Skip to content
BY 4.0 license Open Access Published by De Gruyter July 14, 2023

Role of the basal ganglia in innate and learned behavioural sequences

  • Natalia Favila ORCID logo EMAIL logo , Kevin Gurney and Paul G. Overton ORCID logo

Abstract

Integrating individual actions into coherent, organised behavioural units, a process called chunking, is a fundamental, evolutionarily conserved process that renders actions automatic. In vertebrates, evidence points to the basal ganglia – a complex network believed to be involved in action selection – as a key component of action sequence encoding, although the underlying mechanisms are only just beginning to be understood. Central pattern generators control many innate automatic behavioural sequences that form some of the most basic behaviours in an animal’s repertoire, and in vertebrates, brainstem and spinal pattern generators are under the control of higher order structures such as the basal ganglia. Evidence suggests that the basal ganglia play a crucial role in the concatenation of simpler behaviours into more complex chunks, in the context of innate behavioural sequences such as chain grooming in rats, as well as sequences in which innate capabilities and learning interact such as birdsong, and sequences that are learned from scratch, such as lever press sequences in operant behaviour. It has been proposed that the role of the striatum, the largest input structure of the basal ganglia, might lie in selecting and allowing the relevant central pattern generators to gain access to the motor system in the correct order, while inhibiting other behaviours. As behaviours become more complex and flexible, the pattern generators seem to become more dependent on descending signals. Indeed, during learning, the striatum itself may adopt the functional characteristics of a higher order pattern generator, facilitated at the microcircuit level by striatal neuropeptides.

1 Introduction

Performing most behavioural patterns requires executing sequences of actions with some degree of order. From pressing a lever, to making a cup of tea, behavioural patterns tend to group themselves into units that are performed in a fluent and seemingly effortless way. How individual actions are integrated into coherent and organised behavioural units, a process called chunking, is an important focus in psychology and neuroscience (Buxton et al. 2017; Drummond 1981; Graybiel and Grafton 2015; Jin et al. 2014). The term chunking was established by Miller (1956) in his classical experiments on memory, in which he found that a single item of information could be formed by a chunk of several items. A chunk has been defined as “a collection of elements having strong associations with one another, but weak associations with elements within other chunks” (Gobet et al. 2001). Being able to represent information in this way is believed to be a fundamental cognitive mechanism, since it presumably alleviates the cognitive and memory load of any system trying to store and process large amounts of information (Solopchuk et al. 2016; Veksler et al. 2014).

In the motor domain, having a mechanism that allows the storage of sequences of actions as integrated units has been suggested as an efficient way of processing the large repertoire of behaviours that an animal can acquire throughout its lifetime. Indeed, it is known that once a motor sequence is learned, its performance is rendered faster and automatic, suggesting a reduction in the cognitive load associated to its performance (Dezfouli and Balleine 2012; Sakai et al. 2003; Savalia et al. 2016; Smith and Graybiel 2016). In vertebrates, evidence points to the basal ganglia – a complex network believed to be involved in action selection – as a key system implicated in action sequence encoding (Graybiel 1998; Jin and Costa 2015).

2 The basal ganglia circuit

The basal ganglia are a group of subcortical nuclei that have been found to be important in motor, cognitive and emotional domains. Dysfunction of these nuclei has been associated with a diverse range of disorders, from motor disorders, such as Parkinson’s disease (PD) and Huntington’s disease, to cognitive disorders, such as obsessive-compulsive disorder and other forms of compulsive behaviours (DeLong 1990; Graybiel 2000). To understand the role of the basal ganglia in behaviour it is important to begin with a short review of its main nuclei and connections.

The basal ganglia consist of six main nuclei (see Figure 1). The striatum is its largest structure and its main input nucleus, receiving substantial inputs from all over the brain, mainly from the cortex and the thalamus, but also from other structures, such as the globus pallidus and substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc) (Guo et al. 2015; Wall et al. 2013). Cortical inputs from motor, sensory and frontal areas either extend no further than the striatum (intratelencephalic) or in the case of neurons contributing to the pyramidal tracts, innervate the striatum via collaterals of axons that extend beyond the telencephalon, projecting to the thalamus, superior colliculus and brainstem, amongst others (Shepherd 2013). Intratelencephalic projections from different cortical areas show substantial overlap within the striatum, while pyramidal tract neurons display more focal projections to striatal subregions, putting the striatum in a privileged position to integrate information from several cortical areas while still maintaining some segregation in the information (Hooks et al. 2018). Another important source of inputs comes from the thalamus, representing approximately 50 % of the glutamatergic innervation to striatum (Doig et al. 2010), with the highest density coming from the parafascicular nucleus (PF) (Mandelbaum et al. 2019). Cortical and thalamic inputs mainly target GABAergic medium spiny neurons (MSNs), which comprise around 90–95 % of the striatum’s neuronal population. The remaining 5 % consists of different types of interneurons, such as cholinergic and GABAergic neurons, which, although a small proportion, have been found to receive inputs from both within and outside the striatum (Silberberg and Bolam 2015) and to play an important role in behavioural output (e.g. Aoki et al. 2015; O’Hare et al. 2017).

Striatal MSNs have been divided into two populations that create two semi-independent pathways referred to as the direct and indirect pathways. In the classical view, the direct pathway is formed of MSNs that mainly express D1-type dopamine receptors and directly project to the substantia nigra pars reticulata (SNr) and to the globus pallidus internal section (GPi), the output nuclei of the basal ganglia. On the other hand, MSNs of the indirect pathway express mainly D2-type dopamine receptors and they indirectly project to the output nuclei, mainly through the globus pallidus external section (GPe) and the subthalamic nucleus (STN), which are reciprocally connected. The STN and the GPe are both connected to the basal ganglia output nuclei by excitatory and inhibitory connections, respectively (Dong et al. 2021). Finally, the GPi and SNr project to the thalamus – with densest projections to the PF and ventromedial thalamic nuclei – which in turn projects back to the cortex creating parallel, re-entrant, topographically organized closed loops, where body subregions such as upper limb, mouth and trunk, are represented separately (Bolam et al. 2000; Foster et al. 2021; Wickens 1997; see Figure 1). In addition to these widely documented basal ganglia – thalamocortical loops, a parallel series of subcortical loops have been identified, connecting the basal ganglia and the brainstem. These loops run through the basal ganglia, outputting via the SNr back to the input structures, such as the superior colliculus and periaqueductal grey (McHaffie et al. 2005). Alternatively, shorter loops between the SNr and the brainstem have recently been identified (Al Tannir et al. 2023), and it has been shown that the SNr projects to over 40 brainstem targets in a topographically organized fashion, giving the basal ganglia direct access to a wide range of brainstem effector systems (McElvain et al. 2021). Thus, this subcortical looped architecture has the capability to control a wide range of behaviourally relevant functions independently of, or in concert with, those arising from the cortex/thalamus. For example, recent evidence suggests that the basal ganglia, in particular the SNr, exerts direct control over glutamatergic neurons in the midbrain locomotor region (Roseberry et al. 2016). Overall, the basal ganglia’s differentiated control of targets in the brainstem and in the cortex, where evidence suggests that the loops through the basal ganglia are themselves divided into numerous topographically organized sub-loops, suggests that the basal ganglia exert highly specific control over diverse behavioural domains (Alexander and Crutcher 1990; Foster et al. 2021).

Figure 1: 
Simplified diagram showing the connections between the main basal ganglia nuclei. The striatum is the main input nucleus, receiving inputs from cortex and thalamus. The basal ganglia network (within the pink square) is classically divided into the direct pathway, which comprises of medium spiny neurons (MSNs) that project directly from the striatum to the substantia nigra pars reticulata (SNr) and globus pallidus internal part (GPi); and the indirect pathway, which comprises of MSNs that project to the output nuclei through connections with the globus pallidus external part (GPe) and the subthalamic nucleus (STN). A hyperdirect pathway connects the cortex with the STN and the SNR/GPi, and the SNr/GPi both project to the thalamus and brainstem, as well as the habenula in the case of the GPi. A loop links the GPe and STN, and a return pathway has been identified from the GPe to the striatum, originating from a population of neurons referred to as arkypallidal cells. There is also a direct connection from GPe to the SNR/GPi, and from the thalamus to the striatum. Finally, the substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc) sends a dopaminergic projection to the striatum that is excitatory at D1-type dopamine receptor expressing MSNs and inhibitory at D2-type dopamine receptor expressing MSNs. Excitatory connections are in red and inhibitory connections are in blue.
Figure 1:

Simplified diagram showing the connections between the main basal ganglia nuclei. The striatum is the main input nucleus, receiving inputs from cortex and thalamus. The basal ganglia network (within the pink square) is classically divided into the direct pathway, which comprises of medium spiny neurons (MSNs) that project directly from the striatum to the substantia nigra pars reticulata (SNr) and globus pallidus internal part (GPi); and the indirect pathway, which comprises of MSNs that project to the output nuclei through connections with the globus pallidus external part (GPe) and the subthalamic nucleus (STN). A hyperdirect pathway connects the cortex with the STN and the SNR/GPi, and the SNr/GPi both project to the thalamus and brainstem, as well as the habenula in the case of the GPi. A loop links the GPe and STN, and a return pathway has been identified from the GPe to the striatum, originating from a population of neurons referred to as arkypallidal cells. There is also a direct connection from GPe to the SNR/GPi, and from the thalamus to the striatum. Finally, the substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc) sends a dopaminergic projection to the striatum that is excitatory at D1-type dopamine receptor expressing MSNs and inhibitory at D2-type dopamine receptor expressing MSNs. Excitatory connections are in red and inhibitory connections are in blue.

This means that the direct and indirect pathways of the basal ganglia can modulate behaviour through both cortical and subcortical loops. These two pathways are believed to have an important role in action selection (Cui et al. 2013; Graybiel 2000; Redgrave et al. 1999). The basic idea behind the classical model is that when MSNs of the direct pathway are activated, they directly inhibit the output nuclei, SNr and GPi, and given that these nuclei in turn exert tonic inhibition over their thalamic targets, activation of direct pathway MSNs ends up releasing the thalamus and the cortex from inhibition, facilitating behaviour. On the other hand, activation of the indirect pathway leads to inhibition of the GPe, which, given its inhibitory projections to the STN, releases the excitatory STN input to the output nuclei. Thus, activation of the indirect pathway ends up increasing the activity of the basal ganglia output nuclei, increasing inhibition over thalamus and cortex, inhibiting behaviour (Albin et al. 1989; DeLong 1990). Behavioural inhibition is further effected via two additional pathways: (1) by a specialised class of inhibitory neurons in the GPe, the arkypallidal cells, which project back to the striatum and hence are able to suppress behaviour (Mallet et al. 2016); and (2) through a class of GPe neurons that directly inhibit the SNr (Dong et al. 2021). With respect to the direct and indirect pathways, presumably the existence of subcortical loops requires the classical model to be extended such that the direct and indirect pathways exert their control over the brainstem via direct projections from the SNr (Al Tannir et al. 2023).

The striatum also receives a substantial dopaminergic input from the SNc and ventral tegmental area, and dopamine has a robust effect over striatal activity (Fisher et al. 2017; Matsuda et al. 2009). Dopaminergic afferents reach the striatum primarily at the level of the dendritic spines and shafts of MSNs, converging in many cases with glutamatergic cortical afferents (Freund et al. 1984), thus, dopamine is in a privileged position to modulate cortico-striatal synapses (Reynolds and Wickens 2002; Wickens 1997). The effects of dopamine on the striatum have been found to be manifold, depending on the area of the striatum, the activation of specific dopamine receptors and on the pre and postsynaptic firing pattern (Reynolds and Wickens 2002). Normally, low levels of dopamine, pre (cortical) and post (striatal) synaptic activity will cause long-term depression (LTD) (Calabressi et al. 1992). However, the timing and pattern of the dopamine released plays a key role, thus, it has been reported that if dopamine is released in a high frequency phasic manner, at the same time that cortical and striatal activity are present, long-term potentiation (LTP) is induced at cortico-striatal synapses (Reynolds and Wickens 2002; Wickens et al. 1996).

In summary, activation of the direct pathway disinhibits the thalamus and the brainstem, and thus its main role has been suggested to be to allow the expression of behaviours, whereas activation of the indirect pathway increases inhibition over the thalamus and the brainstem, thus, decreasing behavioural expression, with dopamine playing a key role in modulating striatal output. When it comes to learning and executing sequential behavioural patterns, although it is known that patients with disorders such as PD, in which the striatum is severely affected, display disrupted sequencing and automaticity of actions (Casarrubea et al. 2019; Harrington and Haaland 1991; Tremblay et al. 2010), the underlying mechanisms responsible for encoding action sequences as units are only just beginning to be understood. In the following sections some of the main results obtained from innate and learned action sequences are reviewed.

3 Innate behavioural sequences

It has been argued that examining how action sequences are implemented in models of innate and seemingly simple behaviours, could help us elucidate how more complex behavioural patterns are assembled, given that many of the higher order processes that we observe in animals and humans are believed to be the result of modifications to innate behavioural mechanisms (Berridge and Whishaw 1992; Grillner and Wallen 2004). Fixed action patterns are classically defined as behavioural patterns that are (1) innate, that is, they have not been modified by learning, and (2) triggered by specific stimuli, both external (e.g. the presence of an object) and internal (e.g. the release of a hormone). The complexity of these fixed action patterns can vary from simple actions, like the Greylag goose retrieving eggs back to its nest, to elaborate sequences, such as the mating dance of the three-spined stickleback (Tinbergen 1951). Although it is now more accepted that some innate patterns can be modified to some extent by processes such as learning and sensory feedback (Grillner and Wallen 2004), using fixed action patterns as models to study action sequences has the advantage that any disruption found in their sequential implementation is minimally confounded by other cognitive processes. Thus, they give the opportunity to study the mechanisms behind sequential patterning in a relatively isolated preparation (Berridge and Whishaw 1992; Kalueff et al. 2007). There are several examples of innate behaviours that have been used to study the serial order problem. In this review we will focus on three major categories: innate rhythmic behaviours, rodent grooming, and birdsong, all of which have been thoroughly studied.

3.1 Rhythmic behaviours

Many of the most basic behaviours that are in our behavioural repertoire, such as breathing, and others that do not seem so basic, such as walking, are formed of rhythmic sequences of movements. Many of these patterns involve temporally organised sequences of muscle activations that are regulated subcortically by neural networks called central pattern generators (CPGs) (Bucher et al. 2015). CPGs are neural networks that control many innate automatic behavioural sequences that form some of the most basic behaviours in an animal’s repertoire, something that has been called the motor infrastructure (Grillner and Wallen 2004). CPGs are present in many species, both in invertebrates, controlling behaviours such as crawling in leeches and swimming in molluscs (Cacciatore et al. 2000; Sakurai and Katz 2016); and in vertebrates, underlying locomotion and other behaviours in several species, such as lampreys, zebrafish, turtles and cats, to mention a few (Berkowitz et al. 2010; Marder 2000). Although most studies of CPGs have been performed on small invertebrates and lower vertebrates, due to their reduced number of neurons, several shared characteristics across many species have been found (Bass 2014; Grillner and Wallen 2002; Grillner et al. 2005).

The main characteristic of CPGs is that they can generate continuous rhythmic activity without external tonic timing inputs or sensory feedback (Marder and Bucher 2001; Satterlie 1985). Thus, as their name indicates, their pattern of activity can be produced intrinsically, both by synaptic connections and through neuromodulation (Bucher et al. 2015; Marder and Bucher 2001). This means that the basic behavioural patterns controlled by CPGs can still be found after deafferentation. For example, both leeches and cats can still produce somewhat normal coordinated locomotion after deafferentation and, in the case of leeches, even after complete decerebration (Cacciatore et al. 2000; Frigon and Grossard 2010).

Although sensory feedback is not necessary for the production of the basic activity pattern, depending on the behaviour, some characteristics of the CPG’s dynamics do depend on sensory feedback to different degrees. For example, the rhythmic wing sequence of movements executed by some insects to fly can still be produced after sensory deafferentation, but it is considerably slowed down and the inter-segmental coordination is affected (Pearson and Wolf 1987). Other CPGs, like the ones controlling swimming in the crayfish, can still produce basic rhythmicity and coordination after all sensory afferents have been cut (Hughes and Wiersma 1960). The dependency on sensory feedback is believed to be subject to how stable or variable the environment was in which the behaviours evolved (Cacciatore et al. 2000).

3.2 Neural bases of CPGs

A CPG unit has been described as “a group of neurons that can generate recurrent bursts” (Grillner 2006). This bursting activity pattern in vertebrates arises from relatively simple designs formed mostly of motoneurons and glutamatergic (excitatory) and glycinergic (inhibitory) interneurons located in the brainstem and spinal cord (Grillner 2003). There are two main neural mechanisms by which a CPG network is able to produce rhythmic sequential activity. First, some CPGs have neurons with intrinsic oscillatory properties, referred to as “pacemakers”, which are able to impose rhythm on a network that by itself does not burst periodically (Marder and Bucher 2001). These types of networks usually control behaviours where the rhythm needs to be present for prolonged times or even at all times, such as respiration in mammals and swimming in jellyfish (Marder and Bucher 2001; Rekling and Feldman 1998; Satterlie and Nolan 2001).

Nonetheless, it is more common that the patterned activity of a CPG is the result of its synaptic interactions, and that the resulting behaviour can be started and stopped at will, as in locomotion, which can be initiated and ended in a goal-directed manner (Grillner 2006; Grillner and Wallen 2004). Thus, in cases where there are no pacemaker neurons, the rhythmic activity of the networks emerges from the connections between its neurons and the descending afferents that reach the CPGs (Bucher et al. 2015; Satterlie 1985). In general terms, it is believed that CPG reconfiguration depends significantly on neuro-modulatory projections, both from descending projections and from sensory feedback (Marder 2000; Ramakrishnan et al. 2014). The basal ganglia output nuclei send projections to brainstem nuclei, which project back to the striatum through the thalamus, thus, putting the basal ganglia in a position to modulate the selection of motor plans encoded in brainstem and spinal CPGs via descending commands (Grillner et al. 2005; McHaffie et al. 2005; see Figure 2).

Figure 2: 
Central pattern generator (CPG) selection. The cortex in mammals and the corresponding structures in lower vertebrates (pallium), alongside the thalamus, provide an excitatory (red) input to striatum. The striatum consists primarily of inhibitory (blue) GABAergic neurons, which in turn inhibit the output nuclei of the basal ganglia (pallidum, and in mammals, the substantia nigra pars reticulata [not shown]), as part of the basal ganglia’s direct pathway. The output nuclei again consist of GABAergic neurons, and their high level of baseline activity keeps CPGs elsewhere in the nervous system under tonic inhibition. This is released when the striatum is activated and inhibits the output nuclei, resulting in a disinhibition of the CPGs. Dopamine (DA) affects the responsiveness of striatal neurons. Reproduced with permission from Grillner et al. (2006).
Figure 2:

Central pattern generator (CPG) selection. The cortex in mammals and the corresponding structures in lower vertebrates (pallium), alongside the thalamus, provide an excitatory (red) input to striatum. The striatum consists primarily of inhibitory (blue) GABAergic neurons, which in turn inhibit the output nuclei of the basal ganglia (pallidum, and in mammals, the substantia nigra pars reticulata [not shown]), as part of the basal ganglia’s direct pathway. The output nuclei again consist of GABAergic neurons, and their high level of baseline activity keeps CPGs elsewhere in the nervous system under tonic inhibition. This is released when the striatum is activated and inhibits the output nuclei, resulting in a disinhibition of the CPGs. Dopamine (DA) affects the responsiveness of striatal neurons. Reproduced with permission from Grillner et al. (2006).

In conclusion, CPGs are neuronal networks that control basic sequences of movements, and are able to sustain their basic firing pattern without external inputs, although, in some instances they can be modified by sensory feedback and descending influences, giving them some flexibility (Grillner 2006). Those descending influences, for example from the basal ganglia, are also in a position to select appropriate CPGs as required (Grillner et al. 2005; McHaffie et al. 2005). In the following section we will review grooming sequences, an innate behaviour that – although controlled by CPGs in the brainstem – its sequential patterning seems to come from the basal ganglia.

3.3 Grooming behaviour

Grooming behaviours, such as body licking, face washing and paw licking, are innate behaviours present in many species and can be rich in structure. Berridge et al. (1987) discovered that among the several behaviours that are performed within a grooming bout, rodents execute grooming chains with a very specific order, both spontaneously and triggered by certain stimuli (e.g. water on the rodent’s fur). These stereotypical grooming chains are present in many species – such as squirrels, guinea pigs, gerbils and hamsters – that differ up to 65 million years in their evolution, suggesting that the implementation of this patterned behaviour, and possibly the mechanism underlying it, is highly conserved (Berridge 1990).

The stereotypical grooming chain is different in each species of rodent. Nonetheless, in all of them, around four sequential phases with a hierarchical structure can be found (Berridge 1990). In rats, the grooming chain consists of four phases executed in a specific order, with a cephalo-caudal direction (Kalueff et al. 2007). The first phase consists of a series of very fast and small elliptical strokes around the nose. This is followed by a series of unilateral strokes around the mystacial vibrissae below the eye and a set of large bilateral and symmetrical strokes that usually go over the ears. The chain is finished when animals turn to their flank and begin to lick their body. The completion of the first three phases takes between 3 and 5 s, while the last element’s length can vary, lasting up to 30 s (Berridge 1990; Berridge et al. 1987; see Figure 3).

Figure 3: 
Phases of the grooming chain. Bottom: Drawings showing the four phases (A–D) of the grooming chain in their order of appearance; Top: The movement path of the forelimbs over time, showing the distance from midline (vertical dimension). The inset diagram to the left shows a rat’s face as viewed from below on the video monitor on which the distances were determined. The movements from midline are measured from the midline to the centre of the forepaw (Y dimension on this drawing), with the base of the vibrissae, eyes, and ears as landmarks. Reproduced with permission from Aldridge and Berridge (1998); © 1998 The Society for Neuroscience.
Figure 3:

Phases of the grooming chain. Bottom: Drawings showing the four phases (A–D) of the grooming chain in their order of appearance; Top: The movement path of the forelimbs over time, showing the distance from midline (vertical dimension). The inset diagram to the left shows a rat’s face as viewed from below on the video monitor on which the distances were determined. The movements from midline are measured from the midline to the centre of the forepaw (Y dimension on this drawing), with the base of the vibrissae, eyes, and ears as landmarks. Reproduced with permission from Aldridge and Berridge (1998); © 1998 The Society for Neuroscience.

It has been calculated that the appearance of this grooming chain is 13,000 times more likely than would be expected by chance, representing an exceptional case of serial order (Berridge et al. 1987). Thus, it is believed that the execution of the grooming chain is not the result of some random process, but rather the product of an active sequential mechanism. However, it is also possible to observe the behaviours that are part of the grooming chain performed in an unstructured way during grooming bouts. This has been used as a way to compare whether a treatment has an effect only in the context of the grooming chain or on the whole grooming bout (Berridge 1990). Importantly, the grooming chain is executed as a unit. Once the first element of the chain is performed, the probability that the rest of the elements will be completed in the same order is around 0.9 or higher (Berridge and Whishaw 1992). Therefore, the execution of the first element, the elliptical strokes, is usually taken as a reliable criterion to identify the presence of a grooming chain. The high degree of stereotypy and the fact that the elements of the chain are easily distinguishable, have made it a useful behavioural model to study the implementation of sequential organisation (Kalueff et al. 2007).

3.4 Neural substrate underlying the grooming chain

Berridge, Aldridge and collaborators exploited the grooming chain as a behavioural model to investigate the neural structures underlying the implementation of action sequences. Through different lesion and electrophysiological studies, they have found that the basal ganglia are a key network in the performance of this sequential innate behaviour. Accordingly, it has been found that disruptions in the execution of the grooming chain are present in animal models of Tourette’s syndrome (Taylor et al. 2010), obsessive compulsive disorder (Berridgem et al. 2005) and Huntington’s disease (Tartaglione et al. 2016), all of them disorders related to dysfunction of the basal ganglia.

From several studies, the striatum has emerged as a key region for the implementation of the sequential order of the grooming chain. Striatal damage decreases the probability of completing the grooming chain and increases its duration, without actually damaging the ability of the rats to perform each behaviour individually (Berridge and Fentress 1987b; Tartaglione et al. 2016). Lesions to brain structures known to have a role in motor control, such as the cerebellum, primary and secondary motor cortex or the entire neocortex, do not seem to produce any lasting effects on the sequential organisation of the grooming chain (Berridge and Whishaw 1992). Furthermore, lesions to other structures in the rat, such as globus pallidus (equivalent to the primate GPe) or ventral pallidum do not affect grooming chain serial performance either (Cromwell and Berridge 1996).

In more detailed studies, it has been found that only lesions in the anterior dorsolateral striatum (DLS) disrupt the serial execution of the grooming chain, with only around 24 % of the chains being completed correctly. Excitotoxic lesions in the dorsomedial, ventromedial or ventrolateral striatum do not disrupt its execution (Cromwell and Berridge 1996). This is interesting, given that the DLS has been related to the performance of habitual behaviours, suggesting a possible overlap of mechanisms (Yin and Knowlton 2006). Again, interestingly, sensory deafferentation of the face also has no effect on grooming chain performance, suggesting that the serial order of this behavioural pattern is not based on somatosensory feedback, but rather on some central mechanism possibly implemented or at least modulated by the striatum (Berridge and Fentress 1987a). This independence from sensory feedback and the fact that the order of the grooming chain seems to be independent of timing from cortical inputs has led to the suggestion that CPGs in the brainstem are modulated by the striatum, which contributes to the sequential pattern (Cromwell and Berridge 1996).

Electrophysiological studies have revealed that neurons in the DLS display higher firing rates when each of the chain behaviours are executed in the context of the ordered grooming chain than when these same behaviours are performed in an unordered fashion. This is not observed in the ventromedial striatum, where neuronal activity seems to be more strongly related to the initiation of the grooming sequence (Aldridge and Berridge 1998; Aldridge et al. 1993). Neurons in the SNr have also been found to show distinctive firing patterns according to whether the grooming behaviours are performed inside or outside the grooming chain. Neurons in SNr are excited during the initiation of the chain, and significantly more inhibited as the grooming chain is performed (Meyer-Luehmann et al. 2002). The distinctive increase in activity observed when the grooming chain is executed has also been observed in the execution of sequences that, although not as stereotyped, follow a certain order. Aldridge et al. (2004) analysed what they called the “warm-up sequence”, a sequence of behaviours that occurs when rats transition from periods of immobility to periods of movement. This sequence is composed of resting, head and torso movements and locomotion, and it tends to occur in this particular order, although not in such a fixed way as the grooming chain. Nevertheless, a similar increase of striatal activity is observed during its execution. Although this increased firing is at a lower level than the one observed in the grooming chain, these results suggest that the striatum could be involved in sequential action organisation in a general way.

Finally, damaging dopaminergic nigrostriatal projections can lead to a decrease in the percentage of correct completions of the grooming chain, suggesting a role for dopamine in sequence implementation (Berridge 1989; Pelosi et al. 2015). Accordingly, increasing dopamine levels in the mouse brain renders the performance of the grooming chain more rigid, making hyper-dopaminergic mice more resistant to disruptions of the chain (Berridge et al. 2005). Likewise, D1-type receptor agonists generate super-stereotypy in the grooming chain; and co-administration of D2-type receptor antagonists decrease these effects, suggesting that both D1-type and D2-type dopamine receptors are involved in the implementation of sequential stereotypy (Taylor et al. 2010).

In summary, studies carried out with the grooming chain as a model have suggested that the basal ganglia, and in particular the DLS, play a crucial role in the implementation of these sequential patterns, with increased firing rates only during ordered sequences both in striatum and SNr, with an important role for dopamine. It has been proposed that the role of the striatum might lie in selecting and allowing the CPGs in charge of the grooming chain to gain access to the motor system in the correct order, while inhibiting other behaviours (Berridge and Wishaw 1992). In the following section we will discuss some results that have been found in another innate sequential behaviour linked to the basal ganglia, birdsong, which unlike grooming, has a learning phase.

3.5 Birdsong

Many species of birds sing songs in order to reproduce and defend territory. These songs are arrays of complex sequences of syllables that display long-range correlations that can extend up to 10 s over time (Markowitz et al. 2013); thus, they have also been used as a behavioural model to study sequence codification. Much like the grooming chain, birdsongs are arrangements of syllables that are not random – they tend to follow a predictable order and they recurrently start and finish in the same way (Gil and Slater 2000). However, unlike other innate behaviours, birds go through a learning period before they crystallise their song structures, which has suggested that birdsong arose from the relaxation of an innate mechanism (Gardner et al. 2005).

The process of vocal learning in birds is usually divided into two general phases: (1) a sensory phase in which birds listen to the songs of more experienced “tutor” birds, and (2) a sensorimotor learning phase, in which birds practice the memorised songs and perfect them. After these phases, songs are crystallised into highly stereotyped patterns (Williams 2004). Therefore, birdsongs are an interesting sequential behavioural pattern to review, because both innate and learning mechanisms are at play in the development of the songs’ structure (Gardner et al. 2005).

Depending on the species, birds develop different song repertoires, from Bengalese finches performing only one stereotyped song, to nightingales that produce over 200 different song types. To deal with the serial information present in the songs, it has been shown that many species of birds, such as canaries, zebra finches and nightingales, produce phrases of syllables, sometimes called “motifs”, which themselves can be grouped into songs (Hultsch and Todt 1989). This arrangement of syllables is believed to be done in a hierarchical manner, which, much like chunks in the motor domain, allows an efficient way to process and store the large number of syllables a bird can come to produce (Markowitz et al. 2013).

These syllable phrases have been described as “subsets of sequentially associated items”. They are characterised by having large transition probabilities between elements of the same phrase or chunk, and low transition probabilities between phrase boundaries. Furthermore, these syllable phrases are separated from each other by long silent intervals. The timing of these intervals correlates with how the syllables are sequenced, with high transition probabilities associated with short silent intervals and low transition probabilities with long ones (Matheson and Sakata 2015; Takashi et al. 2010), suggesting a similar sequence representation as the one described in mammals, who display short inter-response times between actions within a chunk, and long inter-response times between chunks (Sakai et al. 2003).

This syllable organisation gives rise to highly consistent songs. The high degree of song stereotypy is believed to have been selected by evolution, since female birds prefer males that sing more stereotyped syllable sequences (Sakata and Vehrencamp 2011). Indeed, it is known that some aspects of the syntactic organisation of songs is imposed by innate mechanisms, given that birds reared in isolation are able to develop some structured songs (Liu and Nottebohm 2007). Moreover, even if birds are exposed to incorrect tutor songs, they still develop structured songs. For example, birds tutored with songs without the species-typical first element, tend to invent their own initial element (Hultsch and Todt 1989). Interestingly, creating chunks of syllables also seems to be, at least partly, an innate characteristic, since birds exposed to really long tutor songs (i.e. with less and shorter boundaries) have a tendency to spontaneously segment the songs into smaller segments, even though they were not explicitly tutored to do it (Hultsch 1992).

Nonetheless, although birds exposed to these “incorrect” tutor songs or birds reared alone (i.e. untutored) preserve some characteristics of normal songs, they do display odd structures, with decreased sequential stereotypy and smaller song repertoires (Hultsch 1992; Hughes et al. 2002), meaning that learning in birdsongs plays an important role for the development of their structure. During the learning period, sensory feedback is very important to refine the precise execution of the song, but once learned, a song’s rendition becomes very stable, and it does not change even if birds are exposed to new tutor songs (Brainard and Doupe 2000). However, after crystallisation, if auditory feedback is disturbed by external noises, birdsongs display disruptions both in sequencing and timing aspects, with less stereotyped songs and with slower tempo (Sakata and Brainard 2006). This seems to be in contrast with chain grooming, a much less flexible behaviour, which can be carried out normally without any sensory feedback (Berridge and Fentress 1987a). Thus, more flexible mechanisms play an important role in the execution of crystallised syllable sequences.

3.6 Neural circuits underlying birdsong

The system in charge of producing songs in birds involves the avian cortex, basal ganglia, thalamus and brainstem nuclei. This song system has been typically divided into two pathways: (1) the motor or posterior pathway which is necessary for acquisition and execution, and (2) the anterior pathway which is only necessary for acquisition (Nottebohm 2005). In the motor pathway, neurons from the high vocal centre (HVC) send projections to the robust nucleus of the arcopallium (RA), which in turn is directly connected to brainstem and midbrain neurons that control the vocal and respiration muscles (Bertram et al. 2014). On the other hand, in the anterior pathway, neurons from the HVC send projections to Area X, a structure homologous to the mammalian striatum. Area X in turn sends exclusive inhibitory projections to a portion of the dorsolateral anterior thalamic nucleus, which in turn sends exclusive projections to the lateral magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium (LMAN; Bottjer et al. 1989), which by projecting back to the RA closes the cortico-basal ganglia-thalamocortical loop. HVC and RA are analogous to the premotor and motor cortex in mammals, respectively (Brainard and Doupe 2013), thus, the posterior pathway is mostly a motor cortical circuit, while the anterior pathway resembles the cortico-thalamo-basal ganglia loop observed in mammals (Jarvis et al. 1998; Mooney 2009). A comparison is shown in Figure 4.

Figure 4: 
Comparison of the mammalian and avian basal ganglia network. LMAN: The lateral magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium, HVC: High vocal centre, RA: robust nucleus of the arcopallium. Excitatory connections are in red and inhibitory connections are in blue. Yellow/semi-circles indicate mixed excitatory/inhibitory connections.
Figure 4:

Comparison of the mammalian and avian basal ganglia network. LMAN: The lateral magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium, HVC: High vocal centre, RA: robust nucleus of the arcopallium. Excitatory connections are in red and inhibitory connections are in blue. Yellow/semi-circles indicate mixed excitatory/inhibitory connections.

The roles of these two pathways differ at the different stages of song acquisition. The motor pathway, as its names indicates, is fundamental for motor control. Lesions to this pathway lead to the complete loss of singing (Nottebohm et al. 1976). Furthermore, the highly stereotyped performance of crystallised songs is believed to come from activity in this pathway, since lesions to the HVC (i.e. homologue of the premotor cortex) lead to disruptions in the songs’ stereotypy and timing (Long and Fee 2008; Thompson and Johnson 2007). Recordings made from HVC neurons have shown that these neurons fire only once per song phrase or motif, suggesting a sparse hierarchical coding of song phrases in this area (Hahnloser et al. 2002). Neurons in the HVC are connected in a chain fashion, believed to be responsible for producing the ordered sequences of syllables (Long et al. 2010), which shares some similarities with the neuronal chains believed to orchestrate sequential activation for crawling patterns in leeches (Cacciatore et al. 2000).

Lesions to the anterior pathway (i.e. the avian basal ganglia) only disrupt the songs’ structure when they are performed during the phase of song acquisition. If lesions to the LMAN are made during acquisition, the stereotypy of the songs significantly increases, producing highly repetitive patterns from very early on, indicating that one of the main roles of this pathway is to introduce variability, a key component for learning (Ölveczky et al. 2005). This variability is apparently not completely random, since it has been shown that stimulating the LMAN can bias the song towards specific goals (Kao et al. 2005). On the other hand, birds that are lesioned in Area X, equivalent to the mammalian striatum, are never able to develop a structured song, displaying longer than normal syllables and less sequence stereotypy (Scharff and Nottebohm 1991). When birds are already adults, lesions to Area X slow down the song production, increasing the inter-syllable intervals, although unlike the chain grooming and the mammalian striatum, sequence production is preserved (Chen et al. 2014). That said, similar to the mammalian striatum, Area X in birds receives a dopaminergic input. Optogenetic inactivation or excitation of these dopaminergic terminals leads to online changes in the songs’ structure, suggesting that dopamine serves as a key teaching signal shaping the songs (Xiao et al. 2018). Finally, the medial portion of the avian dorsolateral anterior thalamic nucleus seems to be more implicated in song initiation, since birds lesioned in this area, although still able to produce certain calls, hardly sing and when they do, they show disrupted rhythm, possibly showing a deficit in initiating and pacing of syllable sequences (Chen et al. 2014).

In conclusion, it seems that developing chunks or motifs is a strategy that has been used by several species to deal with large amounts of serial information, with a hierarchical representation being favoured. However, in contrast to the grooming chain and other innate behaviours, in birdsong there is an added learning process, in which both sensory feedback and variability are two fundamental aspects for the development of stereotyped sequences. In terms of the neural circuits involved, while in the grooming chain the striatum along with its downstream targets are apparently enough for its sequential implementation, in birdsong, damage to the homologous striatum, Area X, preserves the ability to produce a sequentially ordered song once learnt. However, it renders birds unable to learn new structured songs. Syllable sequencing with learnt songs seems to be imposed by the cortical pathway, with the avian cortico-basal ganglia network playing a key role in the sensorimotor learning of the songs. Furthermore, the flexibility in song production suggests that as behaviours become more complex, such that they incorporate a learned component, CPGs in the brainstem and spinal cord seem to become more dependent on descending signals. In the following section, we take learning one step further and review some of the findings in scenarios in which completely new sequences of behaviours have to be acquired.

4 Learned behavioural sequences

Innate behaviours are only a part of an animal’s behavioural repertoire. One of the most important abilities linked to survival is the capacity to learn new behavioural patterns in order to adjust to a changing environment. There are several ways in which a new behaviour can be acquired. In this section we will focus on reinforcement learning (RL), in which – by trial and error – animals learn to modify their behaviour in order to obtain reinforcers, such as food or shelter (Sutton and Barto 1998). RL is believed to involve the acquisition of two basic relationships: a response-outcome relationship and a stimulus-response relationship. These two associations are believed to be the basis of goal-directed and habitual behaviours, respectively (Balleine et al. 2009). The cortico-basal ganglia network has been thoroughly implicated in these two learning systems, with the dorsomedial striatum (DMS) found to underlie goal-directed processes and the dorsolateral striatum, habitual ones (Lipton et al. 2019; Yin and Knowlton 2006). Although learning an action sequence encompasses both associations, there are added challenges when instrumentally learning a new sequence of actions.

First of all, in most instances of action sequence learning, there is no template to which each element of a sequence can be compared, unlike birds learning songs, which hold a copy of their tutor’s song in memory and adjust their performance in accordance. Usually, the feedback about whether the actions were performed correctly or not is only obtained after the whole sequence is completed, meaning that animals must learn to assign credit to temporally distant elements. Although the main proposal has been that credit back-propagates as action sequences are learned, that is, the last element of an action sequence is learned first and earlier elements are subsequently learned, recent findings have called into question this idea (Fu and Anderson 2008; Geddes et al. 2018). Additionally, it has been found that a well learned action sequence can resurface in the behavioural repertoire of an animal even after it has been extinguished (Bacha-Mendez et al. 2007). This is believed to indicate that the sequence has been chunked into an integrated unit, possibly involving not only action–outcome and stimulus–action relations, but also action–action associations. This suggests that some kind of neural representation of the sequence as a unit and action–action associations must be encoded and stored somewhere in the brain. However, how learned action sequences are actually put together and then represented is still a matter of debate. In the following section we turn to some of the findings that have shed some light to these questions.

4.1 Neural substrate underlying sequence learning and performance

Just as the cortico-basal ganglia network is necessary for learning sequences of syllables in birds, it has also been found to be fundamental for sequential learning and chunking in mammals (Boyd et al. 2009; Fee and Scharff 2010; Graybiel 1998). In this section, studies involving lesions, electrophysiological recordings, pharmacological interventions and optogenetic manipulations of the basal ganglia during sequential learning tasks are reviewed.

First of all, as with the innate grooming chain, the striatum has been found to be a key region in learned action sequences, with different roles for the medial and lateral aspects. Lesions to the DLS, but not the DMS during the early stage of learning have been found to selectively disrupt action sequence acquisition, without actually producing any deficit in single action learning, suggesting that the DLS might have a very specific role in action concatenation (Geddes et al. 2018; Yin 2010). This has also been reported in humans, in which evidence also suggests that sequencing is a task of the striatum, while premotor areas of the cortex and cerebellum are more involved in other motor and cognitive aspects of the task (Janacsek et al. 2020; Wymbs et al. 2012).

Furthermore, electrophysiological recordings have revealed that as a sequence is learned a bracketing activity at the beginning and end of the sequence emerges and remains even after devaluation, suggesting that this activity pattern might represent the action sequence as a unit (Jin and Costa 2010; Jog et al. 1999; Smith and Graybiel 2013). This start/stop activity is expressed both in direct and indirect pathway MSNs, with direct pathway MSNs firing both at the beginning and end of a sequence, and indirect pathway MSNs firing preferentially at the beginning of the sequence (Jin et al. 2014). Direct pathway MSNs also display sustained firing during the complete execution of a learned action sequence, while indirect pathway MSNs have been found to display inhibited firing (Jin and Costa 2010; Jin et al. 2014). Importantly, this seems to be a specific characteristic of the DLS, since this bracketing activity is not found in the DMS (Martiros et al. 2018). It has also been reported that fast spiking interneurons (FSIs) in the striatum are important for habitual behaviour (O’Hare et al. 2017). FSI interneurons play a crucial role in regulating the output of striatal circuits by forming strong connections with MSNs, particularly those belonging to the direct pathway, thereby serving as one of the primary mediators of feedforward inhibition within the striatum (Gittits et al. 2010). In action sequences, FSIs have been found to develop specific firing patterns as an action sequence is learned, firing mostly in the middle of a learned action sequence (Martiros et al. 2018). The start/stop activity in MSNs alongside the activity of FSIs in the middle of learned action sequences is presumably at least in part facilitated by the inhibitory impact of FSIs on MSNs (Mallet et al. 2005). Importantly, these activity patterns, both in MSNs and interneurons, are only observed when the sequences are performed correctly, indicating that these different patterns, possibly encoding the action sequences as a unit, emerge in the basal ganglia as a consequence of RL (Martiros et al. 2018).

The striatum is not only important during early-stage sequence learning, as in birdsong, but also once the sequence has been well learned, with specific roles for the direct and indirect pathways. Completely ablating MSNs of the direct pathway in the dorsal striatum has been found to completely disrupt the performance of a crystallised sequence, with animals showing a return to initial performance, becoming unable to correctly complete the sequence. On the other hand, ablating indirect MSNs produces a deficit in switching between elements of the sequence (Geddes et al. 2018; Rothwell et al. 2015). Importantly, these findings have been shown not to be the result of disrupted locomotion, motivation or general switching, but rather they seem to be indicating a specific deficit in sequential performance.

With the development of optogenetics, transient activation or inactivation at particular time points is now possible, making manipulations very specific. This has allowed further differentiation of the roles of direct and indirect pathway MSNs during the performance of learned sequences. Transient optogenetic stimulation of direct pathway MSNs performed in the middle of a learned sequence facilitates behaviour by adding actions to the sequence; whereas transient stimulation of indirect pathway MSNs leads to elimination of ongoing actions, making the sequences shorter (Geddes et al. 2018). Accordingly, Tecuapetla et al. (2016) found that activating DLS indirect pathway MSNs in the middle of a well learned sequence leads animals to abort the ongoing sequence and switch to other unrelated behaviours. On the other hand, if optogenetic activation or inactivation of each pathway in the DLS is performed right before an action sequence is started, this leads to increased latency to the first element of the sequence (Tecuapetla et al. 2016). Thus, it seems that very specific activity patterns of striatal MSNs are critical for action sequence acquisition and performance.

Given that it is known that striatal MSNs are quiescent much of the time, these findings have led to the question of what is driving these striatal firing patterns. One of the main excitatory inputs to the striatum comes from the cortex (Wall et al. 2013), and it has been shown that NMDA- and AMPA-dependent plasticity at these synapses are necessary for acquisition of sequential stepping patterns on a rotarod (Dang et al. 2006; Nakamura et al. 2017; Yin et al. 2009). Thus, one proposal is that cortico-striatal plasticity is one of the mechanisms that shapes MSN activity during action sequence learning (Jin and Costa 2015; Tremblay et al. 2010). That said, studies investigating the role of cortical inputs in action sequences has yielded mixed results. During the initial learning phase, lesions to the primary and secondary motor cortices render animals unable to learn action sequences (Kawai et al. 2015). In line with these findings, Rothwell et al. (2015) have also reported that the during acquisition of a two-action sequence, the synapses between secondary motor cortex (M2) and striatum are strengthened, and that these synapses are fundamental for action sequence initiation, even after crystallisation of the learned action sequence. Finally, the bracketing firing pattern found in striatum during the execution of a learned motor sequence has also been observed in the prefrontal cortex during the performance of oculomotor sequences (Fuji and Graybiel 2003).

However, Ostlund et al. (2009) report that damage to the dorso-medial prefrontal cortex (i.e. M2) does not impair rats in learning to perform an action sequence, but it does prevent sequence-level representations to form, only noticeable in a devaluation test, not in performance itself. Furthermore, others have reported that once an action sequence has been learned, bilateral lesions to primary and secondary motor cortex have no effect in its performance (Dhawale et al. 2021; Kawai et al. 2015). Recordings made in the primary motor cortex by Martiros et al. (2018) seem to confirm this, since their results revealed that although the cortex represents the individual actions of a sequence, this is regardless of their reinforcement history. Moreover, optogenetically inhibiting these cortical neurons has no effect on the sequence performance, or in the bracketing activity of the striatum. Thus, it seems that some parts of the cortex might be necessary for learning, playing a tutor role to the striatum, but not for storing or performing a well learned action sequence (Dhawahle et al. 2021).

So, it seems that as a sequence is learned and progressively chunked, it can be executed without inputs from the cortex. This is associated with a more automatic performance, as indicated by a reduction in inter-response times (Sakai et al. 2003), and a decrease in the sensitivity to the environmental feedback resembling a characteristic of CPG networks (Dezfoulli et al. 2014; Grillner 2006). This has led to the proposal that, once a sequence is learned, its underlying neuronal representation might resemble a CPG network (Yin et al. 2009). This is in line with the proposal that CPG-like structures could be found in other parts of the central nervous system. Indeed, it has been suggested that there are similarities between the CPG network arrangements found in the spinal cord and brainstem, and neural networks found in cortex, both with similar oscillatory properties (Yuste et al. 2005). Although the CPG-like structures suggested to be in cortex would largely be more flexible than those found subcortically, Yuste et al. (2005) propose that a basic CPG-like neuronal organisation could be found throughout the CNS, in which excitatory recurrent networks are ingrained in inhibitory circuits, with neurons displaying oscillations between up states (depolarized) and down states (hyperpolarized). Interestingly, in vitro studies have shown that the striatum has neuronal ensembles that display spatiotemporal activity patterns with similar characteristics to those found in CPGs, displaying recurrent and synchronised activity patterns (Carrillo-Reid et al. 2008).

Furthermore, it is most likely that other structures besides the cortex that send projections to the striatum are also important for the organisation of sequences, and that the process of learning and performing a behavioural sequence is really distributed in several areas (Penhune and Steele 2012). The pedunculopontine and laterodorsal tegmental nuclei provide a cholinergic input to the striatum (Dautan et al. 2016), however the thalamus is the major subcortical source of innervation, with the PF having the highest density of striatally-projecting neurons of any subcortical structure (Mandelbaum et al. 2019). In a recent study, Diaz-Hernandez et al. (2018) have shown that activity in the thalamic reticular nucleus is modulated by the initiation and performance of an action sequence, and that optogenetic inhibition of these neurons delays the beginning and execution of a learned action sequence. A similar function for sequence initiation has been found in birdsong (Chen et al. 2014). This makes sense, given that motor information from subcortical-basal ganglia loops goes through a thalamic relay before reaching the striatum (McHaffie et al. 2005).

Finally, it is known that dopamine is a main modulator of cortico-striatal synaptic plasticity (Reynolds and Wickens 2002). Thus, not surprisingly, dopamine has also been implicated in chunking of action sequences. Accordingly, rats lesioned in the SNc display an abnormal temporal structure of open field behavioural sequences (Casarrubea et al. 2019), and blocking D2-type dopamine receptors in the striatum during sequence learning in monkeys disrupts motor chunking (Levesque et al. 2007). As an action sequence is learned, it has been reported that preferential dopamine release moves from the last element to the first element of the sequence (Collins et al. 2016; Wassum et al. 2012), thus possibly contributing to the bracketing activity found in the DLS. Accordingly, difficulties in chunking have been well reported in PD patients, who suffer from dopamine depletion in the striatum. PD patients are known to have difficulties initiating, performing and ending action sequences, and in particular, they show difficulties switching between two different actions (Georgiou et al. 1994; Harrington and Haaland 1991). This seems to be dopamine dependent, given that only when PD patients are off their medication are they unable to chunk actions, as evidenced by an inability to reduce the inter-response times after extended training (Tremblay et al. 2010). Furthermore, PD patients do not show an increase in striatal activity that is normally found in healthy individuals when executing automatic action sequences. Instead, they show greater cortical activity than controls, suggesting that the cortex never stops playing its tutor role (Wu et al. 2010). PD patients not only have sequencing deficits in the motor domain, but they have also been found to display disrupted cognitive sequencing (i.e. in a serial prediction task), which correlates with decreased striatal activity (Schönberger et al. 2015). Overall, these findings have led to the suggestion that PD patients are unable to shift control from cortical to subcortical areas, which might be why they cannot chunk actions together (Tremblay et al. 2010).

Taken together, experimental and clinical evidence suggests a complex role for the cortico-basal ganglia network in learned action sequences, with distinctive roles for the direct and indirect pathways and their inputs. As in chain grooming, learned action sequences also display specific striatal activity patterns; however, unlike chain grooming, which can occur without the whole cortex, the acquisition and possibly some aspects of the performance of a learned action sequence seem to require different parts of the cortex, with cortico-striatal plasticity believed to play a central role in acquisition, mediated partially by dopamine. Interestingly, this is similar to findings in birdsong, in which the cortex plays a central role, and dopamine is also believed to be fundamentally involved in the plasticity needed for learning syllable sequences. However, in spite of a recent surge in research, the mechanisms in the striatum that lead to action–action associations are still not fully understood. That said, some clues are beginning to emerge from closer study of the striatal microcircuit, especially in relation to striatal neurotransmission.

5 Striatal microcircuit: neurotransmitters in the striatum

There is a complex microcircuit within the striatum with several neurotransmitters systems believed to play different functions, and it has been pointed out that the complex biochemical links known to mediate communication between MSNs have been largely left out from classical models of the basal ganglia (Calabresi et al. 2014). As described earlier, approximately 95 % of the neurons in the striatum are GABAergic MSNs and they can be divided into two populations, those from the direct pathway, and those from the indirect pathway. However, besides GABA, these two neuronal populations express different neuropeptides and dopamine receptors, with direct pathway MSNs mostly expressing substance P (SP) and D1-type dopamine receptors, and indirect pathway MSNs mainly expressing enkephalin and D2-type dopamine receptors (Gerfen et al. 1990). This diversity of neuromodulators suggests a complex chemical regulation of striatal activity. Although dopamine has been the focus of much research in relation to action sequences, SP and enkephalin have also been reported to influence learning and memory (Huston and Hasenöhrl 1995), and they actually interact with dopamine in interesting ways (Brimblecombe and Cragg 2015). Furthermore, SP and enkephalin have been recently proposed as possible chemical mediators of action sequence chunking (Buxton et al. 2017).

5.1 Substance P

SP is part of a family of neuropeptides referred to as tachykinins that is present both in the central and peripheral nervous systems. Its effects are mediated primarily through the NK1 receptor, a G-protein coupled receptor, but it also binds to NK2 and NK3 receptors in a lesser degree (Rupniak and Kramer 2002). In the central nervous system, NK1 receptors and SP fibres can be found in the basal ganglia, nucleus accumbens (NAc), amygdala, thalamus and hypothalamus, amongst other areas. In the basal ganglia specifically, NK1 receptors and SP fibres can be found in SNr, globus pallidus, NAc and striatum, however, cell bodies containing SP are only present in striatum and NAc (Ribeiro-da-Silva and Hökfelt 2000; Shults et al. 1984). In the striatum, SP is mainly released by direct pathway MSNs, and SP boutons mainly target other MSNs, primarily at the dendritic shafts and spines; though they also contact striatal interneurons (Bolam and Izzo 1988; Bolam et al. 1986). Accordingly, NK1 receptors can be found both postsynaptically on cholinergic and GABAergic striatal interneurons, and presynaptically on axon terminals contacting MSNs, most likely afferents from cortex or thalamus (Chen et al. 2001, 2003; Jakab and Goldman-Rakic 1996). The conditions under which SP is released are still under research. However, MSNs are known to often fire in bursts of 3–10 action potentials (Stern et al. 1997) and antidromically activating globus pallidus-projecting MSNs in a bursting pattern has a greater SP-mediated facilitatory effect on glutamatergic excitation than single spike activation (Blomeley et al. 2009). Furthermore, it has been found that high frequency but not low frequency activation of direct pathway MSNs causes the release of SP in the NAc (Francis et al. 2019), which makes sense given that high frequency stimulation is known to be required for some peptide release (Hökfelt et al. 2000). This evidence indirectly suggests that SP may be preferentially released during high frequency bursts. Given that SP is co-released with GABA, burst-induced release may be necessary to counteract the effects of co-released GABA.

SP influences neuronal activity through different pathways. First of all, although NK1 receptors have not been reported on MSNs directly, it has been demonstrated that SP can directly elicit depolarization of MSNs (Blomeley and Bracci 2008). This is believed to be mediated by presynaptic effects, since SP has been shown to facilitate the response of neighbouring MSNs to glutamatergic inputs in the rat, through presynaptic NK1 receptors (Blomeley et al. 2009). SP facilitation of glutamatergic inputs to MSNs leads on to the second way that SP influences neuronal activity. As shown in Figure 5, in some dual-cell recordings in the striatum, if an MSN is repeatedly activated before a cortical input arrives to a second connected neighbouring MSN, the response amplitude in the second MSN increases over time, suggesting some kind of long-term plasticity mediated by SP. This could mean that SP connections between MSNs might encode the order in which two neurons are repeatedly activated by cortical inputs. A similar finding has been shown in the spinal cord of lampreys. It has been reported that SP facilitates the response to descending reticulospinal inputs by potentiating glutamatergic transmission, which ultimately leads the network to a more stable and higher frequency of bursting, which behaviourally would lead to faster and “better” swimming in the lamprey (Parker et al. 1998). Whether this is a long-term effect in the spinal cord is not known.

Figure 5: 
Long term plasticity mediated by SP. An example of data from a paired recording experiment in a striatal slice in which glutamatergic afferents to striatal neurons were electrically activated. Five spikes were evoked by current injection in medium spiny neuron 1 (MSN1) 100 ms before every other afferent stimulation. The amplitude of the response to glutamatergic afferent activation in a second connected medium spiny neuron (MSN2) are shown preceded (right) or not preceded (left) by spikes in the first medium spiny neuron. GABAA, GABAB and broad spectrum opioid receptor antagonists were present throughout. While no significant change over time was observed for responses not preceded by spikes, a significant linear trend (p < 0.001) was present for responses preceded by spikes. Blomeley and Bracci (unpublished data).
Figure 5:

Long term plasticity mediated by SP. An example of data from a paired recording experiment in a striatal slice in which glutamatergic afferents to striatal neurons were electrically activated. Five spikes were evoked by current injection in medium spiny neuron 1 (MSN1) 100 ms before every other afferent stimulation. The amplitude of the response to glutamatergic afferent activation in a second connected medium spiny neuron (MSN2) are shown preceded (right) or not preceded (left) by spikes in the first medium spiny neuron. GABAA, GABAB and broad spectrum opioid receptor antagonists were present throughout. While no significant change over time was observed for responses not preceded by spikes, a significant linear trend (p < 0.001) was present for responses preceded by spikes. Blomeley and Bracci (unpublished data).

Besides directly affecting MSNs, either post or presynaptically, applying SP to the striatum has also been found to produce excitatory responses in cholinergic interneurons, increasing acetylcholine (Ach) levels in freely moving rats (Anderson et al. 1993; Aosaki and Kawaguchi 1996). Furthermore, it has also been reported that SP released by direct pathway MSNs causes a long-lasting potentiation of indirect pathway MSNs through cholinergic interneurons in the NAc, suggesting that SP might play a fundamental role in communication between the direct and indirect pathways through interneuron networks, at least in the NAc (Francis et al. 2019). Finally, several studies have found a modulatory effect of SP on dopamine. Although there is no consensus on whether SP increases or decreases dopamine levels in the striatum (Gauchy et al. 1996; Gygi et al. 1993; Kraft et al. 2001; Tremblay et al. 1992), Brimblecombe and Cragg (2015) have proposed that the mixed results concerning SP and dopamine are due to different effects of SP on the matrix and striosomes, two biochemical compartments of the striatum (Crittenden and Graybiel 2011). Their results suggest that SP upregulates dopamine only in striosomes, inhibits it at the striosome-matrix boundaries and leaves it unaltered in matrix.

These results suggest that SP’s effects on striatal output are manifold. Thus, not surprisingly, studies in which SP, NK1 agonists or antagonists have been injected, either locally or systemically, have produced numerous effects on behaviour. In terms of general locomotion, systemic injections of SP have been reported to increase behavioural output, with increased locomotion, grooming, scratching and rearing having been reported (Hall et al. 1987; Katz and Gelbart 1978; Van Wimersma Greidanus and Maigret 1988). Accordingly, blocking SP has been found to inhibit stereotypical behaviours (Duffy et al. 2002). However, others have reported that mice injected with an NK1 antagonist and mice lacking NK1 receptors actually display hyperactivity or no effect on locomotion (Kertes et al. 2010; Porter et al. 2015; Yan et al. 2010). Either way, these effects of SP on behavioural output have been suggested to be partially regulated by dopamine, since intrastriatally blocking NK1 receptors decreases the locomotion induced by D1-type dopamine receptor agonists or dopamine-related drugs like amphetamine (Duffy et al. 2002; Gonzalez-Nicolini and McGinty 2002; Krolewski et al. 2005). Furthermore, administration of an NK1 receptor antagonist significantly diminishes cocaine-induced DA release (Kraft et al. 2001).

The possibility that SP plays a role in the serial organisation of behaviour has recently been proposed by Buxton et al. (2017) using a computational modelling approach. According to Buxton et al.’s (2017) model, SP, being an excitatory neuropeptide co-released by direct pathway MSNs, contributes to striatal activity, allowing sustained selection of actions, and facilitates the response of neighbouring neurons, aiding subsequent actions to be selected in the correct order. Interestingly, this is similar to the proposal of Cacciatore et al. (2000), who suggest that the sequential coordination of leeches’ body segments could be achieved with a neural chain, in which neurons from one unit directly excite the next unit, spreading activity in an orderly fashion. In summary, this computational model suggests that directed release of SP in the striatum improves action selection performance, both in ordered and unordered sequences of actions.

The potential role of SP in the serial organisation of behaviour has received further support from experimental studies. Earlier studies have used the 5-choice serial reaction time task, a task that uses random sequences of nose pokes guided by light. Using this task, it has been found that mice lacking NK1 receptors display a greater percentage of omissions in the sequence (i.e. they fail to respond), perseverations and premature responses, and they take longer times to retrieve the reward (Porter et al. 2015; Weir et al. 2013; Yan et al. 2011). Overall, these results suggest that mice lacking NK1 receptors display disrupted action selection and attentional deficits in a sequential unordered task. Although interesting, the structure of the task (i.e. random sequences with guiding stimuli) means that the mice were not able to develop integrated sequences.

More recently, we have examined role of SP in innate and learned action sequences by examining the behavioural impact of the systemic administration of an SP antagonist. Looking first at chain grooming (innate behaviour), we found that blocking SP receptors made the highly fixed transitions inside the grooming chain and the overall grooming bout transition structure significantly more variable and less diverse than in the control group injected with saline (Favila et al. 2021). Overall, this suggest that blocking SP led to a general break down in the fluency of behavioural patterns, making them more variable and simpler. Interestingly, when rats are decerebrated at the metencephalic and mesencephalic level they are still able to produce a few complete grooming chains (Berridge 1989), although with a decreased efficiency. Therefore, it is plausible to think that the results obtained after blocking SP were due to effects at the striatal level.

Moving to learned action sequences, using a simple left-right (L-R) lever behavioural chunk, we found that blocking SP receptors had the effect of making learning a new sequence (e.g. R-L after L-R), and simultaneously extinguishing an overlearned old sequence, faster than in the control group, whereas blocking SP receptors had no effect on the stable performance of a well learned sequence (Favila et al. 2023). The results obtained with the SP antagonist appeared surprising at first, given that the grooming chain results seem to intuitively suggest that injecting the SP antagonist should have had a detrimental effect on learning a new sequence. Taking a closer look at the results, the effect of the antagonist seems to have been on the extinguishing process of the first learned sequence, which disintegrated faster when SP was blocked, allowing the rats to learn a new sequence faster. These experiments suggest that the effect of blocking SP was on the initial phase when the contingencies change, by particularly affecting the speed at which an overlearned sequence was extinguished. As a consequence, it is tempting to propose that the role of the SP could be to consolidate action sequence representation by facilitating cortico-striatal plasticity (Figure 5), which would result in the orderly spreading of activity among striatal neurons. That said, computational modelling based on our behavioural results highlights the possibility that an interaction at the striatal level between SP and dopamine affecting the reward prediction error, an important concept in reinforcement learning, could also make a contribution (Favila et al. 2023).

5.2 Enkephalin

Enkephalin is an endogenous opioid neuropeptide that acts mainly through δ and µ opioid receptors, both G-protein coupled receptors. Enkephalin is widely expressed in the nervous system, with high concentrations in the amygdala, NAc, periaqueductal grey and hypothalamus, amongst others. In the basal ganglia in particular, its highest concentration can be found at the striatum and globus pallidus, and in both structures cell bodies containing enkephalin can be observed (Ingham et al. 1991; Mallet et al. 2012; Miller and Cuatrecasas 1978). Although computational modelling suggests that enkephalin facilitates action sequence production (Buxton et al. 2017), that possibility has been poorly explored experimentally, with the exception of Horner et al. (2012), who reported that intra-striatal treatment with the µ opioid receptor agonist DAMGO in the striatum led to an increase in repetition, frequency, duration and spatial distribution of stereotypic behaviours induced by methamphetamine.

6 Conclusions

Evidence suggests that the basal ganglia play a crucial role in the concatenation of simpler behaviours into more complex chunks, in the context of innate behavioural sequences such as chain grooming in rats, as well as sequences in which innate capabilities and learning interact such as birdsong, and sequences that are learned from scratch, such as lever press sequences in operant behaviour. It has been proposed that the role of the striatum, the largest input structure of the basal ganglia, might lie in selecting and allowing the relevant CPGs to gain access to the motor system in the correct order, while inhibiting other behaviours (Berridge and Wishaw 1992). With chain grooming, lesions of a wide range of striatally-projecting structures including the entire neocortex do not seem to produce any lasting effects on the sequential organisation of the grooming chain (Berridge and Whishaw 1992; Cromwell and Berridge 1996). Although thalamic inputs to the striatum were still intact in these early studies, they suggest that the striatum is able to organise sequential behaviours via aspects of intra-striatal organisation. As behaviours become more complex and flexible, for example when animals are learning new behavioural sequences, the striatum seems to become more dependent on external signals. However, after learning, in mammals (unlike birds) the striatum itself may adopt the functional characteristics of a higher order CPG, operating relatively independently of the need for external inputs. There are clearly many unknowns here, but one of the most salient is how does the striatum move from needing to be instructed by external inputs to being a possibly largely autonomous higher order CPG?

Although speculative, enough is known to piece together a plausible proposal. Cortico-striatal synaptic plasticity has been proposed as one of the mechanisms that shapes MSN activity during action sequence learning (Jin and Costa 2015; Tremblay et al. 2010). Striatal SP appears to be able to facilitate cortico-striatal excitation, consolidating retention of learned sequences (Favila et al. 2023), and when present, cortico-striatal synapses exhibit evidence of plasticity (Figure 5). In simpler rhythmic systems like the leech, sequential movements are produced via a neural chain in which neurons from one unit directly excite the next unit, spreading activity in an orderly fashion (Cacciatore et al. 2000). Similar connectivity is apparent in the HVC of songbirds (Long et al. 2010). Murray and Escola (2017) have recently suggested that sequential firing patterns across neuronal ensembles encoding specific behavioural components in the striatum could be implemented by depotentiation of inhibitory synapses between MSNs, effectively establishing a neural chain in the striatum. Although inhibitory synapses between MSNs are relatively sparse and weak in terms of their impact (Czubayko and Plenz 2002; Jaeger et al. 1994; Tunstall et al. 2002), each MSN is thought to receive inputs from around 300 adjacent MSNs (Guzman et al. 2003) and many MSNs firing together may mediate more effective inter-cellular inhibition. According to computational modelling, depotentiation of inhibitory synapses between MSNs is an effective means of encapsulating a sequence at the striatal level and acting as the substrate upon which learning works (Murray and Escola 2017). The missing link is how cortico-striatal synaptic plasticity translates into decreased inhibitory connectivity strength between MSNs. In relation to that, one possibility is suggested by work in the hippocampus where it has been demonstrated that enhanced glutamate-mediated transmission at some synapses in the CA1 region can produce heterosynaptic long term depression of inhibitory synaptic transmission at others (Chevaleyre and Castillo 2003). This effect in the hippocampus is initiated by excitatory metabotropic glutamate (mGlu) 1/5 receptors (Chevaleyre and Castillo 2003). Low to moderate levels of mGluR1 and a high level of mGluR5 mRNAs are expressed in the vast majority of either striatonigral or striatopallidal projection neurons (Kerner et al. 1997; Testa et al. 1994), suggesting that the appropriate synaptic machinery is present in the striatum for similar heterosynaptic effects to occur. Hence, cortico-striatal plasticity may lead to decreased levels of MSN-MSN inhibition in the striatum for a given behavioural sequence. MSNs, connected individually or in clusters via depotentiated inhibitory interconnections and innervating appropriate downstream CPGs, could form the neurobiological substrate for chunks.

That said, there are clearly still many unanswered questions. For example, given that individual grooming behaviours can occur inside or outside of the grooming chain (Favila et al. 2021), how can a single behaviour that occurs within a chunk be selected without engaging the remainder of the chunk? Grooming behaviours selected outside of chains are associated with lower levels of striatal activity than those same behaviours selected inside of chains (Aldridge and Berridge 1998), suggesting that a certain level of neuronal activation may be required to trigger a chain. The neuropeptide SP would be well-positioned to exert influence over behavioural sequence selection given that peptide release necessitates high-frequency stimulation (Francis et al. 2019; Hökfelt et al. 2000). Another set of unknowns are around the role of dopamine in chunking. Agonists at D1-type dopamine receptors lead to so called ‘super-stereotypy’ within the grooming chain (Berridge and Aldridge 2000) and we have hypothesised elsewhere that SP may interact with dopamine in the striatum to produce some of its effects (Favila et al. 2023). Dopamine may also ‘bracket’ the behavioural chunk, encoding the beginning and end (Jin and Costa 2010). Bracketed dopaminergic activity, alongside bracketed MSN activity (Jin et al. 2014; Martiros et al. 2018), have yet-to-be-discovered roles to play in chunking. Likewise, as both the direct and indirect pathway appear to have a role to play in chunking (Geddes et al. 2018; Rothwell et al. 2015), the manner in which those pathways interact has yet to be unravelled. Although there is still much to be discovered, there is a definite sense that a full understanding of the relationship between the basal ganglia and sequential behaviour is now within reach.


Corresponding author: Natalia Favila, German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Venusberg-Campus 1, Gebäude 99, 53127 Bonn, Germany, E-mail:

Award Identifier / Grant number: A2014

Funding source: Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnologia

Award Identifier / Grant number: 409168

  1. Author contributions: All the authors have accepted responsibility for the entire content of this submitted manuscript and approved submission.

  2. Research funding: This work was supported by the PhD scholarship 409168 granted by Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnologia (CONACyT), grant A2014 granted by The John Black Charitable Foundation and by statutory funds of the Department of Psychology at The University of Sheffield.

  3. Conflict of interest statement: The authors have no conflicts of interest or competing interests to report.

References

Aoki, S., Liu, A.W., Zucca, A., Zucca, S., and Wickens, J.R. (2015). Role of striatal cholinergic interneurons in set-shifting in the rat. J. Neurosci. 35: 9424–9431, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.0490-15.2015.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Albin, R.L., Young, A.B., and Penney, J.B. (1989). The functional anatomy of basal ganglia disorders. Trends Neurosci. 12: 366–375, https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(89)90074-x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Aldridge, J.W. and Berridge, K.C. (1998). Coding of serial order by neostriatal neurons: a “natural action” approach to movement sequence. J. Neurosci. 18: 2777–2787, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.18-07-02777.1998.Search in Google Scholar

Aldridge, J.W., Berridge, K.C., Herman, M., and Zimmer, L. (1993). Neuronal coding of serial order: syntax of grooming in the neostriatum. Psychol. Sci. 4: 391–395, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00587.x.Search in Google Scholar

Aldridge, J.W., Berridge, K.C., and Rosen, A.R. (2004). Basal ganglia neural mechanisms of natural movement sequences. Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 82: 732–739, https://doi.org/10.1139/y04-061.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Alexander, G.E. and Crutcher, M.D. (1990). Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: neural substrates of parallel processing. Trends Neurosci. 13: 266–271, https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(90)90107-l.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Al Tannir, R., Pautrat, A., Baufreton, J., Overton, P.G., and Coizet, V. (2023). The subthalamic nucleus: a hub for sensory control via short three-lateral loop connections with the brainstem? Curr. Neuropharmacol. 21: 22–30, https://doi.org/10.2174/1570159x20666220718113548.Search in Google Scholar

Anderson, J.J., Chase, T.N., and Engber, T.M. (1993). Substance P increases release of acetylcholine in the dorsal striatum of freely moving rats. Brain Res. 623: 189–194, https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(93)91426-s.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Aosaki, T. and Kawaguchi, Y. (1996). Actions of Substance P on rat neostriatal neurons in vitro. J. Neurosci. 16: 5141–5153, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.16-16-05141.1996.Search in Google Scholar

Bachá-Méndez, G., Reid, A.K., and Mendoza-Soylovna, A. (2007). Resurgence of integrated behavioral units. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 87: 5–24, https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2007.55-05.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Balleine, B.W., Liljeholm, M., and Ostlund, S.B. (2009). The integrative function of the basal ganglia in instrumental conditioning. Behav. Brain Res. 199: 43–52, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2008.10.034.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Bass, A.H. (2014). Central pattern generator for vocalization: is there a vertebrate morphotype? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 28: 94–100, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2014.06.012.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Berkowitz, A., Roberts, A., and Soffe, S.R. (2010). Roles for multifunctional and specialized spinal interneurons during motor pattern generation in tadpoles, zebrafish larvae, and turtles. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 4: 36, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00036.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Berridge, K.C. (1989). Progressive degradation of serial grooming chains by descending decerebration. Behav. Brain Res. 33: 241–253, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0166-4328(89)80119-6.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Berridge, K.C. (1990). Comparative fine structure of action: rules of form and sequence in the grooming patterns of six rodent species. Behaviour 113: 21–56, https://doi.org/10.1163/156853990x00428.Search in Google Scholar

Berridge, K.C. and Fentress, J.C. (1987a). Deafferentation does not disrupt natural rules of action syntax. Behav. Brain Res. 23: 69–76, https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(87)90243-9.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Berridge, K.C., Fentress, J.C., and Parr, H. (1987). Natural syntax rules control action sequence of rats. Behav. Brain Res. 23: 59–68, https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(87)90242-7.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Berridge, K.C. and Fentress, J.C. (1987b). Disruption of natural grooming chains after striatopallidal lesions. Psychobiology 15: 336–342, https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03327290.Search in Google Scholar

Berridge, K.C. and Whishaw, I.Q. (1992). Cortex, striatum and cerebellum: control of serial order in a grooming sequence. Exp. Brain Res. 90: 275–290, https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00227239.Search in Google Scholar

Berridge, K.C. and Aldridge, J.W. (2000). Super-stereotypy I: enhancement of a complex movement sequence by systemic dopamine D1 agonists. Synapse 37: 194–204, https://doi.org/10.1002/1098-2396(20000901)37:3<194::aid-syn3>3.0.co;2-a.10.1002/1098-2396(20000901)37:3<194::AID-SYN3>3.0.CO;2-ASearch in Google Scholar

Berridge, K.C., Aldridge, J.W., Houchard, K.R., and Zhuang, X. (2005). Sequential super-stereotypy of an instinctive fixed action pattern in hyper-dopaminergic mutant mice: a model of obsessive-compulsive disorder and Tourette’s. BMC Biol. 3: 4, https://doi.org/10.1186/1741-7007-3-4.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Bertram, R., Daou, A., Hyson, R.L., Johnson, F., and Wu, W. (2014). Two neural streams, one voice: pathways for theme and variation in the songbird brain. Neuroscience 277: 806–817, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.07.061.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Blomeley, C. and Bracci, E. (2008). Substance P depolarizes striatal projection neurons and facilitates their glutamatergic inputs. J. Physiol. 586: 2143–2155, https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2007.148965.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Blomeley, C.P., Kehoe, L.A., and Bracci, E. (2009). Substance P mediates excitatory interactions between striatal projection neurons. J. Neurosci. 29: 4953–4963, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.6020-08.2009.Search in Google Scholar

Bolam, J.P., Hanley, J.J., Booth, P.A.C., and Bevan, M.D. (2000). Synaptic organisation of the basal ganglia. J. Anat. 196: 527–542, https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1469-7580.2000.19640527.x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Bolam, J.P., Ingham, C.A., Izzo, P.N., Levey, A.I., Rye, D.B., Smith, A.D., and Wainer, B.H. (1986). Substance P-Containing terminals in synaptic contact with cholinergic neurons in the neostriatum and basal forebrain: a double immunocytochemical study in the rat. Brain Res. 397: 279–289, https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(86)90629-3.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Bolam, J.P. and Izzo, P.N. (1988). The postsynaptic targets of substance P-immunoreactive terminals in the rat neostriatum with particular reference to identified spiny striatonigral neurons. Exp. Brain Res. 70: 361–377, https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00248361.Search in Google Scholar

Bottjer, S.W., Halsema, K.A., Brown, S.A., and Miesner, E.A. (1989). Axonal connections of a forebrain nucleus involved with vocal learning in zebra finches. J. Comp. Neurol. 279: 312–326, https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.902790211.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Boyd, L.A., Edwards, J.D., Siengsukon, C.S., Vidoni, E.D., Wessel, B.D., and Linsdell, M.A. (2009). Motor sequence chunking is impaired by basal ganglia stroke. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 92: 35–44, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2009.02.009.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Brainard, M.S. and Doupe, A.J. (2000). Auditory feedback in learning and maintenance of vocal behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 1: 31–40, https://doi.org/10.1038/35036205.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Brainard, M.S. and Doupe, A.J. (2013). Translating birdsong: songbirds as a model for basic and applied medical research. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 36: 489–517, https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-152826.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Brimblecombe, K.R. and Cragg, S.J. (2015). Substance P weights striatal dopamine transmission differently within the striosome-matrix axis. J. Neurosci. 35: 9017–9023, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.0870-15.2015.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Bucher, D., Haspel, G., Golowasch, J., and Nadim, F. (2015). Central pattern generators. In: eLS. Wiley, Chichester, UK, pp. 1–12.10.1002/9780470015902.a0000032.pub2Search in Google Scholar

Buxton, D., Bracci, E., Overton, P.G., and Gurney, K. (2017). Striatal neuropeptides enhance selection and rejection of sequential actions. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 11: 62. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2017.00062.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Cacciatore, T.W., Rozenshteyn, R., and Kristan, W.B.Jr. (2000). Kinematics and modeling of leech crawling: evidence for an oscillatory behavior produced by propagating waves of excitation. J. Neurosci. 20: 1643–1655, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.20-04-01643.2000.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Calabresi, P., Maj, R., Pisani, A., Mercuri, N., and Bernardi, G. (1992). Long-term synaptic depression in the striatum: physiological and pharmacological characterization. J. Neurosci. 12: 4224–4233, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.12-11-04224.1992.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Calabresi, P., Picconi, B., Tozzi, A., Ghiglieri, V., and Di Filippo, M. (2014). Direct and indirect pathways of basal ganglia: a critical reappraisal. Nat. Neurosci. 17: 1022–1030, https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3743.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Carrillo-Reid, L., Tecuapetla, F., Tapia, D., Hernández-Cruz, A., Galarraga, E., Drucker-Colin, R., and Bargas, J. (2008). Encoding network states by striatal cell assemblies. J. Neurophysiol. 99: 1435–1450, https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01131.2007.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Casarrubea, M., Di Giovanni, G., Crescimanno, G., Rosa, I., Aiello, S., Di Censo, D., Ranieri, B., Santangelo, A., Busatta, D., Cassioli, E., et al.. (2019). Effects of substantia nigra pars compacta lesion on the behavioral sequencing in the 6-OHDA model of Parkinson’s disease. Behav. Brain Res. 362: 28–35, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2019.01.004.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Chen, J.R., Stepanek, L., and Doupe, A.J. (2014). Differential contributions of basal ganglia and thalamus to song initiation, tempo, and structure. J. Neurophysiol. 111: 248–257, https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00584.2012.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Chen, Liang-Wei, Wei, L.-C., Liu, H.-L., Qiu, Y., and Chan, Y.-S. (2001). Cholinergic neurons expressing substance P receptor (NK1) in the basal forebrain of the rat: a double immunocytochemical study. Brain Res. 904: 161–166, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0006-8993(01)02460-x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Chen, L.-W., Cao, R., Liu, H.-L., Ju, G., and Chan, Y.S. (2003). The striatal GABAergic neurons expressing substance P receptors in the basal ganglia of mice. Neuroscience 119: 919–925, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0306-4522(03)00223-9.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Chevaleyre, V. and Castillo, P.E. (2003). Heterosynaptic LTD of hippocampal GABAergic synapses: a novel role of endocannabinoids in regulating excitability. Neuron 38: 461–472, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00235-6.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Collins, A.L., Greenfield, V.Y., Bye, J.K., Linker, K.E., Wang, A.S., and Wassum, K.M. (2016). Dynamic mesolimbic dopamine signalling during action sequence learning and expectation violation. Sci. Rep. 6: 20231, https://doi.org/10.1038/srep20231.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Crittenden, J.R. and Graybiel, A.M. (2011). Basal ganglia disorders associated with imbalances in the striatal striosome and matrix compartments. Front. Neuroanat. 5: 59, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnana.2011.00059.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Cromwell, H.C. and Berridge, K.C. (1996). Implementation of action sequences by a neostriatal site: a lesion mapping study of grooming syntax. J. Neurosci. 16: 3444–3458, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.16-10-03444.1996.Search in Google Scholar

Cui, G., Jun, S.B., Jin, X., Pham, M.D., Vogel, S.S., Lovinger, D.M., and Costa, R.M. (2013). Concurrent activation of striatal direct and indirect pathways during action initiation. Nature 494: 238–242, https://doi.org/10.1038/nature11846.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Czubayko, U. and Plenz, D. (2002). Fast synaptic transmission between striatal spiny projection neurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 99, 15764–15769, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.242428599.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Dang, M.T., Yokoi, F., Yin, H.H., Lovinger, D.M., Wang, Y., and Li, Y. (2006). Disrupted motor learning and long-term synaptic plasticity in mice lacking NMDAR1 in the striatum. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 103, 15254–15259, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0601758103.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Dautan, D., Hacioğlu Bay, H., Bolam, J.P., Gerdjikov, T.V., and Mena-Segovia, J. (2016). Extrinsic sources of cholinergic innervation of the striatal complex: a whole-brain mapping analysis. Front. Neuroanat. 10: 1, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnana.2016.00001.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

DeLong, M.R. (1990). Primate models of movement disorders of basal ganglia origin. Trends Neurosci. 13: 281–285, https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(90)90110-v.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Dezfouli, A. and Balleine, B.W. (2012). Habits, action sequences and reinforcement learning. Eur. J. Neurosci. 35: 1036–1051, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08050.x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Dezfouli, A., Lingawi, N.W., and Balleine, B.W. (2014). Habits as action sequences: hierarchical action control and changes in outcome value. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 369: 20130482, https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0482.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Dhawale, A.K., Wolff, S.B.E., Ko, R., and Ölveczky, B.P. (2021). The basal ganglia control the detailed kinematics of learned motor skills. Nat. Neurosci. 24: 1256–1269, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-021-00889-3.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Díaz-Hernández, E., Contreras-López, R., Sánchez-Fuentes, A., Rodríguez-Sibrían, L., Ramírez-Jarquín, J.O., and Tecuapetla, F. (2018). The thalamostriatal projections contribute to the initiation and execution of a sequence of movements. Neuron 100: 739.e5–752.e5, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.052.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Doig, N.M., Moss, J., and Bolam, J.P. (2010). Cortical and thalamic innervation of direct and indirect pathway medium-sized spiny neurons in mouse striatum. J. Neurosci. 30: 14610–14618, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1623-10.2010.Search in Google Scholar

Dong, J., Hawes, S., Wu, J., Le, W., and Cai, H. (2021). Connectivity and functionality of the globus pallidus externa under normal conditions and Parkinson’s disease. Front. Neural Circ. 15: 645287, https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2021.645287.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Drummond, H. (1981). The nature and description of behavior patterns. In: Bateson, P.P.G., and Klopfer, P.H. (Eds.), Perspectives in ethology. Advantages of diversity. Plenum Press, New York, pp. 1–33.10.1007/978-1-4615-7575-7_1Search in Google Scholar

Duffy, R.A., Varty, G.B., Morgan, C.A., and Lachowicz, J.E. (2002). Correlation of neurokinin (NK) 1 receptor occupancy in gerbil striatum with behavioral effects of NK1 antagonists. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 301: 536–542, https://doi.org/10.1124/jpet.301.2.536.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Favila, N., Gurney, K., and Overton, P.G. (2021). Blocking NK1 receptors disrupts the sequential and temporal organization of chain grooming in rats. Neuropharmacology 196: 108716, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2021.108716.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Favila, N., Gurney, K., and Overton, P.G. (2023). The NK1 antagonist L-733,060 facilitates sequence learning. J. Psychopharmacol. 37: 610–626, https://doi.org/10.1177/02698811231161582.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Fee, M.S. and Scharff, C. (2010). The songbird as a model for the generation and learning of complex sequential behaviors. ILAR J. 51: 362–377, https://doi.org/10.1093/ilar.51.4.362.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Fisher, S.D., Robertson, P.B., Black, M.J., Redgrave, P., Sagar, M.A., Wickliffe, C.A., and Reynolds, J.N.H. (2017). Reinforcement determines the timing dependence of corticostriatal synaptic plasticity in vivo. Nat. Commun. 8: 334, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-017-00394-x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Foster, N.N., Barry, J., Korobkova, L., Garcia, L., Gao, L., Becerra, M., Sherafat, Y., Peng, B., Li, X., Choi, J.H., et al.. (2021). The mouse cortico-basal ganglia-thalamic network. Nature 598: 188–194, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-03993-3.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Francis, T.C., Yano, H., Demarest, T.G., Shen, H., and Bonci, A. (2019). High-frequency activation of nucleus accumbens D1-MSNs drives excitatory potentiation on D2-MSNs. Neuron 103: 432.e3–444.e3, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2019.05.031.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Freund, T.F., Powell, J.F., and Smith, A.D. (1984). Tyrosine hydroxylase-immunoreactive boutons in synaptic contact with identified striatonigral neurons, with particular reference to dendritic spines. Neuroscience 13: 1189–1215, https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4522(84)90294-x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Frigon, A. and Gossard, J.P. (2010). Evidence for specialized rhythm-generating mechanisms in the adult mammalian spinal cord. J. Neurosci. 30: 7061–7071, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.0450-10.2010.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Fu, W.-T. and Anderson, J.R. (2008). Solving the credit assignment problem: explicit and implicit learning of action sequences with probabilistic outcomes. Psychol. Res. 72: 321–330, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-007-0113-7.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Fujii, N. and Graybiel, A.M. (2003). Representation of action sequence boundaries by macaque prefrontal cortical neurons. Science 301: 1246–1249, https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1086872.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Gardner, T.J., Naef, F., and Nottebohm, F. (2005). Freedom and rules: the acquisition and reprogramming of a bird’s learned song. Science 308: 1046–1049, https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1108214.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Gauchy, C., Desban, M., Glowinski, J., and Kemel, M.L. (1996). Distinct regulations by septide and the neurokinin-1 tachykinin receptor agonist [pro9] substance P of the N-methyl-d-aspartate-evoked release of dopamine in striosome- and matrix-enriched areas of the rat striatum. Neuroscience 73: 929–939, https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4522(96)00099-1.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Geddes, C.E., Li, H., and Jin, X. (2018). Optogenetic editing reveals the hierarchical organization of learned action sequences. Cell 174: 32–43, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2018.06.012.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Georgiou, N., Bradshaw, J.L., Iansek, R., Phillips, J.G., Mattingley, J.B., and Bradshaw, J.A. (1994). Reduction in external cues and movement sequencing in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 57: 368–370, https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.57.3.368.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Gerfen, C., Engber, T., Mahan, L., Susel, Z., Chase, T., Monsma, F., and Sibley, D. (1990). D1 and D2 dopamine receptor-regulated gene expression of striatonigral and striatopallidal neurons. Science 250: 1429–1432, https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2147780.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Gil, D. and Slater, P.J.B. (2000). Song organisation and singing patterns of the willow warbler Phylloscopus trochilus. Behaviour 137: 759–782, https://doi.org/10.1163/156853900502330.Search in Google Scholar

Gittis, A.H., Nelson, A.B., Thwin, M.T., Palop, J.J., and Kreitzer, A.C. (2010). Distinct roles of GABAergic interneurons in the regulation of striatal output pathways. J. Neurosci. 30: 2223–2234, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.4870-09.2010.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Gobet, F., Lane, P.C., Croker, S., Cheng, P.C., Jones, G., Oliver, I., and Pine, J.M. (2001). Chunking mechanisms in human learning. Trends Cognit. Sci. 5: 236–243, https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01662-4.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Gonzalez-Nicolini, V. and McGinty, J.F. (2002). NK-1 receptor blockade decreases amphetamine-induced behavior and neuropeptide mRNA expression in the striatum. Brain Res. 931: 41–49, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0006-8993(02)02250-3.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Graybiel, A.M. (1998). The basal ganglia and chunking of action repertoires. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 70: 119–136, https://doi.org/10.1006/nlme.1998.3843.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Graybiel, A.M. (2000). The basal ganglia. Curr. Biol. 10: R509–R511, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0960-9822(00)00593-5.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Graybiel, A.M. and Grafton, S.T. (2015). The striatum: where skills and habits meet. Cold Spring Harbor Perspect. Biol. 7: a021691, https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a021691.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Grillner, S. (2003). The motor infrastructure: from ion channels to neuronal networks. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4: 573–586, https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1137.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Grillner, S. (2006). Biological Pattern Generation: the cellular and computational logic of networks in motion. Neuron 52: 751–766, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.008.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Grillner, S., Hellgren, J., Menard, A., Saitoh, K., and Wikstrom, M. (2005). Mechanisms for selection of basic motor programs – roles for the striatum and pallidum. Trends Neurosci. 28: 364–370, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2005.05.004.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Grillner, S. and Wallén, P. (2002). Cellular bases of a vertebrate locomotor system–steering, intersegmental and segmental co-ordination and sensory control. Brain Res. Rev. 40: 92–106, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0165-0173(02)00193-5.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Grillner, S. and Wallén, P. (2004). Innate versus learned movements—a false dichotomy? Prog. Brain Res. 143: 3–12, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0079-6123(03)43001-x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Guo, Q., Wang, D., He, X., Feng, Q., Lin, R., Xu, F., Fu, L., and Luo, M. (2015). Whole-brain mapping of inputs to projection neurons and cholinergic interneurons in the dorsal striatum. PLoS One 10: e0123381, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0123381.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Guzman, J.N., Hernandez, A., Galarraga, E., Tapia, D., Laville, A., Vergara, R., Aceves, J., and Bargas, J. (2003). Dopaminergic modulation of axon collaterals interconnecting spiny neurons of the rat striatum. J. Neurosci. 23: 8931–8940, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.23-26-08931.2003.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Gygi, S.P., Gibb, J.W., Johnson, M., and Hanson, G.R. (1993). Blockade of tachykinin NK1 receptors by CP-96345 enhances dopamine release and the striatal dopamine effects of methamphetamine in rats. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 250: 177–180, https://doi.org/10.1016/0014-2999(93)90639-y.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Hahnloser, R.H.R., Kozhevnikov, A.A., and Fee, M.S. (2002). An ultra-sparse code underlies the generation of neural sequences in a songbird. Nature 419: 65–70, https://doi.org/10.1038/nature00974.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Hall, M.E., Grantham, P., Limoli, J., and Stewart, J.M. (1987). Effects of substance P and neurokinin A (substance K) on motor behavior: unique effect of substance P attributable to its amino-terminal sequence. Brain Res. 420: 82–94, https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(87)90242-3.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Harrington, D.L. and Haaland, K.Y. (1991). Sequencing in Parkinson’s disease. Abnormalities in programming and controlling movement. Brain 1: 99–115.Search in Google Scholar

Hökfelt, T., Broberger, C., Xu, Z.Q., Sergeyev, V., Ubink, R., and Diez, M. (2000). Neuropeptides–an overview. Neuropharmacology 39: 1337–1356, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3908(00)00010-1.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Hooks, B.M., Papale, A.E., Paletzki, R.F., Feroze, M.W., Eastwood, B.S., Couey, J.J., Winnubst, J., Chandrashekar, J., and Gerfen, C.R. (2018). Topographic precision in sensory and motor corticostriatal projections varies across cell type and cortical area. Nat. Commun. 3: 3549, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-05780-7.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Horner, K.A., Hebbard, J.C., Logan, A.S., Vanchipurakel, G.A., and Gilbert, Y.E. (2012). Activation of mu opioid receptors in the striatum differentially augments methamphetamine-induced gene expression and enhances stereotypic behavior. J. Neurochem. 120: 779–794, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-4159.2011.07620.x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Hughes, G. and Wiersma, C. (1960). The co-ordination of swimmeret movements in the crayfish, Procambarus clarkii (Girard). J. Exp. Biol. 37: 657–670, https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.37.4.657.Search in Google Scholar

Hughes, M., Hultsch, H., and Todt, D. (2002). Imitation and invention in song learning in nightingales (Luscinia megarhynchos B., Turdidae). Ethology 108: 97–113, https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1439-0310.2002.00720.x.Search in Google Scholar

Hultsch, H. (1992). Time window and unit capacity: dual constraints on the acquisition of serial information in songbirds. J. Comp. Physiol. A 170: 275–280, https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00191415.Search in Google Scholar

Hultsch, H. and Todt, D. (1989). Memorization and reproduction of songs in nightingales (Luscinia megarhynchos): evidence for package formation. J. Comp. Physiol. A 165: 197–203, https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00619194.Search in Google Scholar

Huston, J.P. and Hasenöhrl, R.U. (1995). The role of neuropeptides in learning: focus on the neurokinin substance P. Behav. Brain Res. 66: 117–127, https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(94)00132-y.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Ingham, C.A., Hood, S.H., and Arbuthnott, G.W. (1991). A light and electron microscopical study of enkephalin-immunoreactive structures in the rat neostriatum after removal of the nigrostriatal dopaminergic pathway. Neuroscience 42: 715–730, https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4522(91)90040-u.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Jaeger, D., Kita, H., and Wilson, C.J. (1994). Surround inhibition among projection neurons is weak or nonexistent in the rat neostriatum. Neurophysiology 72: 2555–2558, https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1994.72.5.2555.Search in Google Scholar

Jakab, R. and Goldman-Rakic, P. (1996). Presynaptic and postsynaptic subcellular localization of substance P receptor immunoreactivity in the neostriatum of the rat and rhesus monkey (Macaca Mulatta). J. Comp. Neurol. 369: 125–136, https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19960520)369:1<125::aid-cne9>3.0.co;2-5.10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19960520)369:1<125::AID-CNE9>3.0.CO;2-5Search in Google Scholar

Janacsek, K., Shattuck, K.F., Tagarelli, K.M., Lum, J.A.G., Turkeltaub, P.E., and Ullman, M.T. (2020). Sequence learning in the human brain: a functional neuroanatomical meta-analysis of serial reaction time studies. NeuroImage 207: 116387, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116387.Search in Google Scholar

Jarvis, E.D., Scharff, C., Grossman, M.R., Ramos, J.A., and Nottebohm, F. (1998). For whom the bird sings. Neuron 21: 775–788, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80594-2.Search in Google Scholar

Jin, X. and Costa, R.M. (2010). Start/stop signals emerge in nigrostriatal circuits during sequence learning. Nature 466: 457–462, https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09263.Search in Google Scholar

Jin, X. and Costa, R.M. (2015). Shaping action sequences in basal ganglia circuits. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 33: 188–196, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2015.06.011.Search in Google Scholar

Jin, X., Tecuapetla, F., and Costa, R.M. (2014). Basal ganglia subcircuits distinctively encode the parsing and concatenation of action sequences. Nat. Neurosci. 17: 423–430, https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3632.Search in Google Scholar

Jog, M.S., Kubota, Y., Connolly, C.I., Hillegaart, V., and Graybiel, A.M. (1999). Building neural representations of habits. Science 286: 1745–1749, https://doi.org/10.1126/science.286.5445.1745.Search in Google Scholar

Kalueff, A.V., Aldridge, J.W., LaPorte, J.L., Murphy, D.L., and Tuohimaa, P. (2007). Analyzing grooming microstructure in neurobehavioral experiments. Nat. Protoc. 2: 2538–2544, https://doi.org/10.1038/nprot.2007.367.Search in Google Scholar

Kao, M.H., Doupe, A.J., and Brainard, M.S. (2005). Contributions of an avian basal ganglia–forebrain circuit to real-time modulation of song. Nature 433: 638–643, https://doi.org/10.1038/nature03127.Search in Google Scholar

Katz, R.J. and Gelbart, J. (1978). Endogenous opiates and behavioral responses to environmental novelty. Behav. Biol. 24: 338–348, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0091-6773(79)90197-4.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Kawai, R., Markman, T., Poddar, R., Ko, R., Fantana, A.L., Dhawale, A.K., Kampff, A.R., and Ölveczky, B.P. (2015). Motor cortex is required for learning but not for executing a motor skill. Neuron 86: 800–812, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.024.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Kerner, J.A., Standaert, D.G., Penney, J.B.Jr, Young, A.B., and Landwehrmeyer, G.B. (1997). Expression of group one metabotropic glutamate receptor subunit mRNAs in neurochemically identified neurons in the rat neostriatum, neocortex, and hippocampus. Mol. Brain Res. 48: 259–269, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0169-328x(97)00102-2.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Kertes, E., László, K., Berta, B., and Lénárd, L. (2010). Positive reinforcing effects of substance P in the rat globus pallidus revealed by conditioned place preference. Behav. Brain Res. 215: 152–155, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.06.027.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Kraft, M., Noailles, P., and Angulo, J.A. (2001). Substance P modulates cocaine-evoked dopamine overflow in the striatum of the rat brain. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 937: 121–131, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb03561.x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Krolewski, D.M., Bishop, C., and Walker, P.D. (2005). Intrastriatal dopamine D1 receptor agonist-mediated motor behavior is reduced by local neurokinin 1 receptor antagonism. Synapse 57: 1–7, https://doi.org/10.1002/syn.20148.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Levesque, M., Bedard, M.A., Courtemanche, R., Tremblay, P.L., Scherzer, P., and Blanchet, P.J. (2007). Raclopride-induced motor consolidation impairment in primates: role of the dopamine type-2 receptor in movement chunking into integrated sequences. Exp. Brain Res. 182: 499–508, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-1010-4.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Lipton, D.M., Gonzales, B.J., and Citri, A. (2019). Dorsal striatal circuits for habits, compulsions and additions. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 13: 28, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2019.00028.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Liu, W.-C. and Nottebohm, F. (2007). A learning program that ensures prompt and versatile vocal imitation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 104, 20398–20403, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0710067104.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Long, M.A. and Fee, M.S. (2008). Using temperature to analyse temporal dynamics in the songbird motor pathway. Nature 456: 189–194, https://doi.org/10.1038/nature07448.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Long, M.A., Jin, D.Z., and Fee, M.S. (2010). Support for a synaptic chain model of neuronal sequence generation. Nature 468: 394–399, https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09514.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Mallet, N., Le Moine, C., Charpier, S., and Gonon, F. (2005). Feedforward inhibition of projection neurons by fast-spiking GABA interneurons in the rat striatum in vivo. J. Neurosci. 25: 3857–3869, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.5027-04.2005.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Mallet, N., Micklem, B.R., Henny, P., Brown, M.T., Williams, C., Bolam, J.P., Nakamura, K.C., and Magill, P.J. (2012). Dichotomous organization of the external globus pallidus. Neuron 74: 1075–1086, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.027.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Mallet, N., Schmidt, R., Leventhal, D., Chen, F., Amer, N., Boraud, T., and Berke, J.D. (2016). Arkypallidal cells send a stop signal to striatum. Neuron 89: 308–316, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.017.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Mandelbaum, G., Taranda, J., Haynes, T.M., Hochbaum, D.R., Huang, K.W., Hyun, M., Venkataraju, K.U., Straub, C., Wang, W., Robertson, K., et al.. (2019). Distinct cortical-thalamic-striatal circuits through the parafascicular nucleus. Neuron 102: 636–652.e7, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2019.02.035.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Marder, E. (2000). Motor pattern generation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 10: 691–698, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0959-4388(00)00157-4.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Marder, Eve and Bucher, D. (2001). Central pattern generators and the control of rhythmic movements. Curr. Biol. 11: R986–R996, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0960-9822(01)00581-4.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Markowitz, J.E., Ivie, E., Kligler, L., and Gardner, T.J. (2013). Long-range order in canary song. PLoS Comput. Biol. 9: e1003052, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003052.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Martiros, N., Burgess, A.A., and Graybiel, A.M. (2018). Inversely active striatal projection neurons and interneurons selectively delimit useful behavioral sequences. Curr. Biol. 28: 560–573.e5, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.01.031.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Matheson, A.M.M. and Sakata, J.T. (2015). Relationship between the sequencing and timing of vocal motor elements in birdsong. PLoS One 10: e0143203, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143203.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Matsuda, W., Furuta, T., Nakamura, K.C., Hioki, H., Fujiyama, F., Arai, R., and Kaneko, T. (2009). Single nigrostriatal dopaminergic neurons form widely spread and highly dense axonal arborizations in the neostriatum. J. Neurosci. 29: 444–453, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.4029-08.2009.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

McElvain, L.E., Chen, Y., Moore, J.D., Brigidi, G.S., Bloodgood, B.L., Lim, B.K., Costa, R.M., and Kleinfeld, D. (2021). Specific populations of basal ganglia output neurons target distinct brain stem areas while collateralizing throughout the diencephalon. Neuron 109: 1721–1738, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2021.03.017.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

McHaffie, J., Stanford, T., Stein, B., Coizet, V., and Redgrave, P. (2005). Subcortical loops through the basal ganglia. Trends Neurosci. 28: 401–407, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2005.06.006.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Meyer-Luehmann, M., Thompson, J.F., Berridge, K.C., and Aldridge, J.W. (2002). Substantia nigra pars reticulata neurons code initiation of a serial pattern: implications for natural action sequences and sequential disorders. Eur. J. Neurosci. 16: 1599–1608, https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02210.x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Miller, G.A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol. Rev. 63: 81–97, https://doi.org/10.1037/h0043158.Search in Google Scholar

Miller, R.J. and Cuatrecasas, P. (1978). The enkephalins. Naturwissenschaften 65: 507–514, https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00439790.Search in Google Scholar

Mooney, R. (2009). Neurobiology of song learning. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19: 654–660, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2009.10.004.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Murray, J.M. and Escola, G.S. (2017). Learning multiple variable-speed sequences in striatum via cortical tutoring. eLife 6: e26084, https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.26084.Search in Google Scholar

Nakamura, T., Nagata, M., Yagi, T., Graybiel, A.M., Yamamori, T., and Kitsukawa, T. (2017). Learning new sequential stepping patterns requires striatal plasticity during the earliest phase of acquisition. Eur. J. Neurosci. 45: 901–911, https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13537.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Nottebohm, F. (2005). The neural basis of birdsong. PLoS Biol. 3: e164, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030164.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Nottebohm, F., Stokes, T.M., and Leonard, C.M. (1976). Central control of song in the canary, Serinus canarius. J. Comp. Neurol. 165: 457–486, https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.901650405.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

O’Hare, J.K., Li, H., Kim, N., Gaidis, E., Ade, K., Beck, J., Yin, H., and Calakos, N. (2017). Striatal fast-spiking interneurons selectively modulate circuit output and are required for habitual behavior. eLife 6: e26231, https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.26231.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Ölveczky, B.P., Andalman, A.S., and Fee, M.S. (2005). Vocal experimentation in the juvenile songbird requires a basal ganglia circuit. PLoS Biol 3: e153, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030153.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Ostlund, S.B., Winterbauer, N.E., and Balleine, B.W. (2009). Evidence of action sequence chunking in goal-directed instrumental conditioning and its dependence on the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 29: 8280–8287, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1176-09.2009.Search in Google Scholar

Parker, D., Zhang, W., and Grillner, S. (1998). Substance P modulates NMDA responses and causes long-term protein synthesis-dependent modulation of the lamprey locomotor network. J. Neurosci. 18: 4800–4813, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.18-12-04800.1998.Search in Google Scholar

Pearson, K.G. and Wolf, H. (1987). Comparison of motor patterns in the intact and deafferented flight system of the locust. J. Comp. Physiol. 160: 269–279, https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00609732.Search in Google Scholar

Pelosi, A., Girault, J.-A., and Hervé, D. (2015). Unilateral lesion of dopamine neurons induces grooming asymmetry in the mouse. PLoS One 10: e0137185, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137185.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Penhune, V.B. and Steele, C.J. (2012). Parallel contributions of cerebellar, striatal and M1 mechanisms to motor sequence learning. Behav. Brain Res. 226: 579–591, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2011.09.044.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Porter, A.J., Pillidge, K., Tsai, Y.C., Dudley, J.A., Hunt, S.P., Peirson, S.N., Brown, L.A., and Stanford, S.C. (2015). A lack of functional NK1 receptors explains most, but not all, abnormal behaviours of NK1R−/− mice1. Gene Brain Behav. 14: 189–199, https://doi.org/10.1111/gbb.12195.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Ramakrishnan, S., Arnett, B., and Murphy, A.D. (2014). Contextual modulation of a multifunctional central pattern generator. J. Exp. Biol. 217: 3935–3944, https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.086751.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Redgrave, P., Prescott, T.J., and Gurney, K. (1999). The basal ganglia: a vertebrate solution to the selection problem? Neuroscience 89: 1009–1023, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0306-4522(98)00319-4.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Rekling, J.C. and Feldman, J.L. (1998). Prebötzinger complex and pacemaker neurons: hypothesized site and kernel for respiratory rhythm generation. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 60: 385–405, https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.physiol.60.1.385.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Reynolds, J.N.J. and Wickens, J.R. (2002). Dopamine-dependent plasticity of corticostriatal synapses. Neural Network. 15: 507–521, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0893-6080(02)00045-x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Ribeiro-da-Silva, A. and Hökfelt, T. (2000). Neuroanatomical localisation of Substance P in the CNS and sensory neurons. Neuropeptides 34: 256–271, https://doi.org/10.1054/npep.2000.0834.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Rothwell, P.E., Hayton, S.J., Sun, G.L., Fuccillo, M.V., Lim, B.K., and Malenka, R.C. (2015). Input- and output-specific regulation of serial order performance by corticostriatal circuits. Neuron 88: 345–356, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.035.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Roseberry, T.K., Lee, A.M., Lalive, A.L., Wilbrecht, L., Bonci, A., and Kreitzer, A.C. (2016). Cell-type-specific control of brainstem locomotor circuits by basal ganglia. Cell 164: 526–537, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2015.12.037.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Rupniak, N.M.J. and Kramer, M.S. (2002). Substance P and related tachykinins. In: Neuropsychopharmacology – 5th Generation of progress. American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, pp. 169–177.Search in Google Scholar

Sakai, K., Kitaguchi, K., and Hikosaka, O. (2003). Chunking during human visuomotor sequence learning. Exp. Brain Res. 152: 229–242, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-003-1548-8.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Sakata, J.T. and Brainard, M.S. (2006). Real-time contributions of auditory feedback to avian vocal motor control. J. Neurosci. 26: 9619–9628, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.2027-06.2006.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Sakata, J.T. and Vehrencamp, S.L. (2011). Integrating perspectives on vocal performance and consistency. J. Exp. Biol. 215: 201–209, https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.056911.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Sakurai, A. and Katz, P.S. (2016). The central pattern generator underlying swimming in Dendronotus iris: a simple half-center network oscillator with a twist. J. Neurophysiol. 116: 1728–1742, https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00150.2016.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Satterlie, R.A. (1985). Reciprocal inhibition and postinhibitory rebound produce reverberation in a locomotor pattern generator. Science 229: 402–404, https://doi.org/10.1126/science.229.4711.402.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Satterlie, R.A. and Nolen, T. (2001). Why do cubomedusae have only four swim pacemakers? J. Exp. Biol. 204: 1413–1419, https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.204.8.1413.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Savalia, T., Shukla, A., and Bapi, R.S. (2016). A unified theoretical framework for cognitive sequencing. Front. Psychol. 7: 1821, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01821.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Scharff, C. and Nottebohm, F. (1991). A comparative study of the behavioral deficits following lesions of various parts of the zebra finch song system: implications for vocal learning. J. Neurosci. 11: 2896–2913, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.11-09-02896.1991.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Schönberger, A.R., Hagelweide, K., Pelzer, E.A., Fink, G.R., and Schubotz, R.I. (2015). Motor loop dysfunction causes impaired cognitive sequencing in patients suffering from Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 77: 409–420, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.09.017.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Shepherd, G.M. (2013). Corticostriatal connectivity and its role in disease. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14: 278–291, https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3469.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Shults, C.W., Quirion, R., Chronwall, B., Chase, T.N., and O’Donohue, T.L. (1984). A comparison of the anatomical distribution of substance P and substance P receptors in the rat central nervous system. Peptides 5: 1097–1128, https://doi.org/10.1016/0196-9781(84)90177-3.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Silberberg, G. and Bolam, J.P. (2015). Local and afferent synaptic pathways in the striatal microcircuitry. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 33: 182–187, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2015.05.002.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Smith, K.S. and Graybiel, A.M. (2013). A dual operator view of habitual behavior reflecting cortical and striatal dynamics. Neuron 79: 361–374, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.05.038.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Smith, K.S. and Graybiel, A.M. (2016). Habit formation. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 18, 33–43. https://doi.org/10.31887/dcns.2016.18.1/ksmith.Search in Google Scholar

Solopchuk, O., Alamia, A., Olivier, E., and Zénon, A. (2016). Chunking improves symbolic sequence processing and relies on working memory gating mechanisms. Learn. Mem. 23: 108–112, https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.041277.115.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Stern, E.A., Kincaid, A.E., and Wilson, C.J. (1997). Spontaneous subthreshold membrane potential fluctuations and action potential variability of rat corticostriatal and striatal neurons in vivo. J. Neurophysiol. 77: 1697–1715, https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1997.77.4.1697.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Sutton, R.S. and Barto, A.G. (1998). Reinforcement learning: an introduction. The MIT Press, Cambridge.10.1109/TNN.1998.712192Search in Google Scholar

Takahasi, M., Yamada, H., and Okanoya, K. (2010). Statistical and prosodic cues for song segmentation learning by bengalese finches (Lonchura striata var. domestica). Ethology 116: 481–489, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01772.x.Search in Google Scholar

Tartaglione, A.M., Armida, M., Potenza, R.L., Pezzola, A., Popoli, P., and Calamandrei, G. (2016). Aberrant self-grooming as early marker of motor dysfunction in a rat model of Huntington’s disease. Behav. Brain Res. 313: 53–57, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2016.06.058.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Taylor, J.L., Rajbhandari, A.K., Berridge, K.C., and Aldridge, J.W. (2010). Dopamine receptor modulation of repetitive grooming actions in the rat: potential relevance for Tourette syndrome. Brain Res. 1322: 92–101, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2010.01.052.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Tecuapetla, F., Jin, X., Lima, S.Q., and Costa, R.M. (2016). Complementary contributions of striatal projection pathways to action initiation and execution. Cell 166: 703–715, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2016.06.032.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Testa, C.M., Standaert, D.G., Young, A.B., and Penney, J.B.Jr. (1994). Metabotropic glutamate receptor mRNA expression in the basal ganglia of the rat. J. Neurosci. 14: 3005–3018, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.14-05-03005.1994.Search in Google Scholar

Thompson, J.A. and Johnson, F. (2007). HVC microlesions do not destabilize the vocal patterns of adult male zebra finches with prior ablation of LMAN. Dev. Neurobiol. 67: 205–218, https://doi.org/10.1002/dneu.20287.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Tinbergen, N. (1951). Behaviour as a reaction to external stimuli. In: The study of instinct. Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 15–56.Search in Google Scholar

Tremblay, P.-L., Bedard, M.-A., Langlois, D., Blanchet, P.J., Lemay, M., and Parent, M. (2010). Movement chunking during sequence learning is a dopamine-dependent process: a study conducted in Parkinson’s disease. Exp. Brain Res. 205: 375–385, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-010-2372-6.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Tremblay, L., Kemel, M.L., Desban, M., Gauchy, C., and Glowinski, J. (1992). Distinct presynaptic control of dopamine release in striosomal-and matrix-enriched areas of the rat striatum by selective agonists of NK1, NK2, and NK3 tachykinin receptors. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 89: 11214–11218, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.89.23.11214.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Tunstall, M.J., Oorschot, D.E., Kean, A., and Wickens, J.R. (2002). Inhibitory interactions between spiny projection neurons in the rat striatum. J. Neurophysiol. 88: 1263–1269, https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.2002.88.3.1263.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Van Wimersma Greidanus, T.B. and Maigret, C. (1988). Grooming behavior induced by substance P. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 154: 217–220, https://doi.org/10.1016/0014-2999(88)90102-1.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Veksler, V.D., Gluck, K.A., Myers, C.W., Harris, J., and Mielke, T. (2014). Alleviating the curse of dimensionality – a psychologically-inspired approach. Biological. Inspired Cognit. Architect. 10: 51–60, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bica.2014.11.007.Search in Google Scholar

Wall, N.R., De La Parra, M., Callaway, E.M., and Kreitzer, A.C. (2013). Differential Innervation of direct- and indirect-pathway striatal projection neurons. Neuron 79: 347–360, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.05.014.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Wassum, K.M., Ostlund, S.B., and Maidment, N.T. (2012). Phasic mesolimbic dopamine signaling precedes and predicts performance of a self-initiated action sequence task. Biol. Psychiatr. 71: 846–854, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.019.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Weir, R., Dudley, J., Yan, T., Grabowska, E., Peña-Oliver, Y., Ripley, T., Stephens, D.N., Stanford, S.C., and Hunt, S. (2013). The influence of test experience and NK1 receptor antagonists on the performance of NK1R-/- and wild type mice in the 5-Choice Serial Reaction-Time Task. J. Psychopharmacol. 28: 270–281, https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881113495722.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Wickens, J. (1997). Basal ganglia: structure and computations. Netw. Comput. Neural Syst. 8: R77–R109, https://doi.org/10.1088/0954-898x_8_4_001.Search in Google Scholar

Wickens, J.R., Begg, A.J., and Arbuthnott, G.W. (1996). Dopamine reverses the depression of rat corticostriatal synapses which normally follows high-frequency stimulation of cortex in vitro. Neuroscience 70: 1–5, https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4522(95)00436-m.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Williams, H. (2004). Birdsong and singing behavior. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1016: 1–30, https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1298.029.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Wu, T., Chan, P., and Hallett, M. (2010). Effective connectivity of neural networks in automatic movements in Parkinson’s disease. NeuroImage 49: 2581–2587, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.051.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Wymbs, N.F., Bassett, D.S., Mucha, P.J., Porter, M.A., and Grafton, S.T. (2012). Differential recruitment of the sensorimotor putamen and frontoparietal cortex during motor chunking in humans. Neuron 74: 936–946, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.038.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Xiao, L., Chattree, G., Oscos, F.G., Cao, M., Wanat, M.J., and Roberts, T.F. (2018). A basal ganglia circuit sufficient to guide birdsong learning. Neuron 98: 208.e5–221.e5, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.02.020.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Yan, T.C., Dudley, J.A., Weir, R.K., Grabowska, E.M., Peña-Oliver, Y., Ripley, T.L., Hunt, S.P., Stephens, D.N., and Stanford, S.C. (2011). Performance deficits of NK1 receptor knockout mice in the 5-choice serial reaction-time task: Effects of d-amphetamine, stress and time of day. PLoS ONE 6: e17586, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017586.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Yan, T., McQuillin, A., Thapar, A., Asherson, P., Hunt, S., Stanford, S., and Gurling, H. (2010). NK1(TACR1) receptor gene ‘knockout’ mouse phenotype predicts genetic association with ADHD. J. Psychopharmacol. 24: 27–38, https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881108100255.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Yin, H.H. (2010). The sensorimotor striatum is necessary for serial order learning. J. Neurosci. 30: 14719–14723, https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3989-10.2010.Search in Google Scholar

Yin, H.H. and Knowlton, B.J. (2006). The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7: 464–476, https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1919.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Yin, H.H., Mulcare, S.P., Hilário, M.R.F., Clouse, E., Holloway, T., Davis, M.I., Hansson, A.C., Lovinger, D.M., and Costa, R.M. (2009). Dynamic reorganization of striatal circuits during the acquisition and consolidation of a skill. Nat. Neurosci. 12: 333–341, https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2261.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Yuste, R., MacLean, J.N., Smith, J., and Lansner, A. (2005). The cortex as a central pattern generator. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6: 477–483, https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1686.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Received: 2023-03-31
Accepted: 2023-06-24
Published Online: 2023-07-14
Published in Print: 2024-01-29

© 2023 the author(s), published by De Gruyter, Berlin/Boston

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Downloaded on 1.5.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/revneuro-2023-0038/html
Scroll to top button