Skip to content
BY-NC-ND 3.0 license Open Access Published by De Gruyter Open Access September 17, 2014

Posterior parietal cortex and visuospatial control in near and far space

  • Indra Mahayana EMAIL logo , Hartono , Lili Tcheang , Chiao-Yun Chen , Chi-Hung Juan and Neil Muggleton

Abstract

Neuropsychological studies of patients with visuospatial neglect have shown differences in perceptual deficits for information in near space (i.e. near to the body) and information in far space. It has been suggested that among the many areas of the human brain, a number of areas are associated with a set of spatial maps specialized for visuospatial control related to this spatial distinction. This paper reviews how parietal cortex is thought to be involved in visuospatial neglect in relation to its control of visuospatial attention in the left and right visual fields and at different viewing distances. In particular, the importance of regions of the parietal cortex in the pathogenesis of neglect and in spatial attention and perception is discussed. Parietal cortex may control different distributions of attention across space by allocating specific attentional resources in near and far space while also showing attentional asymmetry across visual fields. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) as a technique offers the advantage of examining the direct behavioral effect of disruption of many of these areas with excellent temporal and spatial resolution. We discuss the use of TMS and the insights it may offer regarding the roles of these areas in neglect as well as normal visuospatial perception.

[1] Driver J., Vuilleumier P., Perceptual awareness and its loss in unilateral neglect and extinction, Cognition, 2001, 79, 39–88 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00124-410.1016/S0010-0277(00)00124-4Search in Google Scholar

[2] Robertson I.H., Mattingley J.B., Rorden C., Driver J., Phasic alerting of neglect patients overcomes their spatial deficit in visual awareness, Nature, 1998, 395, 169–172 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/2599310.1038/25993Search in Google Scholar

[3] Eglin M., Robertson L.C., Knight R.T., Visual search performance in the neglect syndrome, J. Cogn. Neurosci., 1989, 1, 372–386 http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1989.1.4.37210.1162/jocn.1989.1.4.372Search in Google Scholar

[4] Marotta J., McKeeff T., Behrmann M., Hemispatial neglect: its effects on visual perception and visually guided grasping, Neuropsychologia, 2003, 41, 1262–1271 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00038-110.1016/S0028-3932(03)00038-1Search in Google Scholar

[5] Halligan P.W., Fink G., Marshall J., Vallar G., Spatial cognition: evidence from visual neglect, Trends Cogn. Sci., 2003, 7, 125–133 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00032-910.1016/S1364-6613(03)00032-9Search in Google Scholar

[6] Kravitz D.J., Saleem K.S., Baker C.I., Mishkin M., A new neural framework for visuospatial processing, Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 2011, 12, 217–230 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn300810.1038/nrn3008Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[7] Doricchi F., de Schotten M., Tomaiuolo F., Bartolomeo P., White matter (dis)connections and gray matter (dys)functions in visual neglect: gaining insights into the brain networks of spatial awareness, Cortex, 2008, 30, 1–13 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.03.006Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[8] Saj A., Verdon V., Vocat R., Vuilleumier P., The anatomy underlying acute versus chronic spatial neglect’ also depends on clinical tests, Brain, 2012, 135, 1–5 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awr22710.1093/brain/awr227Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[9] Bird C., Malhotra P., Parton A., Coulthard E., Rushworth M., Husain M., Visual neglect after right posterior cerebral artery infarction, J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry, 2006, 77, 1008–1012 http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2006.09441710.1136/jnnp.2006.094417Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[10] Thiebaut de Schotten M., Urbanski M., Duffau H., Volle E., Levy R., Dubois B., et al., Direct evidence for a parietal-frontal pathway subserving spatial awareness in humans, Science, 2005, 309, 2226–2228 http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.111625110.1126/science.1116251Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[11] Kircher T.T., Senior C., Phillips M.L., Benson P.J., Bullmore E.T., Brammer M., et al., Towards a functional neuroanatomy of self processing: effects of faces and words, Cogn. Brain Res., 2000, 10, 133–144 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00036-710.1016/S0926-6410(00)00036-7Search in Google Scholar

[12] Clower D.M., Hoffman J.M., Votaw J.R., Faber T.L., Woods R.P., Alexander G.E., Role of posterior parietal cortex in the recalibration of visually guided reaching, Nature, 1996, 383, 618–621 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/383618a010.1038/383618a0Search in Google Scholar

[13] Driver J., Mattingley J.B., Parietal neglect and visual awareness, Nat. Neurosci., 1998, 1, 17–22 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/21710.1038/217Search in Google Scholar

[14] Milner A., Cerebral correlates of visual awareness, Neuropsychologia, 1995, 33, 1117–1130 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(95)00052-510.1016/0028-3932(95)00052-5Search in Google Scholar

[15] Vuilleumier P., Sagiv N., Hazeltine E., Poldrack R.A., Swick D., Rafal R.D., et al., Neural fate of seen and unseen faces in visuospatial neglect: a combined event-related functional MRI and event-related potential study, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 2001, 98, 3495–3500 http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.05143689810.1073/pnas.051436898Search in Google Scholar

[16] Zoccolotti P., Judica A., Functional evaluation of hemineglect by means of a semistructured scale: personal extrapersonal differentiation, Neuropsychol. Rehabil., 1991, 1, 33–44 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0960201910840137810.1080/09602019108401378Search in Google Scholar

[17] Colby C.L., Goldberg M.E., Space and attention in parietal cortex, Annu. Rev. Neurosci., 1999, 22, 319–349 http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.22.1.31910.1146/annurev.neuro.22.1.319Search in Google Scholar

[18] Ellison A., Rushworth M., Walsh V., The parietal cortex in visual search: a visuomotor hypothesis, J. Clin. Neurophysiol., 2003, 56, 321–330 10.1016/S1567-424X(09)70236-3Search in Google Scholar

[19] Lamme V.A., Why visual attention and awareness are different, Trends Cogn. Sci., 2003, 7, 12–18 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00013-X10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00013-XSearch in Google Scholar

[20] Ptak R., Fellrath J., Spatial neglect and the neural coding of attentional priority, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 2013, 37, 705–722 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.02610.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.026Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[21] Rees G., Lavie N., What can functional imaging reveal about the role of attention in visual awareness?, Neuropsychologia, 2001, 39, 1343–1353 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00122-110.1016/S0028-3932(01)00122-1Search in Google Scholar

[22] Halligan P.W., Marshall J.C., Left neglect for near but not far space in man, Nature, 1991, 350, 498–450 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/350498a010.1038/350498a0Search in Google Scholar

[23] Weiss P.H., Marshall J.C., Wunderlich G., Tellmann L., Halligan P.W., Neural consequenses of acting in near versus far space: a physiological basis for clinical dissociations, Brain, 2000, 123, 2531–2541 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/123.12.253110.1093/brain/123.12.2531Search in Google Scholar

[24] Butler B.C., Lawrence M., Eskes G.A., Klein R., Visual search patterns in neglect: Comparison of peripersonal and extrapersonal space, Neuropsychologia, 2009, 47, 869–878 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.12.02010.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.12.020Search in Google Scholar

[25] Guariglia C., Antonucci G., Personal and extrapersonal space: a case of neglect dissociation, Neuropsychologia, 1992, 30, 1002–1009 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(92)90051-M10.1016/0028-3932(92)90051-MSearch in Google Scholar

[26] Bjoertomt O., Cowey A., Walsh V., Spatial neglect in near and far space investigated by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation, Brain, 2002, 125, 2012–2022 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awf21110.1093/brain/awf211Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[27] Heber I.A., Siebertz S., Wolter M., Kuhlen T., Fimm B., Horizontal and vertical pseudoneglect in peri- and extrapersonal space, Brain Cogn., 2010, 73, 160–166 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2010.04.00610.1016/j.bandc.2010.04.006Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[28] Lane A.R., Ball K., Smith D.T., Schenk T., Ellison A., Near and far space: understanding the neural mechanisms of spatial attention, Hum. Brain Mapp., 2013, 34, 356–366 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.2143310.1002/hbm.21433Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[29] Mesulam M.M., Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events, Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B, 1999, 354, 1325–1346 http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1999.048210.1098/rstb.1999.0482Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[30] Giglia G., Mattaliano P., Puma A., Rizzo S., Fierro B., Brighina F., Neglectlike effects induced by tDCS modulation of posterior parietal cortices in healthy subjects, Brain Stimul., 2011, 4, 294–299 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2011.01.00310.1016/j.brs.2011.01.003Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[31] McCourt M., Garlinghouse M., Reuterlorenz P., Unilateral visual cueing and asymmetric line geometry share a common attentional origin in the modulation of pseudoneglect, Cortex, 2005, 41, 499–511 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70190-410.1016/S0010-9452(08)70190-4Search in Google Scholar

[32] Herwig U., Satrapi P., Schönfeldt-Lecuona C., Using the international 10–20 EEG system for positioning of transcranial magnetic stimulation, Brain Topogr., 2003, 16, 95–99 http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/B:BRAT.0000006333.93597.9d10.1023/B:BRAT.0000006333.93597.9dSearch in Google Scholar

[33] Muggleton N.G., Postma P., Moutsopoulou K., Nimmo-Smith I., Marcel A., Walsh V., TMS over right posterior parietal cortex induces neglect in a scene-based frame of reference, Neuropsychologia, 2006, 44, 1222–1229 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.00410.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.004Search in Google Scholar

[34] Nixon P., Lazarova J., Hodinott-Hill I., Gough P., Passingham R., The inferior frontal gyrus and phonological processing: an investigation using rTMS, J. Cogn. Neurosci., 2004, 16, 289–300 http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/08989290432298457110.1162/089892904322984571Search in Google Scholar

[35] Ashbridge E., Walsh V., Cowey A., Temporal aspects of visual search studied by transcranial magnetic stimulation, Neuropsychologia, 1997, 35, 1121–1131 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00003-110.1016/S0028-3932(97)00003-1Search in Google Scholar

[36] Molenberghs P., Sale M.V., Testing for spatial neglect with line bisection and target cancellation: are both tasks really unrelated?, PLoS One, 2011, 6, e23017 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.002301710.1371/journal.pone.0023017Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[37] Serino A., Canzoneri E., Avenanti A., Fronto-parietal areas necessary for a multisensory representation of peripersonal space in humans: an rTMS study, J. Cogn. Neurosci., 2011, 23, 2956–2967 http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_0000610.1162/jocn_a_00006Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[38] Mahayana I.T., Liu C.-L., Chang C.F., Hung D.L., Tzeng O., Juan C.-H., et al., Far-space neglect in conjunction but not feature search following transcranial magnetic stimulation over right posterior parietal cortex, J. Neurophysiol., 2014, DOI: 10.1152/jn.00492.2013, Epub ahead of print 10.1152/jn.00492.2013Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[39] Dvorkin A.Y., Bogey R.A., Harvey R.L., Patton J.L., Mapping the neglected space: gradients of detection revealed by virtual reality, Neurorehabil. Neural Repair, 2012, 26, 120–131 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/154596831141006810.1177/1545968311410068Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[40] Göbel S.M., Calabria M., Farnè A., Rossetti Y., Parietal rTMS distorts the mental number line: simulating ‘spatial’ neglect in healthy subjects, Neuropsychologia, 2006, 44, 860–868 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.00710.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.007Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[41] Sack A.T., Using non-invasive brain interference as a tool for mimicking spatial neglect in healthy volunteers, Restor. Neurol. Neurosci., 2010, 28, 485–497 10.3233/RNN-2010-0568Search in Google Scholar

[42] Berti A., Frassinetti F., When far becomes near: remapping of space by tool use, J. Cogn. Neurosci., 2000, 12, 415–420 http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/08989290056223710.1162/089892900562237Search in Google Scholar

[43] Vuilleumier P., Valenza N., Mayer E., Reverdin A., Landis T., Near and far visual space in unilateral neglect, Ann. Neurol., 1998, 43, 403–410 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ana.41043032410.1002/ana.410430324Search in Google Scholar

[44] Mahayana I.T., Tcheang L., Chen C.-Y., Juan C.-H., Muggleton N.G., The precuneus and visuospatial attention in near and far space: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study, Brain Stimul., 2014, pii, S1935-861X(14)00192-2 Search in Google Scholar

[45] Szczepanski S.M., Kastner S., Shifting attentional priorities: control of spatial attention through hemispheric competition, J. Neurosci., 2013, 33, 5411–5421 http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4089-12.201310.1523/JNEUROSCI.4089-12.2013Search in Google Scholar

[46] Ellison A., Schindler I., Pattison L., Milner A., An exploration of the role of the superior temporal gyrus in visual search and spatial perception using TMS, Brain, 2004, 127, 2307–2315 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh24410.1093/brain/awh244Search in Google Scholar

[47] Molenberghs P., Sale M.V., Mattingley J.B., Is there a critical lesion site for unilateral spatial neglect? A meta-analysis using activation likelihood estimation, Front. Hum. Neurosci., 2012, 6, 78 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.0007810.3389/fnhum.2012.00078Search in Google Scholar

[48] Thimm M., Fink G.R., Kust J., Karbe H., Willmes K., Sturm W., Recovery from hemineglect: differential neurobiological effects of optokinetic stimulation and alertness training, Cortex, 2009, 45, 850–862 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2008.10.00710.1016/j.cortex.2008.10.007Search in Google Scholar

[49] Thimm M., Fink G.R., Kust J., Karbe H., Sturm W., Impact of alertness training on spatial neglect: a behavioural and fMRI study, Neuropsychologia, 2006, 44, 1230–1246 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.00810.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.008Search in Google Scholar

[50] Weiss P.H., Marshall J.C., Zilles K., Fink G.R., Are action and perception in near and far space additive or interactive factors?, Neuroimage, 2003, 18, 837–846 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00018-110.1016/S1053-8119(03)00018-1Search in Google Scholar

[51] Cowey A., Small M., Ellis S., Left visuo-spatial neglect can be worse in far than in near space, Neuropsychogia, 1994, 32, 1059–1066 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(94)90152-X10.1016/0028-3932(94)90152-XSearch in Google Scholar

[52] Cowey A., Small M., Ellis S., No abrupt change in visual hemineglect from near to far space, Neuropsychologia, 1999, 37, 1–6 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00071-210.1016/S0028-3932(98)00071-2Search in Google Scholar

[53] Berti A., Rabuffetti M., Ferrarin M., D’Amico A., Smania N., Spinazzola L., et al., Coding of far and near space during walking in neglect patients, Neuropsychology, 2002, 16, 390–399 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.16.3.39010.1037/0894-4105.16.3.390Search in Google Scholar

[54] Medendorp W.P., Crawford J.D., Visuospatial updating of reaching targets in near and far space, Neuroreport, 2002, 13, 633–636 http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200204160-0001910.1097/00001756-200204160-00019Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[55] Medendorp W.P., Goltz H.C., Vilis T., Crawford J.D., Gaze-centered updating of visual space in human parietal cortex, J. Neurosci., 2003, 23, 6209–6214 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-15-06209.2003Search in Google Scholar

Published Online: 2014-9-17
Published in Print: 2014-12-1

© 2014 Versita Warsaw

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 3.0 License.

Downloaded on 18.4.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.2478/s13380-014-0229-3/html
Scroll to top button