12.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

18. Eisenberg ML, Hsieh MH, Walters RC, Krasnow R, Lipshultz LI. The relationship between anogenital distance, fatherhood, and fertility in adult men. PLoS One (2011) 6:e18973. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0018973

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

19. Mendiola J, Roca M, Mínguez-Alarcón L, Mira-Escolano MP, López-Espín JJ, Barrett ES, et al. Anogenital distance is related to ovarian follicular number in young Spanish women: a cross-sectional study. Environ Health (2012) 11:90. doi:10.1186/1476-069X-11-90

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

20. Knickmeyer RC, Baron-Cohen S, Auyeung B, Ashwin E. How to test the extreme male brain theory of autism in terms of foetal androgens? J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:995–6; author reply 997–8. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0553-y

CrossRef Full Text

21. Auyeung B, Taylor K, Hackett G, Baron-Cohen S. Foetal testosterone and autistic traits in 18 to 24-month-old children. Mol Autism (2010) 1:11. doi:10.1186/2040-2392-1-11

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

22. Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S, Ashwin E, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G, et al. Fetal testosterone predicts sexually differentiated childhood behavior in girls and in boys. Psychol Sci (2009) 20:144–8. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02279.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

23. Hines M, Johnston KJ, Golombok S, Rust J, Stevens M, Golding J, et al. Prenatal stress and gender role behavior in girls and boys: a longitudinal, population study. Horm Behav (2002) 42:126–34. doi:10.1006/hbeh.2002.1814

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

24. Sarkar P, Bergman K, O’Connor TG, Glover V. Maternal antenatal anxiety and amniotic fluid cortisol and testosterone: possible implications for foetal programming. J Neuroendocrinol (2008) 20:489–96. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2826.2008.01659.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

25. Tulchinsky D, Hobel CJ, Yeager E, Marshall JR. Plasma estrone, estradiol, estriol, progesterone, and 17-hydroxyprogesterone in human pregnancy. I. Normal pregnancy. Am J Obstet Gynecol (1972) 112:1095–100.

26. Speroff L, Fritz MA. Clinical Gynecologic Endocrinology and Infertility. 7th ed. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins (2005).

27. Strauss JF III. Yen and Jaffe’s Reproductive Endocrinology: Physiology, Pathophysiology, and Clinical Management. 6th ed. Philadelphia: Saunders (2009).

28. Toriola AT, Vääräsmäki M, Lehtinen M, Zeleniuch-Jacquotte A, Lundin E, Rodgers KG, et al. Determinants of maternal sex steroids during the first half of pregnancy. Obstet Gynecol (2011) 118:1029–36. doi:10.1097/AOG.0b013e3182342b7f

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

29. Hampson E, Phillips SD, Soares CN, Steiner M. Steroid concentrations in antepartum and postpartum saliva: normative values in women and correlations with serum. Biol Sex Differ (2013) 4:7. doi:10.1186/2042-6410-4-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

30. Gitau R, Adams D, Fisk NM, Glover V. Fetal plasma testosterone correlates positively with cortisol. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed (2005) 90:F166–9. doi:10.1136/adc.2004.049320

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

31. Sarkar P, Bergman K, Fisk NM, O’Connor TG, Glover V. Amniotic fluid testosterone: relationship with cortisol and gestational age. Clin Endocrinol (Oxf) (2007) 67:743–7. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2265.2007.02955.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

32. Collaer ML, Brook C, Conway GS, Hindmarsh PC, Hines M. Motor development in individuals with congenital adrenal hyperplasia: strength, targeting, and fine motor skill. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2009) 34:249–58. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.09.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

33. Mathews GA, Fane BA, Conway GS, Brook CGD, Hines M. Personality and congenital adrenal hyperplasia: possible effects of prenatal androgen exposure. Horm Behav (2009) 55:285–91. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.11.007

CrossRef Full Text

34. Pasterski V, Geffner ME, Brain C, Hindmarsh P, Brook C, Hines M. Prenatal hormones and childhood sex-segregation: playmate and play style preferences in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Horm Behav (2011) 59:549–55. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.02.007

CrossRef Full Text

35. Witchel SF, Azziz R. Nonclassic congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Int J Pediatr Endocrinol (2010) 2010:625105. doi:10.1155/2010/625105

CrossRef Full Text

36. O’Donnell KJ, Bugge Jensen A, Freeman L, Khalife N, O’Connor TG, Glover V. Maternal prenatal anxiety and downregulation of placental 11beta- HSD2. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2012) 37:818–26. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.09.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

37. Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Suckling J, Ruigrok AN, Chakrabarti B, Ecker C, et al. Biological sex affects the neurobiology of autism. Brain (2013) 136:2799–815. doi:10.1093/brain/awt216

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

38. Berenbaum SA, Hines M. Early androgens are related to childhood sex-types toy preferences. Psychol Sci (1992) 3:203–6. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb00028.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

39. Quinn PC, Liben LS. A sex difference in mental rotation in young infants. Psychol Sci (2008) 19:1067–70. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02201.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

40. Lenroot RK, Gogtay N, Greenstein DK, Wells EM, Wallace GL, Clasen LS, et al. Sexual dimorphism of brain developmental trajectories during childhood and adolescence. Neuroimage (2007) 36:1065–73. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.053

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

41. O’Connor TG, Monk C, Fitelson EM. Practitioner review: maternal mood in pregnancy and child development – implications for child psychology and psychiatry. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2014) 55:99–111. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12153

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: developmental programing, sex steroids, prenatal maternal distress, child developmental outcomes, methodology

Citation: O’Connor TG and Barrett ES (2014) Mechanisms of prenatal programing: identifying and distinguishing the impact of steroid hormones. Front. Endocrinol. 5:52. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2014.00052

Received: 16 December 2013; Accepted: 28 March 2014;
Published online: 14 April 2014.

Edited by:

S. Loche, ASL Cagliari, Italy

Reviewed by:

Hugo Fideleff, Hospital T. Alvarez, Argentina
Paul Saenger, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Winthrop University Hospital, USA

Copyright: © 2014 O’Connor and Barrett. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Thomas G. O’Connor, Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester Medical Center, 300 Crittenden Blvd, Rochester, NY 14642, USA e-mail: tom_oconnor@urmc.rochester.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.