Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Cortical oscillatory dysfunction in Parkinson disease during movement activation and inhibition

  • Elizabeth A. Disbrow ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    elizabeth.disbrow@lsuhs.edu

    Affiliations LSU Health Shreveport Center for Brain Health, Shreveport, Louisiana, United States of America, Department of Neurology, LSU Health Shreveport, Shreveport, Louisiana, United States of America

  • Nathaniel D. Glassy,

    Roles Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation LSU Health Shreveport Center for Brain Health, Shreveport, Louisiana, United States of America

  • Elizabeth M. Dressler,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation

    Affiliation LSU Health Shreveport Center for Brain Health, Shreveport, Louisiana, United States of America

  • Kimberley Russo,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, UC Berkeley, Berkeley, California, United States of America

  • Elizabeth A. Franz,

    Roles Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Action Brain and Cognition Laboratory, Department of Psychology, and fMRIotago, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand

  • Robert S. Turner,

    Roles Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Neurobiology and Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • Maria I. Ventura,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, UC Davis, Sacramento, California, United States of America

  • Leighton Hinkley,

    Roles Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Radiology and Biomedical Imaging, University of California, San Francisco, California, United States of America

  • Richard Zweig,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations LSU Health Shreveport Center for Brain Health, Shreveport, Louisiana, United States of America, Department of Neurology, LSU Health Shreveport, Shreveport, Louisiana, United States of America

  • Srikantan S. Nagarajan,

    Roles Software

    Affiliation Department of Radiology and Biomedical Imaging, University of California, San Francisco, California, United States of America

  • Christina R. Ledbetter,

    Roles Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations LSU Health Shreveport Center for Brain Health, Shreveport, Louisiana, United States of America, Department of Neurosurgery, LSU Health Shreveport, Shreveport, Louisiana, United States of America

  • Karen A. Sigvardt †

    † Deceased.

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, UC Davis, Sacramento, California, United States of America

Abstract

Response activation and inhibition are functions fundamental to executive control that are disrupted in Parkinson disease (PD). We used magnetoencephalography to examine event related changes in oscillatory power amplitude, peak latency and frequency in cortical networks subserving these functions and identified abnormalities associated with PD. Participants (N = 18 PD, 18 control) performed a cue/target task that required initiation of an un-cued movement (activation) or inhibition of a cued movement. Reaction times were variable but similar across groups. Task related responses in gamma, alpha, and beta power were found across cortical networks including motor cortex, supplementary and pre- supplementary motor cortex, posterior parietal cortex, prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate. PD-related changes in power and latency were noted most frequently in the beta band, however, abnormal power and delayed peak latency in the alpha band in the pre-supplementary motor area was suggestive of a compensatory mechanism. PD peak power was delayed in pre-supplementary motor area, motor cortex, and medial frontal gyrus only for activation, which is consistent with deficits in un-cued (as opposed to cued) movement initiation characteristic of PD.

Introduction

Response initiation and inhibition are functions fundamental to executive control that are disrupted in Parkinson disease (PD) [13]. Response initiation, sometimes referred to as ‘movement activation,’ refers to the process of eliciting a desired response, whereas response inhibition refers to the capacity to suppress inappropriate or irrelevant responses that are prepotent. People with PD often show deficits on standard neuropsychological tests of inhibition, such as the Stroop Test [4, 5], which is consistent with the idea that inhibition plays a role in automatic behaviors and action override, functions that are also disrupted in PD [6]. Furthermore, impairments in response activation and inhibition have been proposed to subserve some of the common motor signs of PD. For example, akinesia, or an inability to start movement [710] is consistent with a problem in movement activation [3]. Activation and inhibition are also associated with motor switching and sequencing, which are impaired in PD [3, 11]. However, deficits in response activation and inhibition associated with PD are complex and not fully understood. For instance, in PD, the latency to initiate movement can be shortened by the use of pre-movement external auditory or visual cueing such as walking in time to a metronome or walking paced by floor markers (e.g., [1216]).

Many behavioral studies have shown that people with PD display deficits in both the activation and inhibition of motor responses [1719]. Studies targeting components of responding traditionally use two types of tasks, the classic ‘Go Nogo’ tasks [20] and ‘stop-signal’ tasks [21]. Wu and colleagues [22] found that people with PD performing a Go Nogo task demonstrated increased reaction times for ‘Go’ trials and increased errors for both ‘Go’ and ‘Nogo’ trials as compared to controls. Franz and Miller [23] found that people with mild to moderate PD demonstrated abnormal force output during inhibition of ‘Nogo’ responses in comparison to controls, despite a lack of difference in mean reaction times between PD and control groups. Previously, we [1] also observed an absence of statistically significant reaction time differences between PD and control groups during movement activation and inhibition in PD (though variability was high) despite deterioration in spatial and temporal specificity of blood-oxygen-level-dependent imaging signal in cortical and subcortical regions of interest.

Anatomical evidence indicates that motor and cognitive function are subserved by anatomically segregated parallel circuits from the cerebral cortex to thalamus via the basal ganglia nuclei which form circuits with projections back to the cortex. Segregated parallel loops are thought to mediate distinct motor, cognitive, and limbic functions based on cortical targets [2426]. For example, abnormalities in motor cortical activity observed in PD [27, 28] have been linked to loss of dopamine in the posterior putamen [29, 30]. Similarly, studies of executive dysfunction in PD suggest that loss of dopamine in the caudate results in abnormalities in activity in prefrontal cortex (PFC; [3133] for review). This PFC-connected cognitive circuit has also been implicated in important aspects of both response activation and inhibition behaviors ([34] for review).

There is evidence in the electroencephalography (EEG) literature of delayed onset and decreased power in the event related responses for components of movement activation and inhibition in people with PD [3539], though not all studies agree (see [40] for review). Furthermore, growing evidence suggests that parkinsonian impairments in motor and cognitive function are associated with dysregulated cortical oscillatory activity. Oscillatory activity is thought to reflect modulation of neuronal excitability in synchronously active neural assemblies [41, 42]. For example, using magnetoencephalography (MEG), a diffuse slowing of oscillatory activity at rest has been observed in people with PD, as well as increased beta band power over sensorimotor cortex at rest [43]. Such changes are reported early in disease progression, and can be measured regardless of disease duration, stage, severity, or medication level [44]. A number of studies demonstrate that in PD, global changes in oscillatory power are correlated with cognitive dysfunction as well (for a review, see [4549]). Attenuated desynchronization of both beta band [22] and higher alpha band [50] EEG has been reported in people with PD during Go and Nogo trials [51]. However, few studies have brought to bear the superior temporal and spatial resolutions of MEG to elucidate the abnormalities in brain activity associated with movement activation and inhibition in PD.

Our goal was to examine PD-associated abnormalities in the magnitude and timing of oscillatory power in the basal ganglia thalamocortical circuit subserving response activation and inhibition. We tested the hypothesis that oscillatory activity in the frontal cortical regions subserving these functions had reduced event related power change and delayed onset in people with mild to moderate PD on dopamine replacement therapy compared to control participants. This hypothesis was based on previous work showing increased cortical resting state oscillatory synchronization and increased event related response latency in PD [3540, 43, 5254]. We used MEG, which combines high temporal precision and spatial resolution in the cortex to measure anatomical, at the level of the cortical field, region of interest, and physiological, specifically electrophysiological data. Unlike previous work, we matched motor output across trial types, allowing us to isolate the pre-movement processes of activation and inhibition to examine network dysfunction in PD.

Methods

Subjects

Individuals with PD were recruited from the movement disorders clinic at the UC Davis Medical Center. Clinical diagnosis of Parkinson disease was made according to Queen Square Brain Bank criteria [55] by a movement disorders neurologist and confirmed by review of medical records. Eighteen people with PD (4 female, 14 male) and 18 controls (8 female, 10 male) participated in this study. All participants were right-handed and PD participants had right side (of body) dominant PD, defined by side of initial symptom onset. Statistical power analysis based on Beste and colleagues [56] showed an amplitude difference between PD and control groups of about 8 ±2 μV in P300 peak amplitude for both compatible and incompatible Go trials. Using an alpha of 0.05 for a single comparison, a sample size of 18 per group yielded power of over 90% for an independent, 2 sample t-test comparing PD and control groups. Similar results were obtained from a power analysis of peak latency measures from the same study.

Inclusion criteria were male or female > 55 years old and fluent in English. Additional PD specific inclusion criteria were right-handed and right sided PD dominant onset. Exclusion criteria were history of severe head trauma based on patient report; significant uncorrected visual impairment; significant additional medical conditions known to affect cognitive function; history of substance or alcohol abuse; inability to understand informed consent, study purpose and procedures or other study materials involved in the research study; and women who were or might have been pregnant. Additional exclusion criteria included depression (Beck Depression Scale score >20; [57]), significant cognitive impairment (Mini-Mental State Exam <25; [58]), and excessive daytime sleepiness (Epworth Sleepiness Scale score >10; [59]). MRI specific exclusion criteria were significant claustrophobia or other identified problem making the MRI environment intolerable; body weight >300lbs; pacemakers, artificial limbs, or other implanted medical devices that contained metal; other metal objects, such as jewelry, piercings, braces, or internal prosthetics that were not MRI compatible and could not be removed based on pre-MRI screening form.

PD specific exclusion criteria were atypical PD, persistent tremor (score > 1 on UPDRS items 16, 20 or 21 for best on medication state), presence of motor fluctuations (score >1 on UPDRS items 36–39), or dyskinesia (score >1 on UPDRS items 32–34). Participants with PD were given the UPDRS on medication on a separate day from brain imaging. Written consent from each participant was obtained prior to the experiment and procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board on Human Subjects Research at the University of California, Davis.

All patients had late onset (>55 years at time of diagnosis) idiopathic PD with a history of positive response to dopamine replacement therapy and no alterations in medication for 6 weeks prior to enrollment. PD participants took their prescribed medication in the morning prior to study participation and performed the experiment in their best ON medication state. Thus, PD participants performed the tasks while on levodopa with carbidopa, dopamine agonist, Amantadine, Monoamine Oxidase Type B inhibitors and/or Catechol-O-Methyl Transferase inhibitor treatment for Parkinson disease. Dopamine equivalency for each PD participant’s daily medications was calculated based on established conversion factors [60].

Activation inhibition task

Subjects were presented with a visual cue-target design task and required to respond to the target by pushing a button with one or both index fingers. Fiber optic button boxes (Photon Control, Inc. <http://www.photoncontrol.com>) were held in both the left and right hands. Subjects were trained prior to entering the scanner to respond only to the target arrow(s) with their index fingers pressing hand-held button boxes. The training task consisted of a series of stimuli that contained 2 trials of each trial type. All subjects executed this trial run once before scanning following which they reported understanding the task. Stimuli were generated on a PC using Presentation software (www.neurobs.com/presentation). Stimuli were projected into the magnetically shielded room using a Christie Lx41 projector (Christine Digital, Cypress) onto a screen using a series of mirrors. Participants lay supine and their heads were padded to reduce movement.

On each trial, stimuli consisted of a visual fixation cross (a white + sign on a black background), which was present for the entire trial, and subjects were instructed to focus on this fixation point. A cue arrow appeared superimposed on the fixation cross, followed by a target arrow indicating the response to produce on a particular trial. The arrow cue (presented in orange) appeared for 200 ms followed by a delay interval that varied randomly between 600 and 1200 ms. The target arrow then appeared in the same central position (in blue) for 1000 ms. The inter-trial interval (ITI) was 2000–3000 ms (Fig 1A).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Trial timeline.

(A) Arrow cue timeline for a bilateral cue and unilateral target. (B) Example of arrow cue visual presentation to subjects. The examples are for bilateral or right-hand trials. Left-hand stimulus pattern was identical. Letters indicate cue and target type, B = bilateral, L = left, R = right.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0257711.g001

Cue and target stimuli were pairs of either unidirectional arrows pointing to the left or right, or bidirectional (double-headed) arrows. Subjects were instructed to respond to the target arrow with either a unimanual (in the direction of a single-headed arrow) or bimanual (in the case of bidirectional arrows) button press. One of seven possible pairs of cue/target stimuli was presented: bilateral cue and bilateral target (cue = B, target = B), right cue and bilateral target (RB), left cue and bilateral target (LB), right cue and right target (RR), left cue and left target (LL), bilateral cue and right target (BR), or bilateral cue and left target (BL). These combinations were grouped into two trial types: matched trials, where the cue and target arrows were the same (RR, LL, and BB), and mismatched trials, where the cue and target arrows were mismatched (BR, BL, RB, and LB). The mismatched trials were further classified into two key types that represent distinct types of behavioral functions. In the first type, subjects were required to activate an un-cued button press movement, as in the case of RB and LB trials. For these trials, a unilateral cue followed by a bilateral target requires initiation of a response that is un-cued for one hand, the hand not indicated by the cue. This added response (not previously cued) requires movement activation of a non-activated/non-cued response. In the second type, which we called inhibition trials, subjects were required to inhibit a cued movement, as in the BR and BL trials. In this case, a bilateral response is cued, but subjects must inhibit the already cued response on the target trial, responding with a single hand. In each of the 7 trial types there were 120 trials during a 70-minute scan session (some examples of trial types are shown in Fig 1B). Trial types were presented in random order.

Behavioral data

Behavioral measures (reaction time and errors) were collected using ADC channels from voltage changes in the button box and extracted using CTF Data Editor software (https://www.ctf.com/products). Datasets were sorted by trial type (matched, mismatched: activation, mismatched: inhibition), and errors were detected by visually inspecting voltage changes in ADC channels. An error was defined as a unilateral button press of the incorrect button, a unilateral response when a bilateral response was required or a bilateral response when a unilateral response was required. Error trials were excluded from analysis. For epoching of data, target stimulus presentation was marked as 0 ms. Reaction time was calculated for correct trials as the time between the onset of the target arrow to the onset of the button press for each response.

Acquisition

Neuromagnetic activity was recorded in a magnetically shielded room using a whole-head biomagnetometer (CTF MEG, Coquitlam, Canada) with 275 first-order axial gradiometers and 27 reference sensors that enabled collection of synthetic third-order gradient data with improved signal-to-noise ratio. Coils at the nasion and 1 cm from the tragus, rostral to the left and right periauricular points in the direction of the nasion, were used to quantify head position relative to the sensor array. These points were later co-registered to the structural MRI through a multi-sphere head model. Scan sessions where head movement exceeded 5 mm within a run were discarded and repeated. Epochs of 3–6 seconds duration were acquired using a sampling rate of 1200 Hz.

For reconstructions of the MEG data in source space, a structural MRI scan was acquired on a 1.5 T GE Signa scanner using an MP-RAGE (multiplanar rapidly acquired gradient echo) imaging sequence with the following parameters: repetition time (TR), 7.87 s; echo time (TE), 2.69 ms; flip angle, 8 degrees; slices, 200; field of view, 256 mm; resolution, 1 x 1 x 1.2 mm.

Data for this study has been made publicly available under Creative Commons CC0 1.0 Universal (CC0 1.0) at https://doi.org/10.12751/g-node.nb35ss. The data is stored in BIDS format using the CTF *.ds filetype. It can be opened in either proprietary CTF software (Omega 2000) or open-source M/EEG software packages (NUTMEG, fieldtrip). The Fieldtrip website expands on the file format here: https://www.fieldtriptoolbox.org/getting_started/ctf/.

Analysis

Trials were corrected for noise and movement artifacts and error trials were discarded using DataEditor software (https://www.ctf.com/products). Noise and artifacts were identified visually by scanning trials for patterns caused by eye blinks, saccades, and head motion (MEG sensor amplitude exceeding 20 pT). Neural sources were estimated in the time-frequency domain using the Neurodynamic Utility Toolbox for MEG (NUTMEG; https://www.nitrc.org/projects/nutmeg/) across a shared computing cluster at the California Institute for Quantitative Biomedical Research (www.qb3.org). Changes in induced (non-phase locked) activity were estimated using an adaptive spatial filtering technique (lead field resolution = 5 mm), which effectively weights each source estimate (voxel) relative to the signal of the MEG sensors [6163].

Source power for each voxel was determined by comparing the magnitude of the signal during an ‘active’ experimental time window versus a pre-stimulus baseline ‘control’ window [61, 64] using a noise-corrected pseudo-F statistic expressed in logarithmic units (decibels (dB); [61]). The data were stimulus-locked (target onset = 0 ms) followed by 25 ms time windows until 1112.5 ms post-target onset. Data were passed through a filter bank and partitioned into partially overlapping time windows of varying size (100, 150, 200, and 300 ms) optimized to capture spectral peaks in the MEG signal [61, 63, 65]. Twenty-five ms steps were then estimated in the alpha (8–12 Hz), beta (12–30 Hz), and gamma (30–55 Hz) bands.

Whole-brain reconstructions of oscillatory activity were co-registered with each individual’s structural MRI. Each reconstruction was spatially normalized by applying a transformation matrix derived from the normalization of the structural MRI to a standard T1 template brain (Montreal Neurological Institute; MNI305) using SPM2 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm2). Averages and variance maps were smoothed using a Gaussian kernel with a 20 mm3 width (full-width half-maximum) [63, 65]. Spatially normalized activation maps were entered into a group analysis using statistical non-parametric mapping (SnPM) [66], a statistical metric known to accommodate non-normally distributed MEG datasets [67]. Regions of interest (ROI) based on Brodmann nomenclature were derived from MNI coordinates in the normalized brain [68]. Permutation testing (2N possible combinations of negations) to assess significance were performed both within-group (one-sample t-test, mean difference between conditions) and between the control and PD groups (unpaired t-tests). Within group analyses were corrected for multiple comparisons at a familywise error rate cutoff of p<0.05. Between-group analysis was corrected with a false discovery rate threshold of p<0.05. For the gamma band, we began with a conservative (multiple comparison corrected) threshold of p<0.05 as a first step, with a more liberal threshold (significant at p<0.05, uncorrected) as a second step if significant effects were not observed at a conservative level. The details of this approach have been described elsewhere [61, 67, 69]. As we have noted previously [61], the reduced power inherent to higher frequency oscillations often require us to rely on more liberal thresholds with a concomitant increase in risk for type 1 error [61, 65].

Latency data was obtained from simple activation and inhibition trials (not contrasts). Onset and duration for each region of interest was determined by using the time frequency viewer to select the first and last 25 ms time window where the ROI was significant at its threshold. Between group differences in behavioral and latency data were evaluated using repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with post hoc analysis using a p value of 0.05.

Results

Subjects

There were no significant differences across groups for age F(1, 35) = 0.65, p = 0.43 or years of education F(1,35) = 1.64, p = 0.21. Gender distribution was not significantly different across groups X2(1, 35) = 2, p = 0.16. Demographic data are presented in Table 1.

thumbnail
Table 1. Demographic data reported as mean and (SD) except for H&Y, which is the median.

M = male, F = female, UPDRS = Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0257711.t001

Behavioral data

Repeated measures ANOVA revealed that there were no significant differences in reaction times for either group or any trial type F(1,70) = 2.25, p = 0.14. Error rates were significantly higher in the PD group for all trials F(1,70) = 6.22, p = 0.015. The means and standard deviations for reaction times and error rate are reported in Tables 2 and 3.

thumbnail
Table 2. Reaction time (ms) for control, activation, and inhibition trials.

Values are mean reaction time (SD) in milliseconds (ms) from target presentation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0257711.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Error rate for control, activation, and inhibition trials.

Values are mean percentages (SD), calculated by number of incorrect trials / number of total trials. *p < .05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0257711.t003

Task based activity

In general we observed brain regions that showed statistically significant event related changes in oscillatory power (p<0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons at a familywise error rate; not shown) following response (button press) activation or inhibition that included Brodmann area 4 (motor cortex), lateral BA 6 (pre-supplementary motor area), medial BA 6 (supplementary motor area), BA 7 (posterior parietal cortex), BA 24 (anterior cingulate cortex), and BA 9/10 (medial and anterior prefrontal cortex, specifically medial and superior frontal gyrus). Areas 9 and 10 were reported together because activity frequently overlapped at the border between these two regions. Right- and left-hand trials both tested activation and inhibition in a similar fashion, and results from right- and left-hand trials were similar, so data from right-hand response trials were used to illustrate the results. Right-hand results were chosen because all subjects were right-handed with right side dominant disease. Right- and left-hand response MEG data were not combined because frontal and sensorimotor cortex results were not aligned due to crossed inputs from the two hands, while frontal lobe activation was independent of response hand.

Across all tasks (control subject matched trial data not shown), primary motor cortex, lateral pre-supplementary motor area, and posterior parietal cortex showed statistically significant decreases in power compared to baseline, while supplementary motor area and anterior cingulate cortex showed increases. In medial/ anterior prefrontal cortex, we observed both increases and decreases in power in multiple frequency bands and time points. Of interest here are results from 1) control subject motor planning (matched vs. mismatched trial contrasts for activation (Fig 2A) and inhibition (Fig 4A); and 2) difference in motor planning across disease groups (control vs. PD subject mismatched trials contrasts for activation (Fig 2B) and inhibition (Fig 4B). Significant PD associated power changes in specific frequency bands, latencies, and brain regions are described below.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Activation contrast analysis.

(A) Differences in peak intensity in the control group for mismatched activation trials versus matched control trials. (B) Differences in peak intensity during mismatched activation trials between the PD and control groups. MS = millisecond. Scale color bar represents power in arbitrary units. Peak activity indicates a significant change at p<0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons except for the gamma band. To identify gamma band activity, we used a more liberal threshold (significant at p<0.05, uncorrected). R = right hand, B = bilateral.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0257711.g002

Activation contrast analysis (unilateral cue, bilateral target vs. bilateral cue, bilateral target)

For activation trials, subjects were presented with a unilateral cue and bilateral target; for example, right cue, bilateral target (RB), which required activation of a (un-cued) left-hand response. In control subjects, brain activity from these trials was compared to that from control trials matched for motor output that did not require response activation (BB, bilateral cue, bilateral target; Fig 2). The control group contrast analysis (Fig 2, left, false discovery rate corrected threshold of p<0.05) revealed peak increased power in the gamma band in left medial/ anterior prefrontal cortex at 337.5 ms. There was also a peak power increase in the alpha band in anterior cingulate cortex at 337.5 ms. In bilateral medial/ anterior prefrontal cortex, there were peaks in increased power in the beta band at 537.5 ms.

To identify differences in PD brain activity patterns following response activation (of a not previously cued response), we compared trials consisting of a unilateral cue and bilateral target in the control versus PD groups. The contrast analysis of mismatched activation trials in the PD group versus control group (RB PD vs. RB Control; Fig 2, right; false discovery rate corrected threshold of p<0.05) revealed a relative power increase in the beta band in the bilateral primary motor cortex that peaked at 387.5 ms. We identified a decrease in power in the alpha band in left supplementary motor area, a decrease in power in the beta band in the left medial/ anterior prefrontal cortex, both at 387.5 ms. In the beta band we also observed a decrease in power in posterior parietal cortex at 512.5 ms in the left hemisphere in the PD versus the control groups.

Repeated measures ANOVA of response latency data from both matched and mismatched (activation and inhibition) trials revealed that changes in power peaked at longer latencies in the PD group relative to those in the control group. For matched (BB) trials (Fig 3A), the PD group gamma band peak power change latency was delayed relative to controls in the left supplementary motor area (BA 6; t(34) = -2.595, p = 0.014), and in the beta band in left medial/ anterior prefrontal cortex (BA 9/10; t(34) = -2.201, p = 0.035). For mismatched activation trials (Fig 3B), peak changes in power also occurred at longer latencies for the PD group, but to differing degrees depending on the area, leading to a significant interaction between ROI and group (F(3.95, 134.26) = 4.504, p = 0.002). PD group peak latency was longer in the gamma band in left supplementary motor area (BA 6; t(34) = -2.595, p = 0.014) and in the beta band in left primary motor cortex (BA 4; t(34) = -2.377, p = 0.023). PD group peak latency was also longer in the alpha band in left medial/ anterior prefrontal cortex (BA 9/10; t(34) = 2.175, p = 0.037).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Activation latency.

(A) Onset and duration (gray and white bars) of power change in each ROI for the matched control trials. Latency of peak power change is indicated by solid black or white lines. (B) Onset and duration of power change in each ROI for the mismatched activation condition. Peak latency was significantly longer in the PD vs. control group: *p < 0.05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0257711.g003

Inhibition contrast analysis (bilateral cue, unilateral target vs. unilateral cue, unilateral target)

For response inhibition trials, subjects were presented with a bilateral cue and unilateral target; for example, bilateral cue, right target (BR), which required inhibition of a left-hand cued response. In control subjects, brain activity from these trials was compared to control trials matched for motor output that did not require response inhibition (RR, unilateral cue, unilateral target; Fig 4A).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Inhibition contrast analysis.

(A) Differences in peak power change in the control group for mismatched inhibition trials versus matched control trials. (B) Differences in peak power change during mismatched inhibition task between the PD and control groups. Conventions as in Fig 2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0257711.g004

Control group contrast analysis (Fig 4A) revealed statistically significant peak increased power (p<0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons at a familywise error rate) in the gamma band in medial supplementary motor area (BA 6) at 287.5 ms. Maximum decreased power was observed in the gamma band in left posterior parietal cortex (BA 7) at 337.5 ms, while peak increased power was found in the beta band in left medial/ anterior prefrontal cortex (BA 9/10) at 362.5 ms. We additionally observed maximum decreased power in the gamma band in right pre-supplementary motor area (BA 6) at 437.5 ms.

To identify differences in PD brain activity patterns following response inhibition, we compared control bilateral cue unilateral target trials to the same trials in the PD group. The contrast analysis of mismatched inhibition trials in the PD group versus the control group (Fig 4, right, false discovery rate corrected threshold of p<0.05) revealed bilateral decreases in beta power in medial/ anterior prefrontal cortex at 312.5 ms (maximum right hemisphere) and 362.5 ms (maximum left hemisphere) in PD that was not present in control data. Increased alpha band power was observed in left medial/ anterior prefrontal cortex in the PD compared to the control group with a peak latency of 387.5 ms. We saw increased power in PD compared to controls in the beta band in primary motor cortex with a peak latency of 387.5 ms. We also observed greater power decreases in the gamma band in the PD group compared to controls with peak latencies of 112.5 and 612.5 ms in contralateral pre-supplementary motor area, indicating delayed onset and termination of desynchronous activity in this region in the PD group (Fig 5B).

thumbnail
Fig 5. Inhibition latency.

(A) Onset and duration (gray and white bars) of power change in each ROI for the matched control trials. Latency of peak intensity is indicated by solid black or white lines. (B) Onset and duration of power change in each ROI for the mismatched inhibition condition. Peak latency was significantly longer in the PD vs. control group: *p < 0.05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0257711.g005

Repeated measures ANOVA of response latency data from both matched and mismatched trials revealed that changes in power peaked at a longer latencies in the PD group relative to those in the control group. We observed differences in peak latency between the PD group and the control group only for matched trials (RR; Fig 5A). PD group peak latency was longer in right medial/ anterior prefrontal cortex in the alpha band (t(34) = -3.213, p = 0.003) and in the beta band (t(34) = -3.327, p = 0.002). Variability was high in gamma latency in left lateral pre-supplementary motor area. There were no peak latency differences between groups in the mismatched inhibition trials (BR; Fig 5B).

Discussion

We tested the hypothesis that oscillatory activity in the frontal cortical regions underlying response activation and inhibition had reduced event related power change and delayed onset in PD compared to control participants. We extend previous work by providing cortical oscillatory power and latency data with ROI spatial resolution specific to motor planning, and describe abnormalities in the cortical activation and inhibition responses associated with PD. Despite similar reaction time performance, in PD motor cortex we found increased beta band synchronization and delayed peak latency in primary motor cortex as in previous studies (see [46] for review). We also found reduced event related power changes on correct trials throughout the frontal cortical regions subserving response activation and inhibition consistent with previous work (for review see [34, 70]). Peak latency was delayed in PD for movement activation across frequency bands and brain regions while inhibition data was not different across groups. Furthermore, the changes in location and oscillatory power change of response were consistent with the concept of a compensatory mechanism.

Anatomy and physiology of movement activation and inhibition in PD

We found a group of cortical regions subserving movement activation and inhibition that included prefrontal, cingulate and supplementary motor cortex. In the control group we found increased beta band power in the left medial frontal gyrus during inhibition of a cued movement. Left medial prefrontal cortex is thought to be involved in sustained attention [7173], short term memory [74], perceptual decision making [75], and overriding automatic responses [76, 77], all of which are likely components of our task. In PD we observed that activation trials were associated with reduced alpha band power changes and delayed peak latency in prefrontal cortex. This region plays a role in the parallel inhibitory and excitatory regulation of neural activities associated with executive function (see [34] and [78] for a review) which are known to be disrupted in PD [79]. Furthermore, the left prefrontal cortex [80] and BA9 subserve error detection [76, 81, 82], and the inhibition response was attenuated in this region in people with PD. Damage to dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (anterior cingulate cortex and supplemental motor area) has also been linked to increased errors in Go Nogo paradigms [18]. Taken together, these findings suggest that increased error rate in PD may be related to abnormal activity in prefrontal cortex.

We also observed event related power changes in cingulate and supplementary motor cortex during movement activation and inhibition that appeared to be preserved in PD. The anterior cingulate, specifically the dorsal anterior cingulate or midcingulate cortex [83, 84], is functionally linked with the prefrontal cortex [8587], and is likely to play a role in attention processing [53, 88], working memory [76, 89, 90], conflict [9195], and error processing [96, 97]. While we found that event related alpha band power changes in the cingulate were preserved in PD, previous data from this region showed significantly higher levels of slow wave activity during resting conditions in people with PD [53, 54]. In fact, while Braak staging suggests that anterior cingulate involvement occurs in later stage PD [98], there is accumulating evidence of abnormalities in cortical thickness, cerebral blood flow, fractional anisotropy, dopamine-2 receptor binding and connectivity earlier in the disease (for review see [99]). Our contrasting findings may be related to the significant difference in experimental design and measurement modality across studies and to the impact of plastic compensatory brain changes [100] on functional measures like ours.

In medial supplementary motor area in the control group we observed increased gamma band power during inhibition of a cued movement that was preserved in PD. The role of the right pre-supplementary motor area in response inhibition has been well documented (e.g., [18, 81, 101106]), and it has been established that the pre-supplementary motor area is involved in motor response inhibition [19, 97, 104, 107, 108], active during stop signal and ‘change of plan’ task paradigms [105] (see [109] for a review), and may be associated with decision making [110], mediation of attention and motor response activation [111]. The normal gamma band power changes may be related to the relatively healthy inhibition related peak power changes observed in PD. In contrast, we found decreased power and increased latency in the alpha band for activation in medial supplementary motor area in PD compared to controls. The link between supplementary motor area function and timing of motor planning such as anticipatory postural adjustments is impaired in PD [112]. It is interesting to note that motor responses were required for both activation and inhibition trials, the difference being that activation requires execution of an un-cued movement. Behavioral deficits specific to un-cued, as opposed to cued movement initiation are well documented in PD [12, 113115], which is consistent with our data showing delayed peak latency in motor premotor and supplementary motor cortex for activation trials.

Frequency specific cortical oscillatory power

Magnetic and electrical signal synchronization and desynchronization are thought to reflect dynamic communication between spatially distributed brain regions [116, 117]. Oscillatory power at different frequencies has been associated with unique behavioral functions (for a review, see [118]). Power in the gamma band (33–60 Hz) reflects active engagement (i.e., in feature integration, attention, and movement preparation, depending on the cortical area) [119, 120]. In controls, changes in gamma band power for activation and inhibition networks [93, 121124]. The pattern of gamma band activity was similar to the peripheral attention network described by Corbetta and colleagues [125], and appeared to be relatively intact in PD. We found no differences in gamma band power change amplitude or location in PD. However peak latency was delayed in several ROI’s, consistent with existing studies showing reduced speed of processing in PD [122, 126128]. In contrast, inhibition related gamma band changes have been reported in the subthalamic nucleus in PD [129], particularly in response to failed inhibition trials [130]. and increased gamma desynchronization following a Go cue was positively correlated with reaction time in PD [130]. Cortical gamma activity is also modulated by thalamic alpha activity [131], and Yoo and colleagues [132] found that alpha-gamma coupling was higher in PD compared to controls.

Reduced power in the beta band frequencies has been associated with movement preparation and execution [7, 133135] as well as cognitive control [118]. Changes in beta power prior to target onset have also been shown to correlate with reaction time in PD and healthy controls [133]. Furthermore, healthy adults show an increase in beta band power over the frontal cortex while inhibiting responses during a stop-signal task [101]. As in previous work [22, 133, 136, 137], changes in beta band power were common in PD. Reduced beta band desynchronization in motor and prefrontal cortex during activation and inhibition may be related to reported increased synchronization in this region at rest in PD [43]. Interestingly, Heideman and colleagues [135] examined PD associated power differences in beta band, differentiating event amplitude, duration, and interval time. They found that beta-state interval time between short-lived, high-amplitude events accounted for decreases in beta band power in averaged responses in PD.

Alpha band activity is increased in cortical areas not engaged in a task [118], and event related synchronization in the alpha band is associated with top down control of response inhibition [138]. Previous work has shown that power changes in the alpha band prior to target onset, which were attenuated in PD, were correlated with reaction time [139, 140]. Across frequencies, where control subjects showed decreased event related power, this decrease was attenuated in PD. For example, we found decreased alpha band power in PD compared to controls in the supplementary motor area and medial/ anterior prefrontal cortex for response activation and inhibition, respectively. Decreased power in the alpha band in the frontal lobe has been associated with executive dysfunction in PD [141]. As in our study, Perfetti and colleagues [139] showed decreased alpha power in fronto-parietal cortex in PD prior to the presentation of a target. The change in alpha band power from baseline was positively correlated with reaction time in a reaching task [139]. Again, the attenuated event related changes in power in PD may be related to increased synchronization in resting state alpha band activity [141].

Compensation

There is a well described lag between the onset of nigrostriatal nerve terminal degeneration and the onset of motor signs in PD [142, 143] that is consistent with a capacity to compensate. In our study, reaction time did not differ significantly between groups, while differences in power were apparent in the network subserving activation of a cued movement, indicating that behavior was preserved in the face of changing brain function. For example, the decrease in alpha power in supplementary motor area during PD activation could be interpreted as recruitment of this region as a compensatory mechanism. Similarly, Buhmann and colleagues [144, 145] observed differences in left dorsal premotor cortex fMRI activity between asymptomatic Parkin mutation carriers and healthy non-carriers who performed a finger to thumb opposition task. Changes in left lateral supplementary motor area were only observed in response to a reduced cue condition where subjects were required to select which finger to tap. In addition, compensatory changes in connectivity in this region have been reported [145]. Compensation, therefore, is a possible explanation for the changes in alpha activity we observed in supplementary motor area [145].

However, disentangling compensation from pathophysiology is difficult. The simultaneous abnormal peaks (387.5 ms) in medial prefrontal cortex, primary motor cortex and supplementary motor area in the alpha and beta bands in PD activation could result from a single widely connected pathological source of circuit dysfunction. For example, the subthalamic nucleus, which is known to be dysfunctional in PD [146], has shown power changes during both movement activation and inhibition in PD [129, 147, 148]. Conversely, the additional distributed activity in primary motor cortex and supplementary motor area could be related to compensation for failing dopaminergic innervation, which is also consistent with the relatively normal response activation reaction times in the PD group. In a recent review of MEG and PD studies, Boon and colleagues [46] report that dopamine replacement therapy or deep brain stimulation normalized beta band power and interregional coupling while alleviating motor symptoms. These authors [46] suggested that the increased beta band power and connectivity in PD was a compensatory mechanism which became redundant once dopamine replacement therapy was administered.

Another possible interpretation of the abnormal distribution of activity is a change is response strategy. While reaction time was similar across groups, errors were more frequent in the PD group, suggestive of a shift in speed/accuracy trade off, maintaining speed at the cost of accuracy. This adjustment may be related to the recruitment of additional brain areas as well. Thus, while our findings of additional cortical power changes in PD are consistent with circuit dysfunction involving premotor and prefrontal cortex, the lack of reaction time deficits or changes in the speed/accuracy trade off in conjunction with activity observed in regions not commonly associated with PD pathology, such as posterior parietal cortex, could represent compensatory activity, though again, it is difficult to definitively differentiate compensation and pathological disinhibition [149, 150].

Clinical relevance

Currently, the utility of MEG in clinical practice is limited [46, 153]. The clinical significance of common MEG findings, such as the link between whole brain resting state spectral slowing and cognitive impairment are not clear. There is also a lack of consistent and comprehensive data examining the impact of dopamine replacement therapy and DBS on MEG outputs in PD ([46] for review). However, in development of clinical treatment advances which include forms of neurostimulation, understanding the underlying neural dynamics is crucial. Our findings inform those aspects of understanding. For example, there may be clinical relevance to hypothesized association of changes in alpha-band activity in supplementary motor cortex with compensatory mechanisms [144, 145]. may be possible to facilitate the brain’s intact compensatory mechanisms using neuromodulatory interventions that boost alpha power in BA 6 [151, 152]. However the clinical implications of the current results, though potentially significant, remain largely a matter of speculation.

Our observation of more widespread abnormalities in the neural activity associated with movement activation, as compared to inhibition, is consistent with the general characterization of parkinsonism as, first and foremost, a disorder of movement and with the preferential vulnerability of dopaminergic innervation of the basal ganglia’s motor territories [153, 154]. Indeed, many of the classical cardinal signs of PD (e.g., akinesia, bradykinesia, postural instability) fall easily within the general category of impairments of motor activation [155]. Data on abnormal cortical activity may inform intervention strategies [156158]. For example, cognitive neurorehabilitation targeting movement activation has shown some success [157, 158]. Future studies of neurorehabilitation using MEG could yield clinically significant insight into the plasticity subserving recovery of function. Impairments of movement inhibition, though clearly present in PD, were described only recently [2] and, arguably, make subtle and complex contributions to classic motor signs such as gait or intricate stepping abnormalities [156]. As in most complicated diseases, the picture of Parkinson’s disease becomes clearer with accumulation of evidence at all levels of the neural-cognitive system.

Limitations

Our ability to measure magnetic task based responses was limited to the cortex, specifically to tangential signals emanating from sulci [159, 160], because of the physics of MEG. Our sensors did not capture signal from subcortical structures such as the basal ganglia which clearly play an important role in response activation and inhibition [11, 27, 161, 162]. We did not evaluate the impact of dopamine replacement therapy on movement activation and inhibition though this response is significant [163, 164] (for review, see [165]). Our analysis of the gamma frequency band was conducted using an uncorrected p value. Despite our best efforts to reduce noise, for example enrolling only right-handed subjects with right side disease onset, gamma band activity was noisy, and the more stringent corrected p value yielded minimal consistent activation patterns. Thus our findings on gamma band activity have an increased susceptibility to Type 1 error, though no differences between PD and control participants were observed in the gamma band event related power changes. We did find changes in the location, amplitude, frequency and latency of response signals in optimally medicated PD participants. However disentangling the contribution of PD pathophysiology, network compensation and chronic, as opposed to the more natural event related sporadic fluctuations in dopamine is a daunting task that is key to understanding the disease mechanisms subserving PD.

Conclusions

Our findings are consistent with decreased event related power changes in the frontal cortex during movement activation and inhibition in PD. However the PD associated changes in the network subserving movement activation were widespread across frontal cortex and included motor regions, while those for inhibition were not as pronounced, though both trial types required a motor response. In addition, PD associated increases in peak latency were observed only in movement activation data, suggesting that deficits in movement production were more complex and potentially influential than deficits in inhibition. While difficult to dissociate from disease pathophysiology and medication effects, the changes in location and power of response were consistent with the concept of compensation following cell death in the substantia nigra.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Susanne Honma for assistance with data collection and analysis. Dr. Karen Sigvardt passed away before the submission of the final version of this manuscript. Dr. Elizabeth A. Disbrow accepts responsibility for the integrity and validity of the data collected and analyzed.

References

  1. 1. Disbrow EA, Sigvardt KA, Franz EA, Turner RS, Russo KA, Hinkley LB, et al. Movement Activation and Inhibition in Parkinson’s Disease: A Functional Imaging Study. J Park Dis. 2013;3: 181–192. pmid:23938347
  2. 2. Obeso I, Wilkinson L, Casabona E, Bringas ML, Álvarez M, Álvarez L, et al. Deficits in inhibitory control and conflict resolution on cognitive and motor tasks in Parkinson’s disease. Exp Brain Res. 2011;212: 371–384. pmid:21643718
  3. 3. Franz E. Converging evidence of the basal ganglia’s role in focused action selection and inhibition of competing information. In: Bezerd E, editor. Recent breakthroughs in basal ganglia research. 2006. pp. 227–239.
  4. 4. Henik A, Singh J, Beckley DJ, Rafal RD. Disinhibition of Automatic Word Reading in Parkinson’s Disease. Cortex. 1993;29: 589–599. pmid:8124936
  5. 5. Hsieh Y-H, Chen K-J, Wang C-C, Lai C-L. Cognitive and Motor Components of Response Speed in the Stroop Test in Parkinson’s Disease Patients. Kaohsiung J Med Sci. 2008;24: 197–203. pmid:18424356
  6. 6. van den Wildenberg WPM, Ridderinkhof KR, van Wouwe NC, Neimat JS, Bashore TR, Wylie SA. Overriding actions in Parkinson’s disease: Impaired stopping and changing of motor responses. Behav Neurosci. 2017;131: 372–384. pmid:28805433
  7. 7. Brown RG, Dowsey PL, Brown P, Jahanshahi M, Pollak P, Benabid AL, et al. Impact of deep brain stimulation on upper limb akinesia in Parkinson’s disease. Ann Neurol. 1999;45: 473–488. pmid:10211472
  8. 8. Berardelli A, Rothwell JC, Thompson PD, Hallett M. Pathophysiology of bradykinesia in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2001;124: 2131–2146. pmid:11673316
  9. 9. Jankovic J. Motor fluctuations and dyskinesias in Parkinson’s disease: Clinical manifestations. Mov Disord. 2005;20: S11–S16. pmid:15822109
  10. 10. Onofrj M, Thomas A. Acute akinesia in Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2005;64: 1162–1169. pmid:15824341
  11. 11. Cools AR, Jaspers R, Schwarz M, Sontag KH, Vries MV, van den Bercken J. Basal Ganglia and Switching Motor Programs. In: McKenzie JS, Kemm RE, Wilcock LN, editors. The Basal Ganglia. Boston, MA: Springer US; 1984. pp. 513–544.
  12. 12. Jahanshahi M, Brown RG, Marsden D. Simple and choice reaction time and the use of advance information for motor preparation in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 1992;115: 539–564. pmid:1606481
  13. 13. Rubinstein TC, Giladi N, Hausdorff JM. The power of cueing to circumvent dopamine deficits: A review of physical therapy treatment of gait disturbances in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2002;17: 1148–1160. pmid:12465051
  14. 14. Dibble LE, Nicholson DE, Shultz B, MacWilliams BA, Marcus RL, Moncur C. Sensory cueing effects on maximal speed gait initiation in persons with Parkinson’s disease and healthy elders. Gait Posture. 2004;19: 215–225. pmid:15125910
  15. 15. Nieuwboer A. Cueing for freezing of gait in patients with Parkinson’s disease: A rehabilitation perspective. Mov Disord. 2008;23: S475–S481. pmid:18668619
  16. 16. Spaulding SJ, Barber B, Colby M, Cormack B, Mick T, Jenkins ME. Cueing and Gait Improvement Among People With Parkinson’s Disease: A Meta-Analysis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2013;94: 562–570. pmid:23127307
  17. 17. van den Wildenberg WPM, van Boxtel GJM, van der Molen MW, Bosch DA, Speelman JD, Brunia CHM. Stimulation of the Subthalamic Region Facilitates the Selection and Inhibition of Motor Responses in Parkinson’s Disease. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18: 626–636. pmid:16768365
  18. 18. Chambers CD, Garavan H, Bellgrove MA. Insights into the neural basis of response inhibition from cognitive and clinical neuroscience. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009;33: 631–646. pmid:18835296
  19. 19. Aron AR, Poldrack RA. Cortical and Subcortical Contributions to Stop Signal Response Inhibition: Role of the Subthalamic Nucleus. J Neurosci. 2006;26: 2424–2433. pmid:16510720
  20. 20. Miller JO, Low K. Motor processes in simple, go/no-go, and choice reaction time tasks: A psychophysiological analysis. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2001;27: 266–289. pmid:11318047
  21. 21. Logan GD. On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A users’ guide to the stop signal paradigm. Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language. San Diego, CA, US: Academic Press; 1994. pp. 189–239.
  22. 22. Wu H-M, Hsiao F-J, Chen R-S, Shan D-E, Hsu W-Y, Chiang M-C, et al. Attenuated NoGo-related beta desynchronisation and synchronisation in Parkinson’s disease revealed by magnetoencephalographic recording. Sci Rep. 2019;9: 1–12. pmid:30626917
  23. 23. Franz EA, Miller J. Effects of response readiness on reaction time and force output in people with Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2002;125: 1733–1750. pmid:12135965
  24. 24. Alexander GE, Crutcher MD, DeLong MR. Basal ganglia-thalamocortical circuits: parallel substrates for motor, oculomotor, “prefrontal” and “limbic” functions. Prog Brain Res. 1990;85: 119–146. pmid:2094891
  25. 25. Alexander GE, Crutcher MD. Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: neural substrates of parallel processing. Trends Neurosci. 1990;13: 266–271. pmid:1695401
  26. 26. Middleton FA, Strick PL. Basal Ganglia Output and Cognition: Evidence from Anatomical, Behavioral, and Clinical Studies. Brain Cogn. 2000;42: 183–200. pmid:10744919
  27. 27. Lanciego JL, Luquin N, Obeso JA. Functional Neuroanatomy of the Basal Ganglia. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med. 2012;2: a009621–a009621. pmid:23071379
  28. 28. Chung SJ, Yoo HS, Lee HS, Oh JS, Kim JS, Sohn YH, et al. The Pattern of Striatal Dopamine Depletion as a Prognostic Marker in De Novo Parkinson Disease. Clin Nucl Med. 2018;43: 787–792. pmid:30153150
  29. 29. Redgrave P, Rodriguez M, Smith Y, Rodriguez-Oroz MC, Lehericy S, Bergman H, et al. Goal-directed and habitual control in the basal ganglia: implications for Parkinson’s disease. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11: 760–772. pmid:20944662
  30. 30. Goldstein DS, Sullivan P, Holmes C, Mash DC, Kopin IJ, Sharabi Y. Determinants of denervation-independent depletion of putamen dopamine in Parkinson’s disease and multiple system atrophy. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2017;35: 88–91. pmid:28034624
  31. 31. Lewis SJG, Slabosz A, Robbins TW, Barker RA, Owen AM. Dopaminergic basis for deficits in working memory but not attentional set-shifting in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43: 823–832. pmid:15716155
  32. 32. Monchi O, Petrides M, Strafella AP, Worsley KJ, Doyon J. Functional role of the basal ganglia in the planning and execution of actions. Ann Neurol. 2006;59: 257–264. pmid:16437582
  33. 33. Obeso JA, Lanciego JL. Past, Present, and Future of the Pathophysiological Model of the Basal Ganglia. Front Neuroanat. 2011;5. pmid:21808607
  34. 34. Ridderinkhof KR, van den Wildenberg WPM, Segalowitz SJ, Carter CS. Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: The role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning. Brain Cogn. 2004;56: 129–140. pmid:15518930
  35. 35. Solís-Vivanco R, Rodríguez-Violante M, Rodríguez-Agudelo Y, Schilmann A, Rodríguez-Ortiz U, Ricardo-Garcell J. The P3a wave: A reliable neurophysiological measure of Parkinson’s disease duration and severity. Clin Neurophysiol. 2015;126: 2142–2149. pmid:25655938
  36. 36. Solís-Vivanco R, Rodríguez-Violante M, Cervantes-Arriaga A, Justo-Guillén E, Ricardo-Garcell J. Brain oscillations reveal impaired novelty detection from early stages of Parkinson’s disease. NeuroImage Clin. 2018;18: 923–931. pmid:29876277
  37. 37. Tokic K, Titlic M, Beganovic-Petrovic A, Suljic E, Romac R, Silic S. P300 Wave Changes in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease. Med Arch. 2016;70: 453–456. pmid:28210020
  38. 38. Hünerli D, Emek-Savaş DD, Çavuşoğlu B, Dönmez Çolakoğlu B, Ada E, Yener GG. Mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease is associated with decreased P300 amplitude and reduced putamen volume. Clin Neurophysiol. 2019;130: 1208–1217. pmid:31163365
  39. 39. Ozmus G, Yerlikaya D, Gokceoglu A, Emek Savas DD, Cakmur R, Donmez Colakoglu B, et al. Demonstration of Early Cognitive Impairment in Parkinson’s Disease with Visual P300 Responses. Noro Psikiyatri Arsivi. 2017;54: 21–27. pmid:28566954
  40. 40. Seer C, Lange F, Georgiev D, Jahanshahi M, Kopp B. Event-related potentials and cognition in Parkinson’s disease: An integrative review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;71: 691–714. pmid:27498083
  41. 41. Fries P. A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9: 474–480. pmid:16150631
  42. 42. Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W. Neural Synchrony in Brain Disorders: Relevance for Cognitive Dysfunctions and Pathophysiology. Neuron. 2006;52: 155–168. pmid:17015233
  43. 43. Pollok B, Krause V, Martsch W, Wach C, Schnitzler A, Südmeyer M. Motor-cortical oscillations in early stages of Parkinson’s disease. J Physiol. 2012;590: 3203–3212. pmid:22547636
  44. 44. Stoffers D, Bosboom JLW, Deijen JB, Wolters EC, Berendse HW, Stam CJ. Slowing of oscillatory brain activity is a stable characteristic of Parkinson’s disease without dementia. Brain. 2007;130: 1847–1860. pmid:17412733
  45. 45. Geraedts VJ, Boon LI, Marinus J, Gouw AA, van Hilten JJ, Stam CJ, et al. Clinical correlates of quantitative EEG in Parkinson disease: A systematic review. Neurology. 2018;91: 871–883. pmid:30291182
  46. 46. Boon LI, Geraedts VJ, Hillebrand A, Tannemaat MR, Contarino MF, Stam CJ, et al. A systematic review of MEG-based studies in Parkinson’s disease: The motor system and beyond. Hum Brain Mapp. 2019;40: 2827–2848. pmid:30843285
  47. 47. Güntekin B, Aktürk T, Yıldırım E, Mantar N, Cadırcı F, Hanoglu L. P07-F Event related EEG brain oscillations differentiate cognitive decline in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Clin Neurophysiol. 2019;130: e68.
  48. 48. Caviness JN, Utianski RL, Hentz JG, Beach TG, Dugger BN, Shill HA, et al. Differential spectral quantitative electroencephalography patterns between control and Parkinson’s disease cohorts. Eur J Neurol. 2016;23: 387–392. pmid:26518336
  49. 49. Hassan M, Chaton L, Benquet P, Delval A, Leroy C, Plomhause L, et al. Functional connectivity disruptions correlate with cognitive phenotypes in Parkinson’s disease. NeuroImage Clin. 2017;14: 591–601. pmid:28367403
  50. 50. Lim VK, Hamm JeffP, Byblow WD, Kirk IanJ. Decreased desychronisation during self-paced movements in frequency bands involving sensorimotor integration and motor functioning in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Res Bull. 2006;71: 245–251. pmid:17113953
  51. 51. Hughes LE, Rittman T, Robbins TW, Rowe JB. Reorganization of cortical oscillatory dynamics underlying disinhibition in frontotemporal dementia. Brain. 2018;141: 2486–2499. pmid:29992242
  52. 52. Bokura H, Yamaguchi S, Kobayashi S. Event-related potentials for response inhibition in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43: 967–975. pmid:15716167
  53. 53. Hong X, Sun J, Wang J, Li C, Tong S. Attention-related modulation of frontal midline theta oscillations in cingulate cortex during a spatial cueing Go/NoGo task. Int J Psychophysiol. 2020;148: 1–12. pmid:31857191
  54. 54. Waninger S, Berka C, Stevanovic Karic M, Korszen S, Mozley PD, Henchcliffe C, et al. Neurophysiological Biomarkers of Parkinson’s Disease. J Park Dis. 2020;10: 471–480. pmid:32116262
  55. 55. Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, Lees AJ. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1992;55: 181–184. pmid:1564476
  56. 56. Beste C, Dziobek I, Hielscher H, Willemssen R, Falkenstein M. Effects of stimulus–response compatibility on inhibitory processes in Parkinson’s disease. Eur J Neurosci. 2009;29: 855–860. pmid:19200076
  57. 57. Lykouras L, Oulis P, Adrachta D, Daskalopoulou E, Kalfakis N, Triantaphyllou N, et al. Beck Depression Inventory in the Detection of Depression among Neurological Inpatients. Psychopathology. 1998;31: 213–219. pmid:9697165
  58. 58. Scheffels JF, Fröhlich L, Kalbe E, Kessler J. Concordance of Mini-Mental State Examination, Montreal Cognitive Assessment and Parkinson Neuropsychometric Dementia Assessment in the classification of cognitive performance in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Sci. 2020;412: 116735. pmid:32087430
  59. 59. Johns MW. A New Method for Measuring Daytime Sleepiness: The Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep. 1991;14: 540–545. pmid:1798888
  60. 60. Tomlinson CL, Stowe R, Patel S, Rick C, Gray R, Clarke CE. Systematic review of levodopa dose equivalency reporting in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2010;25: 2649–2653. https://doi.org/10.1002/mds.23429 pmid:21069833
  61. 61. Dalal SS, Guggisberg AG, Edwards E, Sekihara K, Findlay AM, Canolty RT, et al. Five-dimensional neuroimaging: Localization of the time–frequency dynamics of cortical activity. NeuroImage. 2008;40: 1686–1700. pmid:18356081
  62. 62. Sekihara K, Nagarajan SS. Adaptive Spatial Filters for Electromagnetic Brain Imaging. 1. Aufl. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag, Springer, Springer Berlin Heidelberg; 2008.
  63. 63. Hinkley LBN, Nagarajan SS, Dalal SS, Guggisberg AG, Disbrow EA. Cortical Temporal Dynamics of Visually Guided Behavior. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21: 519–529. pmid:20601397
  64. 64. Robinson S, Vrba J. Functional neuroimaging by synthetic aperture magnetometry (SAM). Recent Adv Biomagn. 1999; 302–305.
  65. 65. Guggisberg AG, Dalal SS, Findlay AM, Nagarajan SS. High-frequency oscillations in distributed neural networks reveal the dynamics of human decision making. Front Hum Neurosci. 2008;2. pmid:18958227
  66. 66. Nichols TE, Holmes AP. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: A primer with examples. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;15: 1–25. pmid:11747097
  67. 67. Singh KD, Barnes GR, Hillebrand A. Group imaging of task-related changes in cortical synchronisation using nonparametric permutation testing. NeuroImage. 2003;19: 1589–1601. pmid:12948714
  68. 68. Dalal SS, Zumer JM, Agrawal V, Hild KE, Sekihara K, Nagarajan SS. NUTMEG: a neuromagnetic source reconstruction toolbox. Neurol Clin Neurophysiol NCN. 2004;2004: 52. pmid:16012626
  69. 69. Hinkley LBN, Dale CL, Luks TL, Findlay AM, Bukshpun P, Pojman N, et al. Sensorimotor Cortical Oscillations during Movement Preparation in 16p11.2 Deletion Carriers. J Neurosci. 2019;39: 7321–7331. pmid:31270155
  70. 70. Munakata Y, Herd SA, Chatham CH, Depue BE, Banich MT, O’Reilly RC. A unified framework for inhibitory control. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15: 453–459. pmid:21889391
  71. 71. Ortuño F, Ojeda N, Arbizu J, López P, Marí-Climent JM, Peñuelas I, et al. Sustained Attention in a Counting Task: Normal Performance and Functional Neuroanatomy. NeuroImage. 2002;17: 411–420. pmid:12482094
  72. 72. Shallice T, Stuss DT, Alexander MP, Picton TW, Derkzen D. The multiple dimensions of sustained attention. Cortex. 2008;44: 794–805. pmid:18489960
  73. 73. Langner R, Eickhoff SB. Sustaining attention to simple tasks: A meta-analytic review of the neural mechanisms of vigilant attention. Psychol Bull. 2013;139: 870–900. pmid:23163491
  74. 74. Babiloni C, Ferretti A, Del Gratta C, Carducci F, Vecchio F, Romani GL, et al. Human cortical responses during one-bit delayed-response tasks: An fMRI study. Brain Res Bull. 2005;65: 383–390. pmid:15833592
  75. 75. Heekeren HR, Marrett S, Ruff DA, Bandettini PA, Ungerleider LG. Involvement of human left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in perceptual decision making is independent of response modality. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2006;103: 10023–10028. pmid:16785427
  76. 76. Mitchell DGV, Rhodes RA, Pine DS, Blair RJR. The contribution of ventrolateral and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex to response reversal. Behav Brain Res. 2008;187: 80–87. pmid:17950474
  77. 77. Kübler A, Dixon V, Garavan H. Automaticity and Reestablishment of Executive Control—An fMRI Study. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18: 1331–1342. pmid:16859418
  78. 78. Narayanan NS, Rodnitzky RL, Uc EY. Prefrontal dopamine signaling and cognitive symptoms of Parkinson’s disease. Rev Neurosci. 2013;24: 267–278. pmid:23729617
  79. 79. Knight RT, Richard Staines W, Swick D, Chao LL. Prefrontal cortex regulates inhibition and excitation in distributed neural networks. Acta Psychol (Amst). 1999;101: 159–178. pmid:10344184
  80. 80. Swick D, Ashley V, Turken AU. Left inferior frontal gyrus is critical for response inhibition. BMC Neurosci. 2008;9: 102. pmid:18939997
  81. 81. Krämer UM, Solbakk A-K, Funderud I, Løvstad M, Endestad T, Knight RT. The role of the lateral prefrontal cortex in inhibitory motor control. Cortex. 2013;49: 837–849. pmid:22699024
  82. 82. Chevrier AD, Noseworthy MD, Schachar R. Dissociation of response inhibition and performance monitoring in the stop signal task using event-related fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp. 2007;28: 1347–1358. pmid:17274022
  83. 83. Heilbronner SR, Hayden BY. Dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex: A Bottom-Up View. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2016;39: 149–170. pmid:27090954
  84. 84. Vogt BA. Midcingulate cortex: Structure, connections, homologies, functions and diseases. J Chem Neuroanat. 2016;74: 28–46. pmid:26993424
  85. 85. Asada H, Fukuda Y, Tsunoda S, Yamaguchi M, Tonoike M. Frontal midline theta rhythms reflect alternative activation of prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex in humans. Neurosci Lett. 1999; 4. pmid:10530512
  86. 86. Kondo H, Osaka N, Osaka M. Cooperation of the anterior cingulate cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex for attention shifting. NeuroImage. 2004;23: 670–679. pmid:15488417
  87. 87. Loh KK, Hadj-Bouziane F, Petrides M, Procyk E, Amiez C. Rostro-Caudal Organization of Connectivity between Cingulate Motor Areas and Lateral Frontal Regions. Front Neurosci. 2018;11. pmid:29375293
  88. 88. Weissman DH, Gopalakrishnan A, Hazlett CJ, Woldorff MG. Dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex Resolves Conflict from Distracting Stimuli by Boosting Attention toward Relevant Events. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15: 229–237. pmid:15238434
  89. 89. Sauseng P, Hoppe J, Klimesch W, Gerloff C, Hummel FC. Dissociation of sustained attention from central executive functions: local activity and interregional connectivity in the theta range. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25: 587–593. pmid:17284201
  90. 90. Woodcock EA, White R, Diwadkar VA. The dorsal prefrontal and dorsal anterior cingulate cortices exert complementary network signatures during encoding and retrieval in associative memory. Behav Brain Res. 2015;290: 152–160. pmid:25960314
  91. 91. van Veen V, Cohen JD, Botvinick MM, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Anterior Cingulate Cortex, Conflict Monitoring, and Levels of Processing. NeuroImage. 2001;14: 1302–1308. pmid:11707086
  92. 92. Hanslmayr S, Pastotter B, Bauml K-H, Gruber S, Wimber M, Klimesch W. The Electrophysiological Dynamics of Interference during the Stroop Task. J Cogn Neurosci. 2008;20: 11. pmid:18275330
  93. 93. Töllner T, Wang Y, Makeig S, Müller HJ, Jung T-P, Gramann K. Two Independent Frontal Midline Theta Oscillations during Conflict Detection and Adaptation in a Simon-Type Manual Reaching Task. J Neurosci. 2017;37: 2504–2515. pmid:28137968
  94. 94. Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, Cohen JD. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol Rev. 2001;108: 624–652. pmid:11488380
  95. 95. Kim C, Chung C, Kim J. Task-dependent response conflict monitoring and cognitive control in anterior cingulate and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices. Brain Res. 2013;1537: 216–223. pmid:24012877
  96. 96. Cohen MX. Error-related medial frontal theta activity predicts cingulate-related structural connectivity. NeuroImage. 2011;55: 1373–1383. pmid:21195774
  97. 97. Sharp DJ, Bonnelle V, De Boissezon X, Beckmann CF, James SG, Patel MC, et al. Distinct frontal systems for response inhibition, attentional capture, and error processing. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2010;107: 6106–6111. pmid:20220100
  98. 98. Braak H, Ghebremedhin E, Rüb U, Bratzke H, Del Tredici K. Stages in the development of Parkinson’s disease-related pathology. Cell Tissue Res. 2004;318: 121–134. pmid:15338272
  99. 99. Vogt BA. Cingulate cortex in Parkinson’s disease. Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Elsevier; 2019. pp. 253–266.
  100. 100. Blesa J, Trigo-Damas I, Dileone M, del Rey L-G, Hernandez LF, Obeso JA. Compensatory mechanisms in Parkinson’s disease: Circuits adaptations and role in disease modification. Exp Neurol. 2017;298: 148–161. pmid:28987461
  101. 101. Swann N, Tandon N, Canolty R, Ellmore TM, McEvoy LK, Dreyer S, et al. Intracranial EEG Reveals a Time- and Frequency-Specific Role for the Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus and Primary Motor Cortex in Stopping Initiated Responses. J Neurosci. 2009;29: 12675–12685. pmid:19812342
  102. 102. Swann NC, Cai W, Conner CR, Pieters TA, Claffey MP, George JS, et al. Roles for the pre-supplementary motor area and the right inferior frontal gyrus in stopping action: Electrophysiological responses and functional and structural connectivity. NeuroImage. 2012;59: 2860–2870. pmid:21979383
  103. 103. Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18: 177–185. pmid:24440116
  104. 104. Obeso I, Wilkinson L, Teo JT, Talelli P, Rothwell JC, Jahanshahi M. Theta burst magnetic stimulation over the pre-supplementary motor area improves motor inhibition. Brain Stimulat. 2017;10: 944–951. pmid:28624346
  105. 105. Duann J-R, Ide JS, Luo X, Li C -s. R. Functional Connectivity Delineates Distinct Roles of the Inferior Frontal Cortex and Presupplementary Motor Area in Stop Signal Inhibition. J Neurosci. 2009;29: 10171–10179. pmid:19675251
  106. 106. Hampshire A, Chamberlain SR, Monti MM, Duncan J, Owen AM. The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: inhibition and attentional control. NeuroImage. 2010;50: 1313–1319. pmid:20056157
  107. 107. Tabu H, Mima T, Aso T, Takahashi R, Fukuyama H. Functional relevance of pre-supplementary motor areas for the choice to stop during Stop signal task. Neurosci Res. 2011;70: 277–284. pmid:21440014
  108. 108. Obeso I, Robles N, Muñoz-Marrón E, Redolar-Ripoll D. Dissociating the Role of the pre-SMA in Response Inhibition and Switching: A Combined Online and Offline TMS Approach. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7. pmid:23616761
  109. 109. Nachev P, Kennard C, Husain M. Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9: 856–869. pmid:18843271
  110. 110. Forstmann BU, Dutilh G, Brown S, Neumann J, von Cramon DY, Ridderinkhof KR, et al. Striatum and pre-SMA facilitate decision-making under time pressure. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2008;105: 17538–17542. pmid:18981414
  111. 111. Chao HH, Luo X, Chang JL, Li CR. Activation of the pre-supplementary motor area but not inferior prefrontal cortex in association with short stop signal reaction time–an intra-subject analysis. BMC Neurosci. 2009;10: 75. pmid:19602259
  112. 112. Jacobs JV, Lou JS, Kraakevik JA, Horak FB. The supplementary motor area contributes to the timing of the anticipatory postural adjustment during step initiation in participants with and without Parkinson’s disease. Neuroscience. 2009;164: 877–885. pmid:19665521
  113. 113. Jahanshahi M, Jenkins H, Brown R, Marsden D, Passingham R, Brooks D. Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. I. An investigation using measurement of regional cerebral blood flow with PET and movement-related potentials in normal and Parkinson’s disease subjects. 1995;119: 1045–1046. pmid:7655888
  114. 114. Wu T, Wang L, Hallett M, Chen Y, Li K, Chan P. Effective connectivity of brain networks during self-initiated movement in Parkinson’s disease. NeuroImage. 2011;55: 204–215. pmid:21126588
  115. 115. Mak M, Cheung V, Wang D, Wong C, Lu Z, Shi L, et al. Difference between brain activations for self- and cue-initiated movements in people with Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2015;30: S17–S18.
  116. 116. Varela F, Lachaux J-P, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J. The brainweb: Phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2: 229–239. pmid:11283746
  117. 117. Kirschfeld K. The physical basis of alpha waves in the electroencephalogram and the origin of the?Berger effect? Biol Cybern. 2005;92: 177–185. pmid:15739111
  118. 118. Engel AK, Fries P. Beta-band oscillations—signalling the status quo? Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2010;20: 156–165. pmid:20359884
  119. 119. Tallon-Baudry C, Bertrand O. Oscillatory gamma activity in humans and its role in object representation. Trends Cogn Sci. 1999;3: 12. pmid:10322469
  120. 120. Jensen O, Kaiser J, Lachaux J-P. Human gamma-frequency oscillations associated with attention and memory. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30: 317–324. pmid:17499860
  121. 121. Harmony T, Alba A, Marroquín JL, González-Frankenberger B. Time-frequency-topographic analysis of induced power and synchrony of EEG signals during a Go/No-Go task. Int J Psychophysiol. 2009;71: 9–16. pmid:18804495
  122. 122. Pal G, O’Keefe J, Robertson-Dick E, Bernard B, Anderson S, Hall D. Global cognitive function and processing speed are associated with gait and balance dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease. J NeuroEngineering Rehabil. 2016;13: 94. pmid:27793167
  123. 123. Shibata T, Shimoyama I, Ito T, Abla D, Iwasa H, Koseki K, et al. Event-related dynamics of the gamma-band oscillation in the human brain: information processing during a GO/NOGO hand movement task. Neurosci Res. 1999;33: 215–222. pmid:10211765
  124. 124. Iijima M, Mase R, Osawa M, Shimizu S, Uchiyama S. Event-Related Synchronization and Desynchronization of High-Frequency Electroencephalographic Activity during a Visual Go/No-Go Paradigm. Neuropsychobiology. 2015;71: 17–24. pmid:25766641
  125. 125. Corbetta M. Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: Identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems? Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1998;95: 831–838. pmid:9448248
  126. 126. Jokinen P, Karrasch M, Brück A, Johansson J, Bergman J, Rinne JO. Cognitive slowing in Parkinson’s disease is related to frontostriatal dopaminergic dysfunction. J Neurol Sci. 2013;329: 23–28. pmid:23561982
  127. 127. Vriend C, van Balkom TD, van Druningen C, Klein M, van der Werf YD, Berendse HW, et al. Processing speed is related to striatal dopamine transporter availability in Parkinson’s disease. NeuroImage Clin. 2020;26: 102257. pmid:32344372
  128. 128. Sawamoto N, Honda M, Hanakawa T, Aso T, Inoue M, Toyoda H, et al. Cognitive slowing in Parkinson disease is accompanied by hypofunctioning of the striatum. Neurology. 2007;68: 1062–1068. pmid:17389313
  129. 129. Fischer P, Pogosyan A, Herz DM, Cheeran B, Green AL, Fitzgerald J, et al. Subthalamic nucleus gamma activity increases not only during movement but also during movement inhibition. eLife. 2017;6. pmid:28742498
  130. 130. Ray NJ, Brittain J-S, Holland P, Joundi RA, Stein JF, Aziz TZ, et al. The role of the subthalamic nucleus in response inhibition: Evidence from local field potential recordings in the human subthalamic nucleus. NeuroImage. 2012;60: 271–278. pmid:22209815
  131. 131. Roux F, Wibral M, Singer W, Aru J, Uhlhaas PJ. The Phase of Thalamic Alpha Activity Modulates Cortical Gamma-Band Activity: Evidence from Resting-State MEG Recordings. J Neurosci. 2013;33: 17827–17835. pmid:24198372
  132. 132. Yoo HB, de la Concha EO, Ridder DD, Pickut BA, Vanneste S. The Functional Alterations in Top-Down Attention Streams of Parkinson’s disease Measured by EEG. Sci Rep. 2018;8: 1–11. pmid:29311619
  133. 133. Meziane HB, Moisello C, Perfetti B, Kvint S, Isaias IU, Quartarone A, et al. Movement Preparation and Bilateral Modulation of Beta Activity in Aging and Parkinson’s Disease. Di Russo F, editor. PLOS ONE. 2015;10: e0114817. pmid:25635777
  134. 134. Yun JY, Jeon B. Betaband event-related desynchronization prior to simple lower limb movement and simulated gait initiation in Parkinson’s disease patient: Magnetoencephalography study. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2018;46: e77.
  135. 135. Heideman SG, Quinn AJ, Woolrich MW, van Ede F, Nobre AC. Dissecting beta-state changes during timed movement preparation in Parkinson’s disease. Prog Neurobiol. 2020;184: 101731. pmid:31778771
  136. 136. Praamstra P, Pope P. Slow Brain Potential and Oscillatory EEG Manifestations of Impaired Temporal Preparation in Parkinson’s Disease. J Neurophysiol. 2007;98: 2848–2857. pmid:17728390
  137. 137. de Jong R, Gladwin TE, ‘t Hart BM. Movement-related EEG indices of preparation in task switching and motor control. Brain Res. 2006;1105: 73–82. pmid:16630582
  138. 138. Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Hanslmayr S. EEG alpha oscillations: The inhibition–timing hypothesis. Brain Res Rev. 2007;53: 63–88. pmid:16887192
  139. 139. Perfetti B, Moisello C, Lanzafame S, Varanese S, Onofrj M, Rocco AD, et al. Attention modulation regulates both motor and non-motor performance: a high-density EEG study in Parkinson’s disease. 2011; 15.
  140. 140. Thut G, Nietzel A, Brandt SA, Pascual-Leone A. α-Band Electroencephalographic Activity over Occipital Cortex Indexes Visuospatial Attention Bias and Predicts Visual Target Detection. J Neurosci. 2006;26: 9494–9502. pmid:16971533
  141. 141. Kamei S, Morita A, Serizawa K, Mizutani T, Hirayanagi K. Quantitative EEG Analysis of Executive Dysfunction in Parkinson Disease: J Clin Neurophysiol. 2010;27: 193–197. pmid:20461018
  142. 142. Bernheimer H, Birkmayer W, Hornykiewicz O, Jellinger K, Seitelberger F. Brain dopamine and the syndromes of Parkinson and Huntington Clinical, morphological and neurochemical correlations. J Neurol Sci. 1973;20: 415–455. pmid:4272516
  143. 143. Fearnley JM, Lees AJ. Ageing and Parkinson’s disease: substantia nigra regional selectivity. Brain. 1991;114: 2283–2301. pmid:1933245
  144. 144. Buhmann C, Binkofski F, Klein C, Büchel C, van Eimeren T, Erdmann C, et al. Motor reorganization in asymptomatic carriers of a single mutant Parkin allele: a human model for presymptomatic parkinsonism. Brain. 2005;128: 2281–2290. pmid:15947065
  145. 145. van Nuenen BFL, van Eimeren T, van der Vegt JPM, Buhmann C, Klein C, Bloem BR, et al. Mapping preclinical compensation in Parkinson’s disease: An imaging genomics approach. Mov Disord. 2009;24: S703–S710. pmid:19877238
  146. 146. Georgiades MJ, Shine JM, Gilat M, McMaster J, Owler B, Mahant N, et al. Hitting the brakes: pathological subthalamic nucleus activity in Parkinson’s disease gait freezing. Brain. 2019;142: 3906–3916. pmid:31665229
  147. 147. Fischer P, Chen CC, Chang Y-J, Yeh C-H, Pogosyan A, Herz DM, et al. Alternating Modulation of Subthalamic Nucleus Beta Oscillations during Stepping. J Neurosci. 2018;38: 5111–5121. pmid:29760182
  148. 148. Klostermann F, Nikulin VV, Kühn AA, Marzinzik F, Wahl M, Pogosyan A, et al. Task-related differential dynamics of EEG alpha- and beta-band synchronization in cortico-basal motor structures. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25: 1604–1615. pmid:17425586
  149. 149. Appel-Cresswell S, de la Fuente-Fernandez R, Galley S, McKeown MJ. Imaging of compensatory mechanisms in Parkinsonʼs disease: Curr Opin Neurol. 2010;23: 407–412. pmid:20610991
  150. 150. Martinu K, Monchi O. Cortico-basal ganglia and cortico-cerebellar circuits in Parkinson’s disease: Pathophysiology or compensation? Behav Neurosci. 2013;127: 222–236. pmid:23244290
  151. 151. Anninos P, Adamopoulos A, Kotini A, Tsagas N, Tamiolakis D, Prassopoulos P. MEG evaluation of Parkinson’s diseased patients after external magnetic stimulation. Acta Neurol Belg. 2007;107: 5-. pmid:17569226
  152. 152. Chen KS, Chen R. Invasive and Noninvasive Brain Stimulation in Parkinson’s Disease: Clinical Effects and Future Perspectives. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 2019;106: 763–775. pmid:31179534
  153. 153. Kish SJ, Shannak K, Hornykiewicz O. Uneven Pattern of Dopamine Loss in the Striatum of Patients with Idiopathic Parkinson’s Disease. N Engl J Med. 1988;318: 876–880. pmid:3352672
  154. 154. Morrish PK, Sawle GV, Brooks DJ. Clinical and [18F] dopa PET findings in early Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1995;59: 597–600. pmid:7500096
  155. 155. Denny-Brown D, Yanagisawa N. The role of the basal ganglia in the initiation of movement. Res Publ—Assoc Res Nerv Ment Dis. 1976;55: 115-. pmid:826994
  156. 156. Pelicioni PHS, Lord SR, Okubo Y, Sturnieks DL, Menant JC. People With Parkinson’s Disease Exhibit Reduced Cognitive and Motor Cortical Activity When Undertaking Complex Stepping Tasks Requiring Inhibitory Control. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2020;34: 1088–1098. pmid:33155508
  157. 157. Nguyen HM, Aravindakshan A, Ross JM, Disbrow EA. Time course of cognitive training in Parkinson disease. NeuroRehabilitation. 2020;46: 311–320. pmid:32250326
  158. 158. Disbrow EA, Russo KA, Higginson CI, Yund EW, Ventura MI, Zhang L, et al. Efficacy of tailored computer-based neurorehabilitation for improvement of movement initiation in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Res. 2012;1452: 151–164. pmid:22459048
  159. 159. Okada Y. Neurogenesis of evoked magnetic fields. Williamson S, Romani G, Kaufman L, Modena I, editors. Biomagn Interdiscip Approach. 1983; 399–408.
  160. 160. Singh SP. Magnetoencephalography: Basic principles. Ann Indian Acad Neurol. 2014;17: S107–S112. pmid:24791076
  161. 161. Mink JW. The basal ganglia: Focused selection and inhibition of competing motor programs. Prog Neurobiol. 1996;50: 381–425. pmid:9004351
  162. 162. Beste C, Willemssen R, Saft C, Falkenstein M. Response inhibition subprocesses and dopaminergic pathways: Basal ganglia disease effects. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48: 366–373. pmid:19782093
  163. 163. Shook SK, Franz EA, Higginson CI, Wheelock VL, Sigvardt KA. Dopamine dependency of cognitive switching and response repetition effects in Parkinson’s patients. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43: 1990–1999. pmid:16243049
  164. 164. Martine van Schouwenburg Esther Aarts, Cools Roshan. Dopaminergic Modulation of Cognitive Control: Distinct Roles for the Prefrontal Cortex and the Basal Ganglia. Curr Pharm Des. 2010;16: 2026–2032. pmid:20370667
  165. 165. Manza P, Amandola M, Tatineni V, Li CR, Leung H-C. Response inhibition in Parkinson’s disease: a meta-analysis of dopaminergic medication and disease duration effects. Npj Park Dis. 2017;3. pmid:28702504