Skip to content
Licensed Unlicensed Requires Authentication Published by De Gruyter Mouton May 24, 2018

Natural locations and the distinction between ‘what’ and ‘where’ concepts: Evidence from differential locative marking in Makalero

  • Juliette Huber EMAIL logo
From the journal Linguistics

Abstract

Differential locative marking is found in languages all over the world. Toponyms, in particular, are well known to often require less locative marking than other types of nouns, and it has been suggested that they express ‘where’ concepts. This paper presents a case study of differential locative marking in Makalero, a Papuan language of East Timor, where nouns fall into three categories: those which need no overt locative marking, i.e., can be zero-marked; those which are used with a semantically general locative verb; and those which can only be used with semantically specific locative verbs. Zero-marking of spatial relations occurs only in a specific construction where the nouns in question are used predicatively, suggesting that the syntactic equivalent of the ‘where’ category is a relational expression, not a noun. There is furthermore a certain degree of flexibility in Makalero locative constructions, and alternative expressions result in predictable semantic differences. These differences show that the lack of perceptual boundaries and of an internal structure are important parameters that distinguish the referents of so-called natural location nouns from nouns which denote ‘what’ concepts. More comparative case studies are needed to enrich our understanding of these notions and the properties characterizing them.

Acknowledgements

First and foremost, I would like to thank Konrad Rybka, who gave me the idea to this paper and commented on the draft. I am also grateful to Niclas Burenhult and Felix Ahlner for their valuable comments on earlier drafts, and to the Makalero people of Iliomar for welcoming me into their community and providing the data on which this paper is based. The research leading to these results was funded by the European Research Council under the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013)/ERC Grant agreement n_263512. I gratefully acknowledge the Lund University Humanities Lab, which hosted this research, for providing me with a stimulating research environment.

References

Bennett, David C. 1993. There is more to location than prepositions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16(2). 239–239.10.1017/S0140525X00029745Search in Google Scholar

Burling, Robbins. 2003. Review of Ray Jackendoff, Foundations of language: Brain, meaning, grammar, evolution. Language in Society 32(3). 415–418.10.1017/S0047404503213051Search in Google Scholar

Bybee, Joan, Revere Perkins & William Pagliuca. 1994. The evolution of grammar: Tense, aspect, and modality in the languages of the world. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Search in Google Scholar

Cablitz, Gabriele H. 2008. When “what” is “where”: A linguistic analysis of landscape terms, place names and body part terms in Marquesan (Oceanic, French Polynesia). Language Sciences 30(2). 200–226.10.1016/j.langsci.2006.12.004Search in Google Scholar

Creissels, Denis. 2008. Spatial cases. In Andrej Malchukov & Andrew Spencer (eds.), The Oxford handbook of case, 609–625. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199206476.013.0043Search in Google Scholar

Croft, William & D. Alan Cruse. 2004. Cognitive linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511803864Search in Google Scholar

Feist, Michele I. 2008. Space between languages. Cognitive Science 32(7). 1177–1199.10.1080/03640210802152335Search in Google Scholar

Heine, Bernd & Tania Kuteva. 2002. World lexicon of grammaticalization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511613463Search in Google Scholar

Huber, Juliette. 2011. A grammar of Makalero: A Papuan language of East Timor. Utrecht: LOT.Search in Google Scholar

Huber, Juliette. 2014. Landscape in East Timor Papuan. Language Sciences 41. 175–196.10.1016/j.langsci.2013.08.013Search in Google Scholar

Huber, Juliette. 2017. Makalero and Makasae. In Antoinette Schapper (ed.), The Papuan languages of Timor, Alor and Pantar: Sketch grammars, vol. 2, 267–352. Berlin & Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.10.1515/9781614519027-005Search in Google Scholar

Huber, Juliette & Antoinette Schapper. 2014. The relationship between aspect and universal quantification: Evidence from three Papuan languages of Timor and Alor. In Marian Klamer & František Kratochvíl (eds.), Number and quantity in East Nusantara: Papers from 12-ICAL, 152–169. Canberra: Asia-Pacific Linguistics.Search in Google Scholar

Ikegami, Yoshihiko. 1987. “Source” vs. “goal”: A case of linguistic dissymmetry. In René Dirven & Günter Radden (eds.), Concepts of case, 122–146. Tübingen: Gunter Narr Verlag.Search in Google Scholar

Jackendoff, Ray. 1983. Semantics and cognition. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

Jackendoff, Ray. 1990. Semantic structures. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

Jackendoff, Ray. 2002. Foundations of language: Brain, meaning, grammar, evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198270126.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Kopecka, Anetta & Bhuvana Narasimhan (eds.). 2012. Events of putting and taking: A crosslinguistic perspective. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.10.1075/tsl.100Search in Google Scholar

Kracht, Marcus. 2002. On the semantics of locatives. Linguistics and Philosophy 25(2). 157–232.10.1023/A:1014646826099Search in Google Scholar

Lakusta, Laura & Barbara Landau. 2005. Starting at the end: The importance of goals in spatial language. Cognition 96(1). 1–33.10.1016/j.cognition.2004.03.009Search in Google Scholar

Landau, Barbara & Ray Jackendoff. 1993. “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16(2). 217–238.10.1017/S0140525X00029733Search in Google Scholar

Langacker, Ronald W. 1987. Foundations of cognitive grammar: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Langacker, Ronald W. 1990. Concept, image, and symbol: The cognitive basis of grammar. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar

Langacker, Ronald W. 2013. Essentials of cognitive grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Launey, Michel. 1982. Introduction à la langue et à la littérature des aztéques, tome 1: grammaire. Paris: L'Harmattan.Search in Google Scholar

Lestrade, Sander. 2010. The space of case. Nijmegen: Radboud University PhD thesis.Search in Google Scholar

Levinson, Stephen C. & P. Wilkins David (eds.). 2006. Grammars of space: Explorations in cognitive diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511486753Search in Google Scholar

Lyons, John. 1977. Semantics, vol. 2. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Mackenzie, J. Lachlan. 1992. Places and things. In Michael Fortescue, Peter Harder & Lars Kristoffersen (eds.), Layered structure and reference in a functional perspective: Papers from the functional grammar conference, Copenhagen, 1990, 253–276. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing.10.1075/pbns.23.11macSearch in Google Scholar

Mackenzie, J. Lachlan. 2005. Places and Things. In Matthew P. Anstey & J. Lachlan Mackenzie (eds.), Crucial readings in functional grammar, 141–165. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.10.1515/9783110901191Search in Google Scholar

Nichols, Johanna. 2011. Ingush grammar. Berkeley & Los Angeles: University of California Press.Search in Google Scholar

Regier, Terry & Mingyu Zheng. 2007. Attention to endpoints: A cross‐linguistic constraint on spatial meaning. Cognitive Science 31(4). 705–719.10.1080/15326900701399954Search in Google Scholar

Rybka, Konrad. 2014. How are nouns categorized as denoting “what” and “where”? Language Sciences 45. 28–43.10.1016/j.langsci.2014.05.002Search in Google Scholar

Schapper, Antoinette, Juliette Huber & van Engelenhoven Aone. 2014. The relatedness of Timor-Kisar and Alor-Pantar languages: A preliminary demonstration. In Marian Klamer (ed.), The Alor-Pantar languages: History and typology, 99–154. Berlin: Language Science Press.Search in Google Scholar

Smith, Barry & David D Mark. 2001. Geographical categories: An ontological investigation. International Journal of Geographical Information Science 15(7). 591–612.10.1080/13658810110061199Search in Google Scholar

Stolz, Thomas, Sander Lestrade & Christel Stolz. 2014. The crosslinguistics of zero-marking of spatial relations. Berlin & Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.10.1524/9783050065304Search in Google Scholar

Talmy, Leonard. 1972. Semantic structures in English and Atsugewi. Berkeley: Linguistics Department, University of California.Search in Google Scholar

Talmy, Leonard. 1983. How language structures space. In Herbert L. Pick & Linda P. Acredolo (eds.), Spatial orientation: Theory, research and application, 225–282. New York: Plenum Press.10.1007/978-1-4615-9325-6_11Search in Google Scholar

Talmy, Leonard. 2000. Toward a cognitive semantics, vol. 1. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/6847.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Thrane, Torben. 1980. Referential-semantic analysis: Aspects of a theory of linguistic reference. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511753145Search in Google Scholar

Tversky, Barbara & Herbert H Clark. 1993. Prepositions aren’t places. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16(2). 252–253.10.1017/S0140525X00029897Search in Google Scholar

Vandeloise, Claude. 1991. Spatial prepositions: A case study from French. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Search in Google Scholar

Published Online: 2018-05-24
Published in Print: 2018-06-26

© 2018 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 1.5.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/ling-2018-0003/html
Scroll to top button