Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Using whole-brain diffusion tensor analysis to evaluate white matter structural correlates of delayed visuospatial memory and one-week motor skill retention in nondemented older adults: A preliminary study

  • Jennapher Lingo VanGilder,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Biological and Health Systems Engineering, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona, United States of America

  • Maurizio Bergamino,

    Roles Formal analysis, Software, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Barrow Neuroimaging Innovation Center, Barrow Neurological Institute, Phoenix, Arizona, United States of America

  • Andrew Hooyman,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Biological and Health Systems Engineering, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona, United States of America

  • Megan C. Fitzhugh,

    Roles Data curation, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Mark and Mary Stevens Neuroimaging and Informatics Institute, Keck School of Medicine, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, United States of America

  • Corianne Rogalsky,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation College of Health Solutions, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona, United States of America

  • Jill C. Stewart,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Exercise Science, University of South Carolina, Columbia, South Carolina, United States of America

  • Scott C. Beeman,

    Roles Methodology, Software, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Biological and Health Systems Engineering, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona, United States of America

  • Sydney Y. Schaefer

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    sydney.schaefer@asu.edu

    Affiliations School of Biological and Health Systems Engineering, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona, United States of America, Department of Physical Therapy and Athletic Training, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, Utah, United States of America

Abstract

Skill retention is important for motor rehabilitation outcomes. Recent work has demonstrated that delayed visuospatial memory performance may predict motor skill retention in older and neuropathological populations. White matter integrity between parietal and frontal cortices may explain variance in upper-extremity motor learning tasks and visuospatial processes. We performed a whole-brain analysis to determine the white matter correlates of delayed visuospatial memory and one-week motor skill retention in nondemented older adults. We hypothesized that better frontoparietal tract integrity would be positively related to better behavioral performance. Nineteen participants (age>58) completed diffusion-weighted imaging, then a clinical test of delayed visuospatial memory and 50 training trials of an upper-extremity motor task; participants were retested on the motor task one week later. Principal component analysis was used to create a composite score for each participant’s behavioral data, i.e. shared variance between delayed visuospatial memory and motor skill retention, which was then entered into a voxel-based regression analysis. Behavioral results demonstrated that participants learned and retained their skill level after a week of no practice, and their delayed visuospatial memory score was positively related to the extent of skill retention. Consistent with previous work, neuroimaging results indicated that regions within bilateral anterior thalamic radiations, corticospinal tracts, and superior longitudinal fasciculi were related to better delayed visuospatial memory and skill retention. Results of this study suggest that the simple act of testing for specific cognitive impairments prior to therapy may identify older adults who will receive little to no benefit from the motor rehabilitation regimen, and that these neural regions may be potential targets for therapeutic intervention.

Introduction

Repetitive practice of functional movement patterns during motor rehabilitation are known to drive learning (or relearning) of novel motor skills, but the learning process is highly variable between individuals [1], such that responsiveness to task-specific training is often patient-specific. A number of neuroimaging and neurophysiological methods have been proposed to better predict a patient’s responsiveness to a given type or dose of motor therapy [27]. However, these methods are often time- and resource-intensive, and yield results that are not readily interpretable by clinicians. In light of this, standardized visuospatial tests may offer a more feasible solution. Visuospatial function has been linked to upper-extremity motor improvement (i.e., learning) in older adults [8, 9] and individuals with stroke pathology [10]. Although these prior studies used experimenter-derived (i.e., unstandardized) measures of visuospatial function, a recent study demonstrated that the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Delayed Recall (a clinical test of delayed visuospatial memory) predicted upper-extremity skill learning in older adults and individuals with stroke pathology [11], suggesting a clinical paper-and-pencil test could aid in predicting motor rehabilitation responsiveness.

Because cognitive and motor functions have historically been evaluated and studied separately, the neural mechanism of this behavioral relationship is currently unclear. It is plausible that visuospatial tests have predictive value because they probe the health of critical neural structures for motor skill learning. Classic neuropsychological studies have long supported the role of parietal cortex in visuospatial function [1215] and more recent neuroimaging studies have shown that the structural integrity of white matter tracts between parietal and frontal cortices is related to motor skill learning [1619]. Specifically, the superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF) has been implicated in both visuospatial processes [20, 21] and skill learning [17], suggesting it may be a candidate neural pathway for explaining our earlier behavioral findings and for predicting motor skill learning in older adults.

Further evidence of this mechanism is provided in a recent preliminary study that evaluated within-session practice effects in a small cohort of individuals with stroke pathology. The structural characteristics of the SLF (e.g., fractional anisotropy, FA) were positively correlated with the amount of skill acquired after a brief practice session on a novel upper-extremity motor task [22]. However, delayed visuospatial memory assessment and skill retention (i.e., the long-term retainment of acquired motor skill performance through repeated practice [23]) were not measured, which prevented us from fully resolving the white matter correlates of this behavioral relationship with this previous study. A retention period (otherwise known as consolidation) is important to consider when applying motor learning principles to motor rehabilitation [24]. Moreover, the previous study used a region-of-interest (ROI) approach, which effectively limits analyses to a specific neural structure. But since motor learning processes involve a vast neural network including frontal, parietal, and subcortical structures [18, 25, 26], it is possible this approach did not reveal other critical pathways for skill learning.

Thus, the purpose of this exploratory whole-brain analysis was to determine whether white matter microstructure was associated with one-week motor skill retention and delayed visuospatial memory test scores in nondemented older adults. By moving beyond a specific neurologic condition (e.g., stroke), findings from this study will more broadly generalize across geriatric populations who may be undergoing motor rehabilitation for a variety of reasons (e.g., hip/knee replacement, Parkinson’s disease). Since an estimated 30–45% of physical therapy caseloads in the United States are adults over age 65 [27], it is critical to consider broad biological mechanisms of motor rehabilitation that are independent of diagnosis. Based on previous findings, we hypothesized that better frontoparietal tract diffusion metrics (e.g., FA and radial diffusivity), including those of the SLF specifically, would positively correlate with both motor skill retention and delayed visuospatial memory test scores.

Methods

Informed written consent was obtained before participation and all experimental procedures were approved by Arizona State University’s Institutional Review Board. Nineteen community-dwelling adults (age (mean±standard deviation) = 68.4±6.8 years, 13 females) were included in this neuroimaging analysis, which was a sub-study of a larger observational experiment in which participants completed a battery of clinical visuospatial tests and 50 weekly training trials of a motor task using their nondominant (left) hand for three consecutive weeks and returned one month later to retest their skill level [28]. One-week skill retention was not reported in the previous study, which instead focused on longer-term retention (one-month); the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition [29] was administered and established cutoff scores were used to screen all participants for nondemented status. The present study includes a subset of those participants who also completed diffusion-weighted neuroimaging (n = 19) prior to behavioral testing.

All participants were right-handed, as determined by a modified Edinburgh Inventory [30]. The nondominant hand was evaluated using grip dynamometry (i.e., maximal grip strength), Purdue Grooved Pegboard (i.e., dexterity) [31], and Semmes monofilaments [32] tests to characterize sensory function, respectively. Participants also completed the Short-Form Geriatric Depression Scale [33] and Katz Activities of Daily Living questionnaire [34] to measure for depressive symptoms and ability to independently complete motor tasks at home, respectively. Participants used their dominant hand to complete the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test [35], a standardized complex figure drawing test that measures visuoconstruction (Figure Copy) and delayed visuospatial memory (Delayed Recall). Participants were first asked to draw a replicate of a complex image as precisely as possible; once finished, all visual stimuli were removed from the testing area. Thirty minutes later, participants were asked to redraw the figure from memory (Fig 1A). A single rater scored each test using established testing guidelines to reduce interrater variability; higher scores indicate better delayed visuospatial memory.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Delayed visuospatial memory test and motor skill task.

A. Participants completed the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Delayed Recall test (measures delayed visuospatial memory). An example drawing from one of the participants is shown. B. Participants used their nondominant hand to perform the motor task that mimicked the upper extremity movements required to feed oneself. This image is adapted from the “Dexterity and Reaching Motor Tasks” by MRL Laboratory that is licensed under CC BY 2.0.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0274955.g001

Motor skill retention

As described previously [28], the functional motor task used for training and retention simulated the reaching and dexterity movements required to feed oneself with a utensil (Fig 1B) yet has also been validated against a more commonly used motor learning paradigm [36]. Briefly, the experimental apparatus is comprised of four plastic cups adhered to a board; three of the cups are ‘target’ cups that are located radially around a center ‘home’ cup that is aligned with the participant’s midline. The participant must use a standard plastic spoon with their nondominant hand to acquire two beans at a time from the ‘home’ cup and transport them to one of the target cups. The participants are instructed to transport the beans first to the target cup located ipsilateral to the participant’s nondominant hand. They then scoop two more beans from the ‘home’ cup and transport them to the middle target cup, then another two beans to the contralateral cup. The home cup contains 30 beans, resulting in 15 total reaches (5 target cycles) per trial. Trial time is the measure of performance, which is the elapsed time from when the participant picks up the spoon until the last of the beans are deposited into the last target cup.

Participants completed 50 training trials (i.e., a total of 750 reaches) and trial times were averaged across five trials to comprise a ‘block’ (thus, participants completed 10 blocks of five trials each across the training session). One-week skill retention was measured as the difference in performance between the last training block and a retest block that was completed one week later.

Neuroimaging acquisition

Participants underwent diffusion magnetic resonance imaging at the Keller Center for Imaging Innovation at Barrow Neurological Institute, Phoenix, Arizona. A 3-Tesla Philips Ingenia MRI (Philips, Healthcare) was used to acquire data using single-shell diffusion weighted acquisitions with the following parameters: 32 diffusion-encoding directions (b-value: 2500 s/mm2. TR/TE: 7065/119 ms; flip-angle = 90°; matrix: 92 × 90; voxel size: 3.0 mm × 3.0 mm; slice thickness: 3.0 mm; number of averages = 1) and one B0 image at the beginning of the acquisition. All MR images were screened for neuropathology by a licensed neuroradiologist prior analysis.

Neuroimaging preprocessing

DICOM images were converted to NIFTI using dcm2niix and were preprocessed using MRtrix 3.0 [37] and FSL 6.0.0 (FMRIB, Oxford, UK); the neuroimaging protocol is published on the protocols.io platform (https://www.protocols.io/private/636a847d207a11ecaf560a58a9feac02). The raw diffusion-weighted images were denoised (dwidenoise) and Gibbs ringing artifacts were removed (mrdegibbs). A whole brain mask was created to extract brain from non-brain tissues (dwi2mask). Data were then corrected for motion and eddy currents by eddy (FSL). To account for the rotational component of registration, the b-vector files were compensated after motion correction and prior to calculating the b matrices. B1-field inhomogeneity was corrected for (dwibiascorrect), and all images were upsampled to 1.25 mm (mrgrid) to improve coregistration with the MNI-ICBM152 template from the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI). For each acquisition, a diffusion tensor model was fit at each voxel to calculate fractional anisotropy (FA) and radial diffusivity (RD) maps (dtifit, https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki; see S1 Fig for diffusion tensor parameter images for a representative participant).

Using the B0 images from all subjects (n = 19), a group template was created using buildtemplateparallel.sh included in the advanced normalization tools (ANTs, http://stnava.github.io/ANTs/). Maps were then nonlinearly coregistered to this template using WarpImageMultiTransform (ANTs) and were spatially smoothed (FSL) using a Gaussian kernel (sigma, 2 mm). The group template was transformed from template space to MNI space using antsResistrationSyN.sh (ANTs).

Statistical analysis

JMP Pro 15.0 (SAS) was used to process participant behavioral data. To reduce the dimensionality of our statistical model and address collinearity among model predictors (i.e., mitigate the effect of reduced statistical significance due to collinearity between skill retention and visuospatial test scores), principal component analysis (PC) was used to create a ‘composite score’ that represented the shared variance of skill retention and Delayed Recall score for each participant. Since our previous work has shown a relationship between these two variables [11, 28], the PC analysis allowed for consideration of only the shared variance between them as an independent variable. Only PCs with an eigenvalue greater than one were carried forward in subsequent analyses.

Using MATLAB 2020 (MathWorks, Inc.), significant PCs and age (a covariate of noninterest) were entered into a general linear model that was applied at each voxel for each diffusion map and an FDR-correction was applied to account for multiple statistical tests. Clusters were defined as at least 100 contiguous voxels where the FDR corrected p-value was < 0.01; clusters were transformed from template space to MNI using antsApplyTransforms (ANTs) and the Johns Hopkins University JHU atlas [38, 39] was used to identify the neuroanatomical location of each cluster.

Results

Participant characteristics, motor and sensory data are presented in Table 1. Overall, participants demonstrated normal tactile sensation, grip strength, and dexterity performance consistent with that of established normative values [32, 40, 41].

Motor training data are presented in Fig 2A; we observed a significant difference between the baseline and final training blocks (p = 0.0087, 95% CI [-9.68, -0.99]) and no difference between the final training and retest blocks (p = 0.1823, 95% CI [-1.48, 7.45]), indicating that overall participants learned the motor task across the training trials and retained the skill over a period of one week without practice. Fig 2B demonstrates that Delayed Recall and motor skill retention scores were positively correlated (R2 = 0.35; p = 0.0079, 95% CI [0.18, 0.82]). These values are reported to simply confirm that participants did indeed learn the motor task (as indicated by one-week retention) and that the amount of motor skill retention was positively related to Delayed Recall scores.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Participant one-week skill retention and correlation with delayed visuospatial memory performance.

A. Participants completed 50 training trials of the reaching task and were retested one week later to determine skill retention. Trials were consolidated into blocks of five trials each. Mean motor performance (trial time in seconds) is plotted on the y-axis, where lower values indicate better performance; vertical error bars show standard deviation. B. Skill retention was measured as the last block of the training session subtracted by the retest block (one week later). Participants’ skill retention is on the y-axis and Delayed Recall scores are on the x-axis; the figure illustrates that skill retention and Delayed Recall scores are positively correlated, where higher Delayed Recall scores predict better skill retention.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0274955.g002

Only one principal component emerged from the PC analysis with an eigenvalue > 1, which accounted for 79.49% of the variance among one-week skill retention and Delayed Recall scores; factor analysis results showed that both variables equally loaded onto the PC at 0.79 (where values closer to 1 indicate that each variable’s variance is wholly explained by the PC). S2 Fig illustrates that the PC was positively correlated with one-week skill retention and Delayed Recall scores, illustrating that the PC did indeed quantitatively represent the shared variance of both participant motor skill retention and Delayed Recall scores.

Results of the voxel-based analysis are provided in Table 2. For FA, positive correlations were found in bilateral anterior thalamic radiations (ATR), corticospinal tracts (CST; in brainstem), and the right superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF); a negative cluster was observed in the left hemisphere that comprised atlas regions of the SLF, ATR, and (superior) CST (Fig 3A). For RD, a positive cluster was observed in this same region and negative clusters were found in the right ATR, bilateral CST (in brainstem), and left SLF (Fig 3B; also S3 Fig shows the distribution of participant FA and RD values in each cluster with respect to their behavioral data). Overall, these results indicate that the integrity of regions within the SLF, ATR, and CST were positively related to one-week skill retention and delayed visuospatial memory; the anatomical overlap between the negative FA and positive RD clusters may be due to well-known model limitations [4244] and is discussed further.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Whole-brain fractional anisotropy and radial diffusivity results.

Fractional anisotropy results are shown in Panel A; the first row illustrates the large positive cluster in the right SLF (orange), the second row illustrates the negative cluster in the left CST/ATR/SLF, and the third row illustrates the positive cluster in bilateral CST in the brainstem. Radial diffusivity results are shown in Panel B; the first row shows the negative cluster in the left SLF, the second row shows the positive cluster in the ATR/CST, and the third row illustrates the negative cluster in the bilateral CST in the brainstem. The last row shows the negative cluster in the right ATR.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0274955.g003

thumbnail
Table 2. Whole-brain fractional anisotropy and radial diffusivity results.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0274955.t002

Discussion

This study aimed to extend our previous work that reported the SLF was related to within-session practice effects in a small sample of individuals with stroke pathology [22]. Here, we used whole-brain analyses to determine the white matter correlates of the behavioral relationship between one-week motor skill retention and delayed visuospatial memory test scores in nondemented older adults. Results indicated that regions within the bilateral CST, SLF, and ATR were associated with one-week motor skill retention and delayed visuospatial memory performance independently of age and support that clinical visuospatial testing may prognose motor training responsiveness and the integrity of specific white matter tracts.

A possible explanation for the observed behavioral relationship between Delayed Recall scores and one-week motor skill retention is that visuospatial memory and motor learning engage overlapping neural pathways. Our results are consistent with reports from neuroanatomical and neurophysiological studies implicating the CST [4547] and SLF [17, 22, 48] in motor learning behaviors, and the SLF [4953] and anterior thalamic nuclei [5459] in visuospatial processing and memory. Moreover, the ATR is thought to relay motor signals via the thalamocortical pathway and has been linked to spatial memory in nonhuman primates [60, 61], further implicating the role of motor networks in visuospatial memory.

In our previous study [22], results indicated diffusion metrics of the SLF were related to the amount of upper-extremity motor skill acquired within a training session, whereas those of the CST did not. Results of the present study suggest that fractional anisotropy and radial diffusivity of the CST, SLF, and ATR were related to one-week skill retention on this same motor task. A potential reason for our different results could be due to the methodological approaches applied to analyze the neuroimaging data and phase of motor skill learning (i.e. acquisition versus retention). Regan et al. (2020) conducted a ROI-based approach that targeted the diffusion metrics of the SLF, whereas the whole-brain approach used here applied the general linear model at each voxel containing white matter. In addition, separate phases of motor skill learning engage distinct neural networks [62, 63], thus, it is plausible that the difference in timescales at which motor behavior was measured explains the discrepancy between the significant white matter regions reported. Regan et al. (2020) evaluated within-session practice effects, which was measured by calculating the change score between baseline and final performances; therefore, this metric included baseline performance and skill acquisition (in contrast to skill retention). Similarly, Borich and colleagues examined the white matter correlates of motor learning on a 2-D visuomotor pursuit task by measuring the difference in performance between baseline and delayed retention trials; they collected diffusion-weighted images from a small group of individuals with stroke pathology after participants completed five separate training sessions. Using whole-brain analyses, their group reported that regions within the posterior limb of the internal capsule were related to better skill retention [64]. Again, the purpose of the present study was to identify the structural white matter correlates of one-week motor skill retention and delayed visuospatial memory, thus, our neuroimaging results reflect this behavioral relationship rather than that of motor behavior alone.

One limitation of this study regards the diffusion-weighted image acquisition protocol. Recent work has shown that free-water correction improves the accuracy and sensitivity of white matter analyses [65, 66] by fitting a bi-tensor model to each voxel to account for partial volume effects (i.e., voxels that contain brain tissue and free-water such as cerebrospinal fluid); however, it is advised to apply this technique to single-shell diffusion-weighted images that were acquired with b-values less than 1000 s/mm2 [67]. Our data were acquired with a single b-value = 2500 s/mm2, therefore we were unable to apply free-water correction due to our imaging acquisition. Indeed, our preliminary data were leveraged from an already-ongoing study of older adults and the pre-existing diffusion protocol limited our analyses to the diffusion tensor model, which assumes Gaussian behavior of water diffusion [68] even within biological restrictive tissues (such as white matter) that is known to be non-Gaussian [69]. Nevertheless, there is evidence that suggests diffusion tensor metrics, particularly FA and RD, are comparable to metrics quantified by advanced biophysical models that require multi-shell data (e.g., diffusion kurtosis imaging [70] and Neurite Orientation Dispersion and Density Imaging [71]. Moreover, positive and negative correlations among a single region of interest emerged from our whole-brain analyses; for example, results indicated several significant clusters present along the CST: a negative correlation in the left superior part of the tract and positive correlation in the brainstem. We observed anatomical overlap between negative FA and positive RD clusters in this region and interpret this finding was likely due to partial volume effects (i.e., crossing fibers as significant clusters in the SLF and ATR were also observed in this region); this interpretation is consistent with other work [17]. It is prudent to mention that results may also be susceptible to artifact due to image smoothing and/or normalization registration during the preprocessing methodology; to address this potential limitation, we visually inspected all images to ensure satisfactory coregistrations. While this study design allowed us to test if pre-existing neuroanatomical measures of white matter tracts were associated with one-week skill retention and Delayed Recall test scores, a future study that involves pre- and post-training neuroimaging will allow us to test the robustness of these findings (i.e., Will we observe microstructural changes in these same tracts?).

Results of this study have several potential clinical implications. First, visuospatial testing may be a more feasible biomarker of motor therapy responsiveness than measures derived from neuroimaging or neurophysiological data (e.g., presence of a motor-evoked potential). For example, while previous studies have shown that whole-brain volume metrics (e.g., T1 scanning, etc.) may predict motor therapy outcomes [72], a visuospatial test is quick and easy to administer during the duration of a typical clinical visit, making it a more feasible alternative in terms of predicting motor rehabilitation responsiveness. Second, we have previously observed the behavioral relationship between cognitive testing and upper-extremity skill retention across patient populations [11, 73], suggesting this relationship is not disease-specific and is broadly generalizable across geriatric populations. Given the prevalence of cognitive impairment even in community-dwelling older adults [3, 74, 75], it is plausible that older adults seeking physical therapy for a variety of reasons could have subtle underlying visuospatial impairments that may impede their responsiveness to therapy, regardless of the etiology (i.e., white matter hyperintensities [76], stroke, etc.). Third, our results open new avenues of research as we have begun to explore motor learning paradigms to better understand AD progression [77]. Research has shown that accelerated decline in visuospatial function may be an early biomarker of prodromal AD [7880]. Given that ATR degeneration is associated with AD progression [81, 82] and that the complex figure copy/recall tests may predict AD onset (up to 20 years before clinical AD) [83], results from this study suggest that an assessment of motor learning could help better identify disease progression in asymptomatic stages [84, 85]. A future study that extends our preliminary work is needed to determine the feasibility and efficacy of clinical visuospatial testing to predict motor rehabilitation outcomes.

Conclusions

In summary, nondemented older adults learned an upper-extremity motor task and retained the skill one week later. The amount of skill retained was related to performance on a clinical test of delayed visuospatial memory; this behavioral relationship was related to the integrity of bilateral corticospinal tracts, anterior thalamic radiations, and the superior longitudinal fasciculi, consistent with previous work. Clinical visuospatial memory testing may provide prognostic insight for one’s potential to benefit from a given dose and type of motor rehabilitation as well as a target for therapeutic intervention.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Diffusion tenor model parameter maps.

Fractional anisotropy (top row), radial (second row), mean (third row), and axial (bottom row) diffusivity maps for an example participant that demonstrates the diffusion tensor model fit the data as expected.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0274955.s001

(TIF)

S2 Fig. Principal component values and one-week skill retention and delayed visuospatial memory performance.

Principal component values (y-axis) for each participant was highly correlated with their one-week skill retention and Delayed Recall scores (x-axes), demonstrating that it was indeed a good representation of the shared variance of both behaviors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0274955.s002

(TIF)

S3 Fig. Fractional anisotropy and radial diffusivity values versus principal component values for each cluster.

Fractional anisotropy (panel A) and radial diffusivity (panel B) values with respect to principal component values for each significant cluster. L = left. R = right. ATR = anterior thalamic radiation. CST = corticospinal tract. SLF = superior longitudinal fasciculus. COG = center of gravity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0274955.s003

(TIF)

References

  1. 1. Anderson D. I., Lohse K. R., Lopes T. C. V., and Williams A. M., “Individual differences in motor skill learning: Past, present and future,” Hum Mov Sci, vol. 78, p. 102818, 2021, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2021.102818 pmid:34049152
  2. 2. Stinear C., “Prediction of recovery of motor function after stroke.,” Lancet Neurol, vol. 9, no. 12, pp. 1228–1232, Dec. 2010, pmid:21035399
  3. 3. Tozlu C. et al., “Machine Learning Methods Predict Individual Upper-Limb Motor Impairment Following Therapy in Chronic Stroke,” Neurorehabil Neural Repair, vol. 34, no. 5, pp. 428–439, Mar. 2020, pmid:32193984
  4. 4. Lipp I. et al., “Predictors of training-related improvement in visuomotor performance in patients with multiple sclerosis: A behavioural and MRI study,” Multiple Sclerosis Journal, p. 1352458520943788, Aug. 2020, pmid:32749927
  5. 5. Wittenberg G. F., Lovelace C. T., Foster D. J., and Maldjian J. A., “Functional neuroimaging of dressing-related skills.,” Brain Imaging Behav, vol. 8, no. 3, pp. 335–345, Sep. 2014, pmid:23070748
  6. 6. Horak FB and Diener HC, “Cerebellar control of postural scaling and central set in stance.,” J Neurophysiol., vol. 72, no. 2, pp. 479–93, 1994. pmid:7983513
  7. 7. Cirstea C. M., Lee P., Craciunas S. C., Choi I.-Y., Burris J. E., and Nudo R. J., “Pre-therapy Neural State of Bilateral Motor and Premotor Cortices Predicts Therapy Gain After Subcortical Stroke: A Pilot Study.,” Am J Phys Med Rehabil, vol. 97, no. 1, pp. 23–33, Jan. 2018, pmid:28737516
  8. 8. Bo J. and Seidler R. D., “Visuospatial working memory capacity predicts the organization of acquired explicit motor sequences,” J Neurophysiol, vol. 101, no. 6, p. 3116, 2009, pmid:19357338
  9. 9. Langan J. and Seidler R. D., “Age differences in spatial working memory contributions to visuomotor adaptation and transfer,” Behavioural brain research, vol. 225, no. 1, pp. 160–168, Nov. 2011, pmid:21784106
  10. 10. Toglia J., Fitzgerald K. A., O’Dell M. W., Mastrogiovanni A. R., and Lin C. D., “The Mini-Mental State Examination and Montreal Cognitive Assessment in persons with Mild Subacute Stroke: Relationship to functional outcome,” Arch Phys Med Rehabil, vol. 92, no. 5, pp. 792–798, 2011, pmid:21530727
  11. 11. Lingo VanGilder J., Hooyman A., Bosch P. R., and Schaefer S. Y., “Generalizing the predictive relationship between 1-month motor skill retention and Rey-Osterrieth Delayed Recall scores from nondemented older adults to individuals with chronic stroke: a short report.,” J Neuroeng Rehabil, vol. 18, no. 1, p. 94, Jun. 2021, pmid:34082761
  12. 12. Mishkin M., Ungerleider L. G., and Macko K. A., “Object vision and spatial vision: Two cortical pathways,” Trends Neurosci, vol. 6, no. C, pp. 414–417, 1983,
  13. 13. Newcombe F., Ratcliff G., and Damasio H., “Dissociable visual and spatial impairments following right posterior cerebral lesions: Clinical, neuropsychological and anatomical evidence,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 25, no. 1, Part 2, pp. 149–161, 1987. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(87)90127-8 pmid:3574655
  14. 14. Owen A. M., Sahakian B. J., Semple J., Polkey C. E., and Robbins T. W., “Visuo-spatial short-term recognition memory and learning after temporal lobe excisions, frontal lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 33, no. 1, pp. 1–24, 1995. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(94)00098-A pmid:7731533
  15. 15. Ungerleider L. G. and Haxby J.V, “‘What’ and ‘where’ in the human brain,” Curr Opin Neurobiol, vol. 4, no. 2, pp. 157–165, 1994. https://doi.org/10.1016/0959-4388(94)90066-3.
  16. 16. Tomassini V. et al., “Structural and functional bases for individual differences in motor learning,” Hum Brain Mapp, vol. 32, no. 3, pp. 494–508, 2011, pmid:20533562
  17. 17. Steele C. J., Scholz J., Douaud G., Johansen-Berg H., and Penhune V. B., “Structural correlates of skilled performance on a motor sequence task,” Front Hum Neurosci, vol. 6, no. October, pp. 1–9, 2012, pmid:23125826
  18. 18. Taubert M. et al., “Dynamic properties of human brain structure: Learning-related changes in cortical areas and associated fiber connections,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 30, no. 35, pp. 11670–11677, 2010, pmid:20810887
  19. 19. Sampaio-Baptista C. et al., “Gray matter volume is associated with rate of subsequent skill learning after a long term training intervention,” Neuroimage, vol. 96, pp. 158–166, 2014, pmid:24680712
  20. 20. McGrath J., Johnson K., O’Hanlon E., Garavan H., Gallagher L., and Leemans A., “White matter and visuospatial processing in autism: a constrained spherical deconvolution tractography study.,” Autism Res, vol. 6, no. 5, pp. 307–319, Oct. 2013, pmid:23509018
  21. 21. Chechlacz M., Gillebert C., Vangkilde S., Petersen A., and Humphreys G.W, Structural Variability within Frontoparietal Networks and Individual Differences in Attentional Functions: An Approach Using the Theory of Visual Attention, vol. 35. 2015. pmid:26224851
  22. 22. Regan E. et al., “Neural correlates of within-session practice effects in mild motor impairment after stroke: a preliminary investigation.,” Exp Brain Res, vol. 239, no. 1, pp. 151–160, Jan. 2021, pmid:33130906
  23. 23. R. Schmidt, T. Lee, C. Winstein, G. Wulf, and H. Zelaznik, “Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis, 6th Edition (online access included),” ProtoView, vol. 2018, no. 16. Ringgold Inc, Beaverton, pp. 1–552, 2018.
  24. 24. Bastian A. J., “Understanding sensorimotor adaptation and learning for rehabilitation,” Curr Opin Neurol, vol. 21, no. 6, pp. 628–633, 2008, pmid:18989103
  25. 25. Seidler R. D., “Neural correlates of motor learning, transfer of learning, and learning to learn.,” Exerc Sport Sci Rev, vol. 38, no. 1, pp. 3–9, Jan. 2010, pmid:20016293
  26. 26. Ghilardi M. et al., “Patterns of regional brain activation associated with different forms of motor learning.,” Brain Res, vol. 871, no. 1, pp. 127–145, Jul. 2000. pmid:10882792
  27. 27. A. Bell, “9 Physical Therapist Tips to Help You Age Well,” American Physical Therapy Association, 2015. https://www.moveforwardpt.com/Resources/Detail/9-physical-therapist-tips-to-help-you-agewell (accessed Mar. 22, 2018).
  28. 28. Lingo VanGilder J., Lohse K. R., Duff K., Wang P., and Schaefer S. Y., “Evidence for associations between Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure test and motor skill learning in older adults.,” Acta Psychol (Amst), vol. 214, p. 103261, Mar. 2021, pmid:33524606
  29. 29. Wechsler D., Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. Oxford, England: Psychological Corp., 1955.
  30. 30. Oldfield R. C., “The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 9, no. 1, pp. 97–113, 1971, pmid:5146491
  31. 31. Merker B. and Podell K., “Grooved Pegboard Test,” in Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology, Kreutzer J. S., DeLuca J., and Caplan B., Eds. New York, NY: Springer New York, 2011, pp. 1176–1178.
  32. 32. Bell-Krotoski J. A., Fess E. E., Figarola J. H., and Hiltz D., “Threshold detection and Semmes-Weinstein monofilaments.,” J Hand Ther, vol. 8, no. 2, pp. 155–162, 1995. pmid:7550627
  33. 33. Yesavage J. A. and Sheikh J. I., “9/Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS),” Clin Gerontol, vol. 5, no. 1–2, pp. 165–173, Nov. 1986,
  34. 34. Katz S., Downs T. D., Cash H. R., and Grotz R. C., “Progress in development of the Index of ADL1,” Gerontologist, vol. 10, no. 1_Part_1, pp. 20–30, Mar. 1970.
  35. 35. Osterrieth P. A., “Le test de copie d’une figure complexe [in French],” Arch Psychol (Geneve), vol. 30, no. 30, pp. 206–356, 1944.
  36. 36. Schaefer S. Y. and Hengge C. R., “Testing the concurrent validity of a naturalistic upper extremity reaching task,” Exp Brain Res, vol. 234, no. 1, pp. 229–240, 2016, pmid:26438508
  37. 37. Tournier J.-D. et al., “MRtrix3: A fast, flexible and open software framework for medical image processing and visualisation.,” Neuroimage, vol. 202, p. 116137, Nov. 2019, pmid:31473352
  38. 38. Wakana S. et al., “Reproducibility of quantitative tractography methods applied to cerebral white matter,” Neuroimage, vol. 36, no. 3, pp. 630–644, Jul. 2007, pmid:17481925
  39. 39. Hua K. et al., “Tract probability maps in stereotaxic spaces: analyses of white matter anatomy and tract-specific quantification,” Neuroimage, vol. 39, no. 1, pp. 336–347, Jan. 2008, pmid:17931890
  40. 40. Mathiowetz V., Kashman N., Volland G., Weber K., Dowe M., and Rogers S., “Grip and pinch strength: Normative data for adults.,” Arch Phys Med Rehabil, vol. 66, no. 2, pp. 69–74, Feb. 1985. pmid:3970660
  41. 41. Earhart G. M., Cavanaugh J. T., Ellis T., Ford M. P., Foreman K. B., and Dibble L., “The 9-hole PEG test of upper extremity function: Average values, test-retest reliability, and factors contributing to performance in people with Parkinson disease.,” J Neurol Phys Ther, vol. 35, no. 4, pp. 157–163, Dec. 2011, pmid:22020457
  42. 42. Hirsch J. G., Bock M., Essig M., and Schad L. R., “Comparison of diffusion anisotropy measurements in combination with the flair-technique.,” Magn Reson Imaging, vol. 17, no. 5, pp. 705–716, Jun. 1999, pmid:10372524
  43. 43. Alexander A. L., Hasan K. M., Lazar M., Tsuruda J. S., and Parker D. L., “Analysis of partial volume effects in diffusion-tensor MRI.,” Magn Reson Med, vol. 45, no. 5, pp. 770–780, May 2001, pmid:11323803
  44. 44. Jones D. K. and Cercignani M., “Twenty-five pitfalls in the analysis of diffusion MRI data,” NMR Biomed, vol. 23, no. 7, pp. 803–820, Aug. 2010. https://doi.org/10.1002/nbm.1543 pmid:20886566
  45. 45. Christiansen L., Larsen M. N., Madsen M. J., Grey M. J., Nielsen J. B., and Lundbye-Jensen J., “Long-term motor skill training with individually adjusted progressive difficulty enhances learning and promotes corticospinal plasticity,” Sci Rep, vol. 10, no. 1, p. 15588, 2020, pmid:32973251
  46. 46. Song S., Sharma N., Buch E. R., and Cohen L. G., “White Matter Microstructural Correlates of Superior Long-term Skill Gained Implicitly under Randomized Practice,” Cerebral Cortex, vol. 22, no. 7, pp. 1671–1677, Sep. 2011, pmid:21914632
  47. 47. Bleyenheuft Y. et al., “Motor Skill Training May Restore Impaired Corticospinal Tract Fibers in Children With Cerebral Palsy,” Neurorehabil Neural Repair, vol. 34, no. 6, pp. 533–546, May 2020, pmid:32407247
  48. 48. Budisavljevic S. et al., “Asymmetry and Structure of the Fronto-Parietal Networks Underlie Visuomotor Processing in Humans,” Cerebral Cortex, vol. 27, no. 2, pp. 1532–1544, 2017, pmid:26759477
  49. 49. Shinoura N., Suzuki Y., Yamada R., Tabei Y., Saito K., and Yagi K., “Damage to the right superior longitudinal fasciculus in the inferior parietal lobe plays a role in spatial neglect,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 47, no. 12, pp. 2600–2603, 2009. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.05.010.
  50. 50. Multani N. et al., “The association between white-matter tract abnormalities, and neuropsychiatric and cognitive symptoms in retired professional football players with multiple concussions,” J Neurol, vol. 263, no. 7, pp. 1332–1341, 2016, pmid:27142715
  51. 51. Chechlacz M., Gillebert C. R., Vangkilde S. A., Petersen A., and Humphreys G. W., “Structural Variability within Frontoparietal Networks and Individual Differences in Attentional Functions: An Approach Using the Theory of Visual Attention,” The Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 35, no. 30, pp. 10647 LP–10658, Jul. 2015, pmid:26224851
  52. 52. Hoeft F. et al., “More is not always better: Increased fractional anisotropy of superior longitudinal fasciculus associated with poor visuospatial abilities in Williams Syndrome,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 27, no. 44, pp. 11960–11965, 2007, pmid:17978036
  53. 53. Mayer K. and Vuong Q., “TBSS and probabilistic tractography reveal white matter connections for attention to object features,” Brain Struct Funct, vol. 219, no. 6, pp. 2159–2171, 2014, pmid:24005260
  54. 54. O‘Mara S., “The anterior thalamus provides a subcortical circuit supporting memory and spatial navigation,” Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, vol. 7. p. 45, 2013. pmid:24009563
  55. 55. R. J. Sutherland and J. M. Hoesing, “Posterior cingulate cortex and spatial memory: A microlimnology analysis.,” Neurobiology of cingulate cortex and limbic thalamus: A comprehensive handbook. Birkhäuser, Cambridge, MA, US, pp. 461–477, 1993.
  56. 56. Winter S. S., Clark B. J., and Taube J. S., “Spatial navigation. Disruption of the head direction cell network impairs the parahippocampal grid cell signal.,” Science, vol. 347, no. 6224, pp. 870–874, Feb. 2015, pmid:25700518
  57. 57. Taube J. S., “Head direction cells recorded in the anterior thalamic nuclei of freely moving rats.,” J Neurosci, vol. 15, no. 1 Pt 1, pp. 70–86, Jan. 1995, pmid:7823153
  58. 58. Aggleton J. P. and Nelson A. J. D., “Why do lesions in the rodent anterior thalamic nuclei cause such severe spatial deficits?,” Neurosci Biobehav Rev, vol. 54, pp. 131–144, Jul. 2015, pmid:25195980
  59. 59. Peyrache A., Lacroix M. M., Petersen P. C., and Buzsáki G., “Internally organized mechanisms of the head direction sense.,” Nat Neurosci, vol. 18, no. 4, pp. 569–575, Apr. 2015, pmid:25730672
  60. 60. Parker A. and Gaffan D., “The effect of anterior thalamic and cingulate cortex lesions on object-in-place memory in monkeys,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 35, no. 8, pp. 1093–1102, 1997. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00042-0 pmid:9256374
  61. 61. Spets D. S. and Slotnick S. D., “Thalamic Functional Connectivity during Spatial Long-Term Memory and the Role of Sex.,” Brain Sci, vol. 10, no. 12, Nov. 2020, pmid:33255156
  62. 62. Doyon J. and Benali H., “Reorganization and plasticity in the adult brain during learning of motor skills,” Curr Opin Neurobiol, vol. 15, no. 2, pp. 161–167, 2005, pmid:15831397
  63. 63. K.R. L., K. W., L.A. B., and N.J. H., “Motor skill acquisition across short and long time scales: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging data,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 59, pp. 130–141, 2014. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.05.001.
  64. 64. Borich M. R., Brown K. E., and Boyd L. A., “Motor skill learning is associated with diffusion characteristics of white matter in individuals with chronic stroke.,” J Neurol Phys Ther, vol. 38, no. 3, pp. 151–160, Jul. 2014, pmid:23934017
  65. 65. Bergamino M., Walsh R. R., and Stokes A. M., “Free-water diffusion tensor imaging improves the accuracy and sensitivity of white matter analysis in Alzheimer’s disease,” Sci Rep, vol. 11, no. 1, p. 6990, 2021, pmid:33772083
  66. 66. Bergamino M., Pasternak O., Farmer M., Shenton M. E., and Hamilton J. P., “Applying a free-water correction to diffusion imaging data uncovers stress-related neural pathology in depression.,” Neuroimage Clin, vol. 10, pp. 336–342, 2016, pmid:27006903
  67. 67. Pasternak O., Shenton M. E., and Westin C.-F., “Estimation of extracellular volume from regularized multi-shell diffusion MRI,” Med Image Comput Comput Assist Interv, vol. 15, no. Pt 2, pp. 305–312, 2012, pmid:23286062
  68. 68. Basser P. J., Mattiello J., and LeBihan D., “MR diffusion tensor spectroscopy and imaging,” Biophys J, vol. 66, no. 1, pp. 259–267, Jan. 1994, pmid:8130344
  69. 69. Cory D. G., “Measurement of translational displacement probabilities by NMR: An indicator of compartmentation,” Magn Reson Med, vol. 14, no. 3, pp. 435–444, Jun. 1990. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.1910140303 pmid:2355827
  70. 70. Coutu J.-P., Chen J. J., Rosas H. D., and Salat D. H., “Non-Gaussian water diffusion in aging white matter,” Neurobiol Aging, vol. 35, no. 6, pp. 1412–1421, 2014. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.12.001 pmid:24378085
  71. 71. Fukutomi H. et al., “Diffusion Tensor Model links to Neurite Orientation Dispersion and Density Imaging at high b-value in Cerebral Cortical Gray Matter,” Sci Rep, vol. 9, no. 1, p. 12246, 2019, pmid:31439874
  72. 72. Wang P. et al., “Correlation of Longitudinal Gray Matter Volume Changes and Motor Recovery in Patients After Pontine Infarction,” Frontiers in Neurology, vol. 9. p. 312, 2018. pmid:29910762
  73. 73. VanGilder J. L., Lopez-Lennon C., Paul S. S., Dibble L. E., Duff K., and Schaefer S. Y., “Relating global cognition with upper-extremity motor skill retention in individuals with mild-to-moderate Parkinson disease,” Front. Rehabilit. Sci.—Interventions for Rehabilitation, p. In press., Jan. 2021. In press.
  74. 74. Burke Quinlan E. et al., “Neural function, injury, and stroke subtype predict treatment gains after stroke.,” Ann Neurol, vol. 77, no. 1, pp. 132–145, Jan. 2015, pmid:25382315
  75. 75. Quinlan E. B., Dodakian L., See J., McKenzie A., Stewart J. C., and Cramer S. C., “Biomarkers of Rehabilitation Therapy Vary according to Stroke Severity,” Neural Plast, vol. 2018, p. 8 pages, 2018. pmid:29721009
  76. 76. Fleischman D. A. et al., “Physical activity, motor function, and white matter hyperintensity burden in healthy older adults.,” Neurology, vol. 84, no. 13. pp. 1294–1300, Mar. 2015. pmid:25762710
  77. 77. Schaefer S. Y., Duff K., Hooyman A., and Hoffman J. M., “Improving prediction of amyloid deposition in Mild Cognitive Impairment with a timed motor task,” Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen, p. In press., Jan. 2021. In Press.
  78. 78. Johnson D. K., Storandt M., Morris J. C., and Galvin J. E., “Longitudinal Study of the Transition From Healthy Aging to Alzheimer DiseaseTransition From Healthy Aging to Alzheimer Disease,” Arch Neurol, vol. 66, no. 10, pp. 1254–1259, Oct. 2009, pmid:19822781
  79. 79. Mitolo M. et al., “Visuospatial memory and neuroimaging correlates in mild cognitive impairment,” Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, vol. 35, no. 1, pp. 75–90, 2013, pmid:23357899
  80. 80. Rizzo M., Anderson S. W., Dawson J., Myers R., and Ball K., “Visual attention impairments in Alzheimer’s disease.,” Neurology, vol. 54, no. 10, pp. 1954–1959, May 2000.
  81. 81. Zhu Q.-Y. et al., “Disruption of thalamic connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease: a diffusion tensor imaging study,” Metab Brain Dis, vol. 30, no. 5, pp. 1295–1308, 2015, pmid:26141074
  82. 82. Torso M. et al., “Strategic Lesions in the Anterior Thalamic Radiation and Apathy in Early Alzheimer’s Disease,” PLoS One, vol. 10, no. 5, p. e0124998, May 2015. pmid:25932637
  83. 83. Caselli R. J. et al., “Neuropsychological decline up to 20 years before incident mild cognitive impairment.,” Alzheimers Dement, vol. 16, no. 3, pp. 512–523, Mar. 2020, pmid:31787561
  84. 84. Schaefer S. Y., Hooyman A., and Duff K., “Using a Timed Motor Task to Predict One-Year Functional Decline in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment.,” Journal of Alzheimer’s disease: JAD, vol. 77, no. 1. pp. 53–58, 2020. pmid:32651327
  85. 85. Schaefer S. Y., Malek-Ahmadi M., Hooyman A., King J. B., and Duff K., “Association between motor task acquisition and hippocampal atrophy across cognitively unimpaired, amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease individuals,” medRxiv, p. 2021.05.31.21258061, Jan. 2021,