Yonsei Med J. 2020 Aug;61(8):726-730. English.
Published online Jul 28, 2020.
© Copyright: Yonsei University College of Medicine 2020
Brief Communication

Network Analysis of Language Disorganization in Patients with Schizophrenia

Seon-Cheol Park,1 Kiwon Kim,2 Ok-Jin Jang,3 Hyung-Jun Yoon,4 Seung-Ho Jang,5 Sung-Wan Kim,6 Bong Ju Lee,1 Jae Hong Park,7 Kang Uk Lee,8 and Joonho Choi9
    • 1Department of Psychiatry, Inje University Haeundae Paik Hospital, Busan, Korea.
    • 2Department of Psychiatry, Veteran Health Service Medical Center, Seoul, Korea.
    • 3Department of Psychiatry, Bugok National Hospital, Changyeong, Korea.
    • 4Department of Psychiatry, College of Medicine, Chosun University, Gwangju, Korea.
    • 5Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Wonkwang University, Iksan, Korea.
    • 6Department of Psychiatry, Chonnam National University Medical School, Gwangju, Korea.
    • 7Department of Psychiatry, College of Medicine, Dong-A University, Busan, Korea.
    • 8Department of Psychiatry, Kangwon University School of Medicine, Chuncheon, Korea.
    • 9Department of Psychiatry, Hanyang University Guri Hospital, Guri, Korea.
Received May 18, 2020; Revised June 16, 2020; Accepted July 03, 2020.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/) which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

Language disorganization, an objective component of formal thought process abnormality, has been regarded as a core symptom of schizophrenia from an evolutionary psychopathology perspective. However, to the best of our knowledge, the network structure of language disorganization has rarely been examined in patients with schizophrenia. Thus, our preliminary study aimed to evaluate the network structure using the Clinical Language Disorder Rating Scale (CLANG) in 167 inpatients with schizophrenia. All 17 of the CLANG items were considered to be ordered categorical variables ranging from 0 to 3. Our results indicated that disclosure failure, excess syntactic constraints, abnormal prosody, and aprosodic speech rank among the top five central domains within the network structure. We deemed that disclosure failure and prosody problems are the most important symptoms of language disorder in schizophrenia. Thus, reduced top-down processing of linguistic information may be a core neurobiological underpinning of language disorganization in schizophrenia. Further studies controlling for the potential effects of confounding factors (i.e., duration of illness) on network analyses of language disorder and formal thought disorder are warranted in patients with schizophrenia.

Keywords
Language disorganization; disclosure failure; network structure; schizophrenia

Language disorganization, which is an objective constituent of formal thought process abnormality, is considered to be a core symptom of schizophrenia.1 Researchers have proposed that language disorganization may indicate greater severity and poor social functioning in patients with psychotic disorders.2 According to the conceptualization of Crow,3 there is a shared common neurobiological underpinning of both language and psychosis. Thus, the nuclear symptoms of schizophrenia can be conceptualized as extreme forms of language disorganization, denoted as disturbance of the “axis of indexicality.”4 Additionally, clinical language disorder has been continually defined as a diagnostic criterion for schizophrenia, although its expression has been changed from “incoherence” or “marked loosening of associations” to “disorganized speech” following increased empiricism in the consecutive Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) editions.1 Network analysis has been used to provide a new perspective in psychopathology by considering “symptoms and associations among them” as the disease itself. On the contrary, traditional theory has regarded “symptoms” as “outcome factors of an underlying disease.”5 Since network analysis defines centrality as the overall inter-connection of symptoms, central symptoms can be characterized by greater influences on the network structure rather than peripheral symptoms.6 To the best of our knowledge, the network structures of positive and negative symptoms, but not language, have been estimated in patients with schizophrenia. Thus, our preliminary study aimed to estimate the network structure of language disorganization in patients with schizophrenia.

As described elsewhere,7 during a survey period from January to June 2014, a total of 167 inpatients with DSM-5 diagnosed schizophrenia8 were recruited from Yong-In Mental Hospital and Chuncheon National Hospital in Korea. Inclusion criteria were age between 19 and 64 years, having greater than 2 weeks of psychiatric hospitalization, and attainment of over 6 years of education. Exclusion criteria were comorbid organic mental disorders, intellectual disability, alcohol or substance abuse, seizure disorders, neurological disorders, and severe physical disorders. DSM-5 was used in the evaluation of psychiatric disorders. Principally, the Thurstone Word Fluency Test was used to detect intellectual disability, given that it has been shown to be positively correlated with intelligence quotient and letter association condition.9 The study protocol and case report form were approved by the Institutional Review Board of Yong-In Mental Hospital (receipt number: 2013-49). Language disorganization in the study participants was evaluated with the Clinical Language Disorder Rating Scale (CLANG),10 which assessed language disorganization and observable expressions of formal thought disorder. CLANG includes a total of 17 items, which are rated on a Likert scale from 0 (normal) to 3 (severe) and has been translated and formally standardized in Korean.7

Using the R-package qgraph,11 the network structure of the 17 CLANG items was estimated in the study participants. The nodes (symptoms) and edges (associations among the symptoms) both constituted the network structure. In addition, the false positive edges were controlled with the least absolute shrinkage and selection operator (LASSO);12 therefore, very small edges were set exactly to zero. Moreover, using the GLASSO (or graphical LASSO) procedures,13 the average edge was defined with the relationship level between two symptoms controlling for all other relationships within the network. Using the shrinkage parameter, extended Bayesian information criteria were minimized, and the underlying network structure was accurately recovered.14, 15 All CLANG items were regarded as ordered-categorical variables and ranged from 0 to 3. Using the modularity-based community-detection algorithm, node clusters were investigated. Using the spin-glass algorithm, we tested whether the number and weighted strength of edges within a cluster exceeded those within another cluster, in terms of communities within the network.16, 17 In terms of centrality indices, node strength centrality was defined as the sum of all associations of a given node with all other nodes, and closeness centrality was defined as a measure of how close a symptom was to all other symptoms. Finally, betweenness centrality was defined as the shortest length of a path connecting any two nodes. Thus, since node strength centrality was substantially correlated with closeness centrality and betweenness centrality, the most central symptoms were estimated within the network structures of the 17 CLANG items.

As described elsewhere,7 the mean age and mean duration of illness of the study participants were 46.5 (SD=11.2) years and 20.9 (SD=10.3) years, respectively (Supplementary Table 1, only online). More than half of the participants were men (51.5%), unmarried (79.1%), educated below a high school graduate level (73.0%), religiously-affiliated, and recruited from Yong-In Mental Hospital (86.8%). In addition, the mean chlorpromazine equivalent dose18 of prescribed antipsychotics was 921.1 (SD=952.0) mg per day. The abbreviations and percentscores of the CLANG items are presented in Table 1.

Table 1
Percent Scores for the CLANG Items (n=167)

Fig. 1A shows how a psychopathological network consisting of the CLANG items was constructed. In terms of the edge statistics, about 88% (n=120) of all possible 136 edges were estimated to be above zero. The estimated network revealed strong positive connections between pragmatic disorder and paraphasic error (0.806), aprosodic speech and abnormal prosody (0.679), lack of detail and poverty of speech (0.611), excess syntactic constraints and referential failures (0.598), excess phonetic association and neologisms (0.528), excess syntactic constraints and abnormal prosody (0.506), and referential failures and aprosodic speech (0.506). On the contrary, the network revealed strong negative connections between poverty of speech and pressure of speech (−0.552), poverty of speech and neologisms (−0.521), and referential failures and abnormal prosody (−0.514). Remarkably, pragmatics disorder, excess phonetic association, and dysfluency were largely isolated within the network. A community-detection method revealed an organization of roughly five clusters as follows: Cluster A included excess phonetic association, pragmatics disorder, neologisms, and paraphasic error. Cluster B included abnormal syntax, lack of semantic association, disclosure failure, excess details, and pressure of speech. Cluster C included excess syntactic constraints and referential failures. Cluster D included lack of details and poverty of speech. Finally, cluster E included aprosodic speech, abnormal prosody, dysfluency, and pressure of speech.

Fig. 1
Network analysis of the 17 Clinical Language Disorder Rating Scale (CLANG) items in patients with schizophrenia (n=167). (A) Network structure of the 17 CLANG items in patients with schizophrenia. Green lines represent positive associations, whereas red lines represent negative associations. Language disorganization was evaluated with CLANG. (B) Node strength centrality, closeness centrality, and betweenness centrality of the 17 CLANG items in patients with schizophrenia. ABP, abnormal prosody; APR, aprosodic speech; ART, dysarthria; ASS, lack of semantic association; ASY, abnormal syntax; DSC, disclosure failure; EDE, excess details; ESY, excess syntactic constrains; FLU, dysfluency; LDE, lack of detail; NEL, neologisms; PAR, paraphasic error; PHO, excess phonetic association; POV, poverty of speech; PRA, pragmatics disorder; PRS, pressure of speech; REF, referential failure.

In terms of the node statistics, there were no abrupt changes with smooth declines in symptom importance as shown in Fig. 1B. Disclosure failure was most centrally situated within the network, followed by excess syntactic constraints and abnormal prosody; therefore, it also showed the highest symptom importance within the network. On the contrary, as pragmatics disorder was nearly unconnected, it represented the lowest node strength within the network, followed by excess phonetic association, and dysfluency.

In our findings, variability in node strength centrality for the 17 CLANG items was estimated. Disclosure failure, excess syntactic constraints, abnormal prosody, and aprosodic speech (mainly related to the disturbance of disclosure and prosody) ranked among the top five central symptoms (ranking 1, 2, 4, and 5 out of 17, respectively) within the network. In contrast, pragmatic disorder, excess phonetic association, and abnormal syntax (mainly related to the disturbance of association) were among the five most peripheral symptoms (ranking 17, 16, and 14 out of 17, respectively). Thus, as disclosure failure and dysprosody were situated relatively centrally and associative disturbance was situated relatively peripherally within the network, we speculated that self-monitoring problems may be the most influential constituent of language disorganization in schizophrenia. Most notably, disclosure failure was the most central domain within the estimated network of language disorganization in patients with schizophrenia. According to definitions in CLANG, disclosure failure (loss of schematic disorganization) involves a “lack of normal organization in which larger speech units progress from one context to the next in a gradual and prepared manner.”10 The immediate failure to construct coherence links contributes to building meaning across the sentences (discourse) in patients with schizophrenia.19 Also, schizophrenia patients, rather than normal controls, have been characterized by less connection between ideas and clauses. Language disorder of schizophrenia can be closely related to a reduction in the top-down process of linguistic information.20 Herein, our findings suggest that disclosure failure may be a relatively essential factor of language disorganization from a psychopathological network perspective. Furthermore, one study has shown that dysprosody is lateralized to nondominant hemispheres in epilepsy patients.21 Thus, we suspect that language disorganization may be neurobiologically underpinned by dysfunction of the nondominant hemisphere. Remarkably, the five organized clusters of language disorganization are partly consistent with the five-factor solution, including pragmatics (i.e., paraphasic error, pragmatics disorder), disclosure (i.e., lack of semantic association, abnormal syntax, disclosure failure, lack of details, dysarthria), production (i.e., excessive dedetails, poverty of speech, pressure of speech), prosody (i.e., aprosodic speech, abnormal prosody, dysfluency, excess syntactic constrains), and association (i.e., excess phonetic association, neologisms, referential failures), which has been previously extracted by a factor analysis of our data.7 Thus, the organization of the five clusters may be supported.

Our study has several limitations. First, estimations of centrality may be biased due to restrictions in the range of CLANG items. In addition, the Pearson correlation coefficients (r) between the standard deviation and node strength centrality of the 17 items were negligible. Second, the study was conducted with a cross-sectional design, and thus, estimated unidirectional networks. As a result, our study could not assess differentiation between outdegree and indegree centrality. Third, since all recruited participants were schizophrenia patients hospitalized in Korean mental hospitals, our findings cannot be generalized to the entire population with schizophrenia. Fourth, since the mean duration of illness of the study participants was greater than 20 years, the chronic course of schizophrenia could affect the estimated network of language disorganization. It has been reported that the first, second, and third phases of the course of schizophrenia are characterized by thinking disorders, neologism and others, and schizophasia, respectively.22 Thus, the possibility that language disorganization may be a chronic feature of schizophrenia rather than a core symptom of schizophrenia cannot be overlooked. However, as per our findings, disclosure failure has been the most central domain with the network of language disorganization, followed by excess syntactic constraints and abnormal prosody. Given that the symptoms of schizophasia have not been situated at a relatively central domain within the network, the chronic feature of schizophrenia may not significantly affect our findings. Despite these limitations, our study is the first to investigate the network structure of language disorganization from a new perspective of psychopathology. Altogether, we deemed that disclosure failure may be the most influential domain of language disorganization in patients with schizophrenia. Moreover, our results may provide future directions for additional studies controlling for the potential effects of confounding factors (i.e., duration of illness) on network analyses of language disorder and formal thought disorder in patients with schizophrenia.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

Supplementary Table 1

Baseline Characteristics of the Study Participants (n=167)

Click here to view.(21K, pdf)

Notes

The authors have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS:

  • Conceptualization: Seon-Cheol Park.

  • Data curation: Seon-Cheol Park, Kang Uk Lee, and Joonho Choi.

  • Formal analysis: Seon-Cheol Park.

  • Investigation: Seon-Cheol Park and Kiwon Kim.

  • Methodology: Seon-Cheol Park and Kiwon Kim.

  • Resources: Seon-Cheol Park, Ok-Jin Jang, Hyung-Jun Yoon, and Seung-Ho Jang.

  • Software: Seon-Cheol Park and Kiwon Kim.

  • Supervision: Sung-Wan Kim, Bong Ju Lee, Jae Hong Park, Kang Uk Lee, and Joonho Choi.

  • Validation: Sung-Wan Kim, Bong Ju Lee, Jae Hong Park, Kang Uk Lee, and Joonho Choi.

  • Visualization: Seon-Cheol Park, Ok-Jin Jang, and Kiwon Kim.

  • Writing—original draft: Seon-Cheol Park.

  • Writing—review & editing: Seon-Cheol Park, Hyung-Jun Yoon, Ok-Jin Jang, and Seung-Ho Jang.

  • Approval of final manuscript: all authors.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This work was supported by the 2019 an Inje University Research Grant. We would like to thank Dr. Jinseob Kim for his valuable help with the network analysis.

References

    1. Park YC, Lee MS, Si TM, Chiu HFK, Kanba S, Chong MY, et al. Psychotropic drug-prescribing correlates of disorganized speech in Asians with schizophrenia: the REAP-AP study. Saudi Pharm J 2019;27:246–253.
    1. Roche E, Creed L, MacMahon D, Brennan D, Clarke M. The epidemiology and associated phenomenology of formal thought disorder: a systematic review. Schizophr Bull 2015;41:951–962.
    1. Crow TJ. The ‘big bang’ theory of the origin of psychosis and the faculty of language. Schizophr Res 2008;102:31–52.
    1. Ceccherini-Nelli A, Turpin-Crowther K, Crow TJ. Schneider's first rank symptoms and continuous performance disturbance as indices of dysconnectivity of left- and right-hemispheric components of language in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2007;90:203–213.
    1. Borsboom D. A network theory of mental disorders. World Psychiatry 2017;16:5–13.
    1. McNally RJ, Robinaugh DJ, Wu GWY, Wang L, Deserno MK, Borsboom D. Mental disorders as causal systems: a network approach to posttraumatic stress disorder. Clin Psychol Sci 2015;3:836–849.
    1. Park SC, Jang EY, Lee KU, Lee JG, Lee HY, Choi J. Psychometric properties of the Korean version of the Clinical Language Disorder Rating Scale (CLANG). Clin Psychopharmacol Neurosci 2016;14:49–56.
    1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.
    1. Cohen MJ, Stanczak DE. On the reliability, validity, and cognitive structure of the Thurstone Word Fluency Test. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2000;15:267–279.
    1. Chen EYH, Lam LCW, Kan CS, Chan CKY, Kwok CL, Nguyen DGH, et al. Language disorganization in schizophrenia: validation and assessment with a new clinical rating instrument. Hong Kong J Psychiatry 1996;6:4–13.
    1. Epskamp S, Cramer AOJ, Waldorp LJ, Schmittmann VD, Borsboom D. qgraph: network visualizations of relationships in psychometric data. J Stat Softw 2012;48:4
    1. Furukawa TA, Levine SZ, Tanaka S, Goldberg Y, Samara M, Davis JM, et al. Initial severity of schizophrenia and efficacy of antipsychotics: participant-level meta-analysis of 6 placebo-controlled studies. JAMA Psychiatry 2015;72:14–21.
    1. Tibshirani R. Regression shrinkage and selection via the lasso. J R Stat Soc Ser B 1996;58:267–288.
    1. van Borkulo C, Boschloo L, Borsboom D, Penninx BW, Waldorp LJ, Schoevers RA. Association of symptom network structure with the course of depression. JAMA Psychiatry 2015;72:1219–1226.
    1. Cohen J. In: Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Revised ed. New York: Academic Press; 1977.
    1. Reichardt J, Bornholdt S. Statistical mechanics of community detection. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys 2006;74:016110
    1. Csardi G, Nepusz T. The igraph software package for complex network research. Inter J Complex Syst 2006:CX.18:1695
      [Preprint].
    1. Woods SW. Chlorpromazine equivalent doses for the newer atypical antipsychotics. J Clin Psychiatry 2003;64:663–667.
    1. Kuperberg GR. Building meaning in schizophrenia. Clin EEG Neurosci 2008;39:99–102.
    1. Stephane M, Kuskowski M, Gundel J. Abnormal dynamics of language in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 2014;216:320–324.
    1. Peters AS, Rémi J, Vollmar C, Gonzalez-Victores JA, Cunha JP, Noachtar S. Dysprosody during epileptic seizures lateralizes to the nondominant hemisphere. Neurology 2011;77:1482–1486.
    1. Kirov K. Studies of the course of schizophrenia. Psychopathology 1990;23:339–341.

Metrics
Share
Figures

1 / 1

Tables

1 / 1

Funding Information
PERMALINK