Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Dynamic cortical participation during bilateral, cyclical ankle movements: Effects of Parkinson’s disease

  • Takashi Yoshida,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Rehabilitation Engineering Laboratory, Toronto Rehabilitation Institute, University Health Network, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, Institute of Biomaterials and Biomedical Engineering, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, Applied Surgical and Rehabilitation Technology Lab, Department of Trauma Surgery, Orthopaedics and Plastic Surgery, University Medical Center Göttingen, Göttingen, Lower Saxony, Germany

  • Kei Masani ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    k.masani@utoronto.ca

    Affiliations Rehabilitation Engineering Laboratory, Toronto Rehabilitation Institute, University Health Network, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, Institute of Biomaterials and Biomedical Engineering, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada

  • Karl Zabjek,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Physical Therapy, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada

  • Milos R. Popovic,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Rehabilitation Engineering Laboratory, Toronto Rehabilitation Institute, University Health Network, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, Institute of Biomaterials and Biomedical Engineering, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada

  • Robert Chen

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Division of Neurology, Department of Medicine, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, Krembil Research Institute, University Health Network, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, Edmond J. Safra Program in Parkinson’s Disease, University Health Network, Toronto, Ontario, Canada

Abstract

Parkinson’s disease (PD) is known to increase asymmetry and variability of bilateral movements. However, the mechanisms of such abnormalities are not fully understood. Here, we aimed to investigate whether kinematic abnormalities are related to cortical participation during bilateral, cyclical ankle movements, which required i) maintenance of a specific frequency and ii) bilateral coordination of the lower limbs in an anti-phasic manner. We analyzed electroencephalographic and electromyographic signals from nine men with PD and nine aged-matched healthy men while they sat and cyclically dorsi- and plantarflexed their feet. This movement was performed at a similar cadence to normal walking under two conditions: i) self-paced and ii) externally paced by a metronome. Participants with PD exhibited reduced range of motion and more variable bilateral coordination. However, participants with and without PD did not differ in the magnitude of corticomuscular coherence between the midline cortical areas and tibialis anterior and medial gastrocnemius muscles. This finding suggests that either the kinematic abnormalities were related to processes outside linear corticomuscular communication or PD-related changes in neural correlates maintained corticomuscular communication but not motor performance.

Introduction

Parkinson’s disease (PD) is known to increase the variability and asymmetry of bilateral rhythmical movements, such as walking [18]. Although the hallmark of PD is well established as progressive neuronal degeneration, mechanisms of many specific kinematic abnormalities in PD are not clearly understood.

Corticomuscular coherence in the beta band had been used to suggest that synchronous cortical oscillations were functionally related to muscle activities during sustained contractions about single joints [9,10]. In our previous studies, we demonstrated that the coherence between the primary sensorimotor cortex and the active leg muscles increased cyclically in the beta band (13 to 30 Hz) during bilateral, cyclical ankle movements [11,12]. Because the ankle movements that we adopted had only a few intended functional requirements such as maintaining a specific movement frequency and coordinating the feet in an anti-phasic manner, our findings suggest that the primary sensorimotor cortex may contribute to these requirements via corticomuscular communication [11,12].

In addition to the aforementioned effects on the consistency and symmetry of bilateral movements [18], PD can impair the performance of anti-phasic movements [1319]. Because these kinematic features are fundamental to the aforementioned ankle movements, it is likely that individuals with PD will perform these movements abnormally. Furthermore, if the cyclical corticomuscular coherence during the ankle movements is relevant to specific functional requirements, kinematic abnormalities in PD would be accompanied by corresponding changes in corticomuscular coherence.

Only a few studies have examined how PD affects corticomuscular coherence, and the experimental evidence is limited to sustained contractions of upper limb muscles [20,21]. During sustained isometric extension of the wrist, healthy individuals and individuals with PD on levodopa show similar magnitudes of coherence within the beta band [20]. In the off-medication condition, individuals with PD showed decreased beta coherence [20], suggesting that dopamine deficiency within the basal ganglia impairs corticomuscular synchronization. To our knowledge, no study has examined corticomuscular coherence during bilateral cyclical movements of the lower limbs at a cadence similar to normal walking in PD.

Although the corticospinal connection may be normal in PD, indicated by the preservation of central motor conduction time [22,23], PD may still affect corticomuscular communication. This is suggested by the anatomical and physiological relationship between the basal ganglia and motor cortex. The basal ganglia, which is affected by degeneration of dopaminergic neurons in PD [2426], is reciprocally connected with the motor cortices [2730]. In rat models of PD, the motor cortex is coherent with the substantia nigra pars reticulata during treadmill walking [31] and the subthalamic nucleus during sustained exploratory movement [32]. Furthermore, such cortico-basal ganglia coherence is abolished by L-dopa or dopamine receptor agonist [31,32] and restored by dopamine D2 receptor antagonist [31], suggesting that the coherence is pathological. Similar cortico-basal ganglia coherence and its dopamine dependence have been observed in individuals with PD during rest or tonic wrist extension [33,34]. If the basal ganglia and motor cortex interact abnormally during the ankle movements, then it is possible that changes in cortical activities may alter corticomuscular communication though the exact effects are uncertain.

Here, we aimed to investigate the mechanism of kinematic abnormalities in PD by examining the corticomuscular communication between the midline cortical areas and the active muscles during bilateral, cyclical ankle movements at a cadence similar to normal walking. To quantify the corticomuscular communication, we calculated the coherence between electroencephalographic (EEG) and electromyographic (EMG) signals. The experimental tasks were performed under two conditions (self-paced and externally-paced by a metronome) as rhythmic aural pacing, at or slightly faster than the preferred cadence, can acutely reduce movement variability in individuals with PD [4], and such change in motor performance may be accompanied by changes in corticomuscular coherence.

Based on the existing evidence, we hypothesized that, compared to healthy individuals, the magnitude of coherence between contracting muscles and the midline primary sensorimotor cortex within the beta band would be lower in individuals with PD. Participants were assessed in the off-medication condition because dopamine has been shown to restore corticomuscular coherence [20] and normalize the interaction between the basal ganglia and motor cortex [31,32].

Materials and methods

Participants

We recruited ten men with PD and eleven aged-matched healthy men. The same sample of healthy men was reported in our previous study [12]. One participant with PD and two healthy participants were excluded from data analysis because of excessive EEG artifacts. The remaining nine participants with PD were 62±7 years old and healthy participants were 66±7 years old (mean±standard deviation). The two groups of participants did not differ significantly in age (p = .227, unpaired t-test). The clinical details of the participants with PD are summarized in Table 1.

All participants were able to walk unassisted and had no history of dementia. Participants with PD had been diagnosed with idiopathic PD, and their disease duration was 10.1±5.5 years (ranging from 3 to 20 years). All participants provided their written informed consent. The experimental protocol (12–5462) was approved by the University Health Network Research Ethics Board (Toronto, Ontario, Canada) and carried out in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations.

Motor and cognitive examination of participants with Parkinson’s Disease

All participants with PD were being treated with levodopa and were studied in the off-medication condition following overnight withdrawal from dopaminergic medications. We administered the motor section (Part III) of the UPDRS before the experimental task. After the experimental task, we administered the MoCA (Version 7.1).

On a separate day before the experiment, we administered the Gait and Falls Questionnaire [35] to four of the nine participants with PD that reported freezing of gait. The questionnaire quantified the severity of freezing and identified possible triggers of episodes.

Experimental task

Each participant sat in a chair with a backrest and performed six runs of bilateral, cyclical ankle movements. The six runs alternated between being self-paced and externally paced by the sound of a metronome, with the first run always being externally paced (i.e., for each type of pacing, there were three runs). Each run lasted approximately one minute and was followed by a rest of approximately one minute.

Participants were instructed to maximally dorsiflex one foot and maximally plantarflex the other foot at each beat of the metronome (in an anti-phasic manner) without flexing or extending their toes. The metronome was set to 108 beats per minute (1.8 Hz), comparable to the cadence of normal overground walking [36]. For self-paced movements, the participants were instructed to replicate the rhythm of the metronome. The passive movements that resulted from the ankle movements (e.g., an upward movement of the knee as the foot dorsiflexed) were not constrained. Because the participants sat with their heels on an elevated footrest, the soles of their feet largely did not come into contact with any surface during the movement. Because the experimental task was performed with no resistance and supported heels, we assumed that the strength of contraction was relatively low with minimal effects of amplitude cancellation [37]. They were also instructed to focus their gaze on a bullseye, which was placed approximately 2 m in front of them in their line of sight as they sat upright and gazed forward. To minimize EEG artifacts, the participants were instructed to relax their upper body and to refrain from talking, swallowing, coughing, clenching their jaw, or blinking excessively. While the participants performed the movement, their EEG signals, EMG signals, and body kinematics were recorded.

Data collection

All signals were recorded in epochs of approximately one minute, which began several cycles after the movement had been initiated and preceded the termination of the movement. The sampling of all signals was synchronized by an analogic switch, which sent a transistor-transistor logic signal that initiated the recording of kinematic data and EMG signals and timestamped the EEG signals.

To track the ankle movements, we used an optical motion capture system: a data acquisition device (MX Giganet, Vicon Motion Systems Ltd., United Kingdom), nine optical cameras (Bonita, Vicon Motion Systems Ltd., United Kingdom), and data acquisition software (Nexus 1.8.5, Vicon Motion Systems Ltd., United Kingdom). We placed 14-mm retroreflective markers over the EEG electrode locations, AF7 and AF8, and over the following bony landmarks: greater trochanter, lateral epicondyle of the femur, lateral malleolus and second metatarsal head on both sides. The instantaneous positions of the markers were recorded at 100 Hz.

EMG signals were recorded using a wireless EMG system (Trigno Wireless EMG System, Delsys Inc., United States). Each EMG sensor used 99.9%-silver electrodes, which were 1 mm in diameter and 5 mm in length. The electrodes were in bipolar configuration with inter-electrode spacing of 10 mm. The EMG sensors were placed bilaterally over the belly of the tibialis anterior muscle and the medial head of the gastrocnemius muscle. EMG signals were sampled at 2 kHz with a bandwidth of 20 Hz to 450 Hz and the common mode rejection ratio of over 80 dB.

EEG signals were recorded using an active electrode system (g.GAMMAsys, g.tec medical engineering GmbH, Austria) with compatible signal amplifiers (g.USBamp, g.tec medical engineering GmbH, Austria) and recording software (g.Recorder, g.tec medical engineering GmbH, Austria). According to the 10–10 system [38], we recorded from AFz, Fz, F1, F2, F3, F4, FCz, FC1, FC2, FC3, FC4, Cz, C1, C2, C3, C4, CPz, CP1, CP2, and Pz, which covered the midline primary sensorimotor cortex and its surrounding. EEG signals were sampled at 1.2 kHz using a monopolar montage with the reference electrode on the left ear lobe and the ground electrode over the right zygomatic process.

Data analysis

Data analysis was performed offline using MATLAB R2016b (The MathWorks, Inc., United States). We quantified motor performance as the intra-individual mean and coefficient of variation of the following parameters: movement cycle duration, range of motion at the ankle, and the phase offset between the two feet. These parameters were selected to quantify the consistency and symmetry of the ankle movements. A movement cycle was defined such that dorsiflexion of the right foot was maximal at 0 and 100% of the cycle. The ankle angle was calculated between two lines: one line joining the markers over the lateral epicondyle of the femur and the lateral malleolus and another line joining the markers over the lateral malleolus and the second metatarsal head. The phase offset was calculated for angular velocities of the two ankles [16], such that the offset would be 180° for a symmetrically coordinated movement. For each participant, intra-individual mean and coefficient of variation of the above parameters were calculated across the minimum number of movement cycles that were completed among all participants after three epochs.

To assess the effects of head movements on EEG signals, we calculated the continuous wavelet transforms of the cyclical EEG signals at Cz using the complex Morlet wavelet. We also calculated the cyclical linear movements of the markers at the EEG electrode locations, AF7 and AF8 to assess the magnitude of head movements during the ankle movements.

For each epoch, EMG signals were centered by subtracting its mean and full-wave rectified to enhance the spectral power at the frequency of common input to the activated muscles [39,40]. To assess the effects of rectification, we estimated the power spectral densities of the cyclical EMG signals using Welch’s method. Cyclical EMG signals were down-sampled at 400 Hz and divided into eight sections of equal length with Hamming windows and 50% overlap. For each epoch, EEG signals were filtered by i) a second-order infinite impulse response notch filter, with a center frequency of 60 Hz and bandwidth of 1 Hz, and ii) a fourth-order Butterworth infinite impulse response filter, between 0.5 Hz and 100 Hz. For both processes, zero-phase digital filtering was used. The filtered EEG signals were decomposed by independent component analysis [41,42]. The resultant components and filtered EEG signals were examined for artifacts visually [43]. The contributions of components that contained artifactual waveforms were subtracted from the filtered EEG signals to generate noise-reduced EEG signals. The subtraction was restricted to the observed duration of artifactual waveforms to minimize the loss of information.

The noise-reduced EEG signals and rectified EMG signals were down-sampled at 400 Hz, and their coherence was calculated for each epoch using the complex Morlet wavelet: where j is the imaginary unit, Fb is a bandwidth parameter, and Fc is the center frequency of the wavelet in Hz. The bandwidth parameter was set to 10, and the center frequency was set to 1.

Corticomuscular coherence was calculated as three-dimensional data across frequency and time. For each participant, the corticomuscular coherence (approximately one-minute long) was segmented into individual movement cycles, and the segments were used to calculate an ensemble average. Each ensemble average was calculated with the minimum number of cycles that were completed among all participants after three epochs.

The significance of each ensemble average was determined by a threshold value [10]: where α is the confidence level in percent, L is the number of segments that are used to calculate the ensemble average, and N is the number of data points (across frequency and time) in the ensemble average. The confidence level was set to 95%. Before applying the threshold, the ensemble average was binned across frequency and time, resulting in one pixel per Hz (between 1 and 100 Hz) and per percent of the movement cycle. The magnitude of corticomuscular coherence was calculated as the volume of significant coherence: the magnitude of coherence above the threshold at each pixel, integrated over the frequency-time plane of the ensemble average. A similar method has been used by Kilner et al. to quantify corticomuscular coherence [44]. For the volume of significant coherence, we also calculated its center frequency as the geometric centroid along frequency.

The cyclical patterns of corticomuscular coherence at Cz were validated using surrogate coherence. For each participant, an ensemble average of coherence was calculated by pairing the ith-cycle segment of the EEG signal with the jth-cycle segment of an EMG signal, such that ij. For each participant, such ensemble averages were calculated 100 times with differently permutated pairing of EEG and EMG signals, and the average magnitude of the 100 ensemble averages was used as surrogate coherence. Patterns of coherence, which were present in experimental coherence but abolished in surrogate coherence, were considered valid as these patterns indicate the synchronization between EEG and EMG signals that does not relate to the mere power of the signals. We chose this approach to eliminate only the cyclical pairing between the EEG and EMG signals. The validation was only performed at Cz because it was the most relevant electrode location (i.e., over the midline primary sensorimotor cortex).

For the intra-individual mean and coefficient of variation of cycle duration and range of motion, we performed 3-way ANOVA with the i) presence of PD (present or absent), ii) type of pacing (self- or external pacing), and iii) side of body (left or right) as factors. For the intra-individual mean and coefficient of variation of the phase offset, we performed 2-way mixed-design ANOVA with the i) presence of PD as a between-subject factor and ii) type of pacing as a within-subject factor. To eliminate redundancy, we only analyzed the phase offset that was calculated with the right ankle as the leading side.

On the volume and center frequency of significant coherence at Cz, we performed 4-way ANOVA with the i) presence of PD, ii) type of pacing, iii) side of body, and iv) muscle (tibialis anterior or medial gastrocnemius muscles) as factors. To examine the cortical distribution of coherence, we performed 5-way ANOVA on the volume of significant coherence with the i) presence of PD, ii) type of pacing, iii) side of body, iv) muscle, and v) EEG electrode location as factors.

We examined how surrogate and experimental coherence at Cz differed in magnitude by performing 2-way ANOVA with i) the presence of PD and ii) type of coherence (experimental or surrogate) as factors. Preliminarily, we had observed that surrogate coherence showed relatively high magnitudes of coherence below 6 Hz. Thus, the 2-way ANOVA was performed separately above and below 6 Hz.

For significant main effects, we performed post hoc multiple comparison tests with Tukey’s honestly significant difference criterion. The significance level was set to 5% for all tests. We also performed multiple-sample tests for equal variances (Bartlett’s test) and Royston’s multivariate normality tests [45] on the data for ANOVA.

Results

Motor and cognitive examination of participants with Parkinson’s Disease

For participants with PD, 11.9±1.7 hours had elapsed since their last dose. The motor scores of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) were 17.6±6.6 (out of 108), with a higher value indicating greater motor impairment. The leg agility subscores were 1.2±0.9 (out of 4) for the right and 1.3±0.9 for the left.

The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) scores were 26.9±1.83 (out of 30), with a lower value indicating greater cognitive impairment. The scores for the Freezing of Gait Questionnaire, which is a subset of the Gait and Falls Questionnaire [35], were 9.0±1.6 (out of 24), with a higher score indicating greater severity of freezing. Of the four participants with PD that reported freezing of gait, three reported start hesitation and one reported freezing while walking straight.

Motor performance of ankle movements

For each type of pacing, healthy participants completed 167±8 cycles and participants with PD completed 166±21 cycles after three one-minute runs. Among all participants, the minimum number of completed cycles was 111. The inter-run rest was 80.6±30.7 seconds for healthy participants and 116±23 seconds for participants with PD.

Fig 1 compares the motor performance of the two groups during the ankle movements. The intra-individual mean of cycle duration was significantly shorter for participants with PD (F1,65 = 4.89, p = .0305) but was not significantly affected by the type of pacing (F1,65 = 1.35, p = .250) or side of body (F1,65 < 0.001, p = .994). The coefficient of variation of cycle duration was not significantly affected by the presence of PD (F1,65 = 2.73, p = .103), type of pacing (F1,65 = 2.78, p = .100), or side of body (F1,65 = 1.42, p = .237). The intra-individual mean of the range of motion was significantly smaller for participants with PD (F1,65 = 31.4, p < .001) but was not significantly affected by the type of pacing (F1,65 = 0.270, p = .605) or side of body (F1,65 = 0.295, p = .589). The coefficient of variation of the range of motion was significantly more variable for participants with PD (F1,65 = 41.4, p < .001) but was not significantly affected by the type of pacing (F1,65 = 1.68, p = .199) or side of body (F1,65 = 2.03, p = .159). The intra-individual mean of the phase offset was not significantly affected by the presence of PD (F1,32 = 0.295, p = .591) or type of pacing (F1,32 = 0.784, p = .383). The coefficient of variation of the phase offset was significantly more variable for participants with PD (F1,32 = 7.67, p = .00928) but not significantly affected by the type of pacing (F1,32 = 0.340, p = .564). According to multiple-sample tests for equal variances and Royston’s multivariate normality tests, the mean cycle duration was neither homogeneous (T = 149.37, p < .001) nor normal (H = 34.53, p < .001), the coefficient of variation of cycle duration was homogeneous (T = 5.28, p = .626) and normal (H = 5.15, p = .639), the mean range of motion was homogeneous (T = 2.36, p = .937) and normal (H = 3.83, p = .320), the coefficient of variation of the range of motion was not homogeneous (T = 14.20, p = .048) but normal (H = 3.78, p = .786), the mean phase offset was homogeneous (T = 6.03, p = .110) and normal (H = 2.85, p = .524), and the coefficient of variation of the phase offset was homogeneous (T = 1.59, p = .663) and normal (H = 2.14, p = .694).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Intra-individual mean and coefficient of variation of motor performance parameters for self-paced and externally-paced movements.

Error bars indicate inter-individual standard deviations.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0196177.g001

Among the factors of analysis of variance (ANOVA) on the parameters of motor performance, the only significant interaction was between the presence of PD and type of pacing on the intra-individual mean of cycle duration (F1,65 = 4.59, p = .0360), indicating that cycle durations of healthy participants were more affected by the type of pacing.

For both participant groups, regardless of the type of pacing, markers at AF7 and AF8 were within a volume of approximately 1 cm3 during each movement cycle. For healthy participants, the average linear head movement was less than 8 mm, 7 mm, and 4 mm, in the anteroposterior, mediolateral, and longitudinal directions, respectively. The equivalent measures for participants with PD were less than 5 mm, 4 mm, and 2 mm.

Cyclical corticomuscular coherence during ankle movements

Fig 2 shows the full-wave rectified EMG signals from representative participants during self-paced movements. Both participants showed cyclical increase in the activation of the tibialis anterior muscles during dorsiflexion and relatively weak activation of the medial gastrocnemius muscles. These observations were also true for externally-paced movements (Fig 3). The continuous wavelet transforms of EEG signals at Cz did not show observable peaks at harmonics of the movement frequency (1.8 Hz) above 5 Hz (Fig 4). Fig 5 shows how the full-wave rectification modulated the estimated power spectral densities of EMG signals, and Fig 6 shows the corresponding change in the pattern of corticomuscular coherence around 20 Hz.

thumbnail
Fig 2. EEG, kinematic, and EMG signals of representative participants during self-paced ankle movements.

Noise-reduced EEG signal from Cz (EEGCz), ankle angle (θAnkle), and full-wave rectified EMG signals from the tibialis anterior (EMGTA) and medial gastrocnemius (EMGMG) muscles are shown. Ankle angles have been centered and normalized to its range.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0196177.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. EEG, kinematic, and EMG signals of representative participants during externally-paced ankle movements.

Noise-reduced EEG signal from Cz (EEGCz), ankle angle (θAnkle), and full-wave rectified EMG signals from the tibialis anterior (EMGTA) and medial gastrocnemius (EMGMG) muscles are shown. Ankle angles have been centered and normalized to its range.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0196177.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. Continuous wavelet transforms of cyclical EEG signals at Cz.

Group averages are shown. The white dotted lines indicate 5 Hz.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0196177.g004

thumbnail
Fig 5. Estimated power spectral densities of EMG signals from the right tibialis anterior (TA) muscle.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0196177.g005

thumbnail
Fig 6. Effects of rectifying EMG signals on cyclical corticomuscular coherence.

(a) Cyclical coherence between Cz and the right tibialis anterior (TA) muscle of a healthy participant during externally-paced movements. (b) Significant portions of the cyclical coherence in (a).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0196177.g006

Fig 7a shows the cyclical coherence between Cz and the tibialis anterior muscles of a participant with PD during externally-paced movements. Within the movement cycle, corticomuscular coherence increased dynamically in the beta band, coinciding with ankle dorsiflexion (cf. Fig 2). Fig 7b shows the group average of cyclical corticomuscular coherence in the beta band. Generally, between Cz and the tibialis anterior muscles, corticomuscular coherence in the beta band increased cyclically during dorsiflexion (cf. Fig 2). Volumes of significant corticomuscular coherence were centered about the beta band (Table 2). The cyclical patterns of coherence were less consistent between Cz and the medial gastrocnemius muscles (Fig 7b).

thumbnail
Fig 7. Cyclical corticomuscular coherence.

(a) Cyclical coherence between Cz and tibialis anterior muscles of a participant with PD during externally-paced movements. (b) Group averages of cyclical corticomuscular coherence in the beta band (13 to 30 Hz). Coherence is calculated between Cz and the tibialis anterior (TA) and medial gastrocnemius (MG) muscles. Solid lines indicate inter-individual mean and dotted lines indicate inter-individual standard deviations.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0196177.g007

thumbnail
Table 2. Volume and center frequency of significant coherence between EEG signal from Cz and EMG signals from the tibialis anterior (TA) and medial gastrocnemius (MG) muscles.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0196177.t002

Table 2 summarizes the volume and center frequency of significant corticomuscular coherence for the two groups. At Cz, the volume of significant coherence was significantly affected by the muscle (F1,133 = 5.12, p = .0253) but not by the presence of PD (F1,133 = 1.31, p = .254), type of pacing (F1,133 = 0.0160, p = .900), or side of body (F1,133 = 0.954, p = .330). Post hoc analysis revealed that the volume of significant coherence between Cz and the tibialis anterior muscles was larger than that between Cz and the medial gastrocnemius muscles. The center frequency was not significantly affected by the presence of PD (F1,133 = 0.0163, p = .899), type of pacing (F1,133 = 0.978, p = .325), muscle (F1,133 = 1.37, p = .244), or side of body (F1,133 = 2.17, p = .143). Neither for the volume nor the center frequency of significant coherence did the factors of ANOVA interact significantly. According to multiple-sample tests for equal variances and Royston’s multivariate normality tests, the magnitude of coherence was neither homogeneous (T = 53.04, p < .001) nor normal (H = 61.68, p < .001). The same tests indicated that the frequency of coherence was homogeneous (T = 14.35, p = .499) but not normal (H = 21.53, p = .029).

Fig 8 shows the cortical distributions of the volume of significant coherence in the beta band between EEG signals and EMG signals from the tibialis anterior muscles. Fig 9 shows the same distributions for the medial gastrocnemius muscles. Generally, the cortical distribution peaked at Cz although this pattern appeared to be more distinct for the tibialis anterior muscles. ANOVA shows that the magnitude of coherence at Cz was significantly larger than the magnitude at all other locations in 89% of the conditions (i.e., combinations between the factors of ANOVA). This was followed by the magnitude at FCz, which was larger than the magnitude at all other locations in 54% of the conditions, and the magnitude at C1, which was larger than the magnitude at locations other than Cz and C2 in 44% of the conditions. The analysis did not show PD-related differences in the cortical distribution of coherence.

thumbnail
Fig 8. Cortical distributions of significant beta-band corticomuscular coherence for the tibialis anterior (TA) muscles.

All bars show the volume of corticomuscular coherence in the units of Hz·%Movement Cycle. Cz is circled. The scale of the vertical axis is the same across electrode locations, and the magnitude of the bar graphs is indicated at CP1. The rostral direction is towards the top of the page.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0196177.g008

thumbnail
Fig 9. Cortical distributions of significant beta-band corticomuscular coherence for the medial gastrocnemius (MG) muscles.

All bars show the volume of corticomuscular coherence in the units of Hz·%Movement Cycle. Cz is circled. The scale of the vertical axis is the same across electrode locations, and the magnitude of the bar graphs is indicated at CP1. The rostral direction is towards the top of the page.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0196177.g009

Validation of experimental corticomuscular coherence

Fig 10 illustrates the validation of experimental coherence using surrogate coherence. Patterns of significant experimental coherence were preserved in surrogate coherence below 6 Hz but were abolished above 6 Hz. Below 6 Hz, the volume of significant coherence was significantly affected by the presence of PD (F1,284 = 31.2, p < .001) and surrogation of coherence (F1,284 = 45.5, p < .001). Post hoc analysis revealed that the volume of significant coherence was larger for healthy participants and for surrogate coherence. Above 6 Hz, the volume of significant coherence was significantly affected by the surrogation of coherence (F1,284 = 171, p < .001) but not by the presence of PD (F1,284 = 1.445, p = .230). Post hoc analysis revealed that the volume of significant coherence was smaller for surrogate coherence.

thumbnail
Fig 10. Validation of experimental corticomuscular coherence.

(a) Significant experimental and surrogate coherence between Cz and the right tibialis anterior muscle of a participant with PD during externally-paced movements. Pixels with significant coherence are shown in black. (b) Volumes of significant coherence between Cz and the tibialis anterior (TA) and medial gastrocnemius (MG) muscles of participants with PD during self-paced movements. Error bars indicate inter-individual standard deviations.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0196177.g010

Below 6 Hz, the presence of PD and type of coherence interacted significantly (F1,284 = 4.61, p = .0326), indicating that the difference in the volume of significant coherence between experimental and surrogate coherence was larger for participants with PD. Above 6 Hz, the interaction between the two factors was not significant (F1,284 = 1.69, p = .194).

Discussion

Participants with PD exhibited several kinematic abnormalities during the ankle movements: faster movement frequencies (especially with self-pacing), reduced and more variable range of motion, and more variable bilateral coordination compared to healthy participants (Fig 1). Similar increase in movement frequency has been reported for self-paced finger tapping at a specified frequency [4650], reduced range of motion has been reported for walking [36] and self-paced finger tapping [51,52], and increased variability has been reported for various parameters of gait in PD [14]. Tests for homogeneity and normality indicated that the observed abnormalities in participants with PD were valid for the reduced range of motion and more variable bilateral coordination. Thus, the consistency and symmetry of the ankle movements differed significantly between participants with and without PD.

The observed kinematic abnormalities may have been caused partially by the requirement for anti-phasic coordination. Compared to healthy individuals, anti-phasic movements in PD can transition spontaneously into in-phasic movements at lower frequencies [15], exhibit greater asymmetry [14,16,17,53], induce freezing [53], or simply fail [18,19]. Although our participants with PD did not exhibit these abnormalities, the existing evidence suggests that individuals with PD experience difficulty with performing anti-phasic movements.

In addition to anti-phasic coordination, participants with PD may have had deficits in learning and automatizing the ankle movements. In monkeys and humans, the basal ganglia appears to participate in learning an unfamiliar motor task [5457] and automatizing its execution [55,5861]. Although the ankle movements were relatively simple, impaired motor learning and task automatization could have contributed to the observed kinematic abnormalities.

In participants with and without PD, coherence between Cz and the tibialis anterior muscles increased cyclically in the beta band during dorsiflexion of the feet (Fig 7b). This increase occurred bilaterally and regardless of the type of pacing (Fig 7b). Furthermore, these patterns of coherence were validated using surrogate coherence. With the shuffled pairing between cycles of EEG and EMG signals, the volume of significant coherence above 6 Hz significantly decreased and became almost negligible (Fig 10). Thus, the patterns of coherence above 6 Hz could be attributed to the cyclical ankle movements. Conversely, the volume of significant coherence below 6 Hz significantly increased with the shuffled pairing (Fig 10). Thus, coherence below 6 Hz was not validated. The patterns of coherence were less distinct between Cz and the gastrocnemius muscles (Fig 7b), possibly due to the relative absence of phasic activation of the gastrocnemius muscles (Fig 2). We observed that full-wave rectification of EMG signals enhanced their power (Fig 5) and the pattern of corticomuscular coherence (Fig 6) around 20 Hz. Similar modulation of the power spectrum by full-wave rectification has been reported previously [62]. Although this frequency was at the lower threshold of the bandwidth of the EMG system, rectification appears to amplify the power at the common motor unit recruitment frequency based on a broad spectrum of the original, unrectified signal. Such behavior has been supported by experimental evidence [40] and computational modeling [37,39].

For the tibialis anterior muscles, the cortical distributions of significant beta corticomuscular coherence generally peaked at Cz (Fig 8). Such somatotopy has been observed for the tibialis anterior muscle during isometric contractions [63]. The cortical distributions were less distinct for the medial gastrocnemius muscles (Fig 9). Again, this may have been due to the muscle being less active than the tibialis anterior muscle.

The minimal influence from movement artifacts was suggested by the absence of peaks in the estimated power spectral densities of cyclical EEG signals at Cz (Fig 4): during tasks that induce substantial electrode movements, artifacts can be present in EEG signals at the movement frequency and its harmonics [64]. The absence of significant artifacts was also indirectly supported by the kinematic data, as the head markers stayed within a space of approximately 1 cm3 during each movement cycle.

Contrary to our hypothesis, the magnitude of corticomuscular coherence did not significantly differ between the two groups (Table 2). As participants with PD exhibited several kinematic abnormalities (Fig 1), the lack of group discrepancy in the magnitude of coherence suggests that the pathological processes, which impaired motor performance, occurred outside linear corticomuscular communication or that changes in neural correlates maintained corticomuscular communication but not motor performance.

Whichever the case, it is likely that pathological processes that affected motor performance involved the basal ganglia, which is implicated in many aspects of motor control [27] and is affected by neuronal degeneration in PD [2426]. Pathological activities within the ganglia may affect motor control via the recipients of the basal ganglia output: the ventral anterior and ventrolateral nuclei of the thalamus, which project back to the motor cortex [27,65,66], or the pedunculopontine nucleus (PPN). Abnormal cortico-basal ganglia interaction has been observed in rat models of PD [31,32] and individuals with PD [33,34] although its exact implications for the ankle movements are unknown. As for the basal ganglia output to the PPN, this may particularly affect the performance of anti-phasic ankle movements. The PPN comprises the mesencephalic locomotor region [6769], which is implicated in initiating and sustaining locomotive actions [7072]. The notion that PD involves pathological oscillations within the PPN, which contribute to the impairment of locomotive actions, is supported by the finding that deep brain stimulation of the PPN improves gait in individuals with PD in the off-medication condition [73,74]. Also, in individuals with PD, brisk ankle movements modulate oscillations in the PPN and cortical-PPN coherence in the beta band [75]. Although the ankle movements in this study were substantially different from locomotion, they share some key functional requirements with locomotion such as the maintenance of rhythm and anti-phasic coordination of the lower limbs. Thus, pathological output from the basal ganglia may have affected the performance of the ankle movements via the PPN and a subsequent pattern generating neuronal circuit at the spinal level [76,77].

Several studies indicated that the neural correlates of cyclical hand movements are affected by PD [13,47,78]. Particularly, during an anti-phasic bimanual task, individuals with PD show greater activation of the primary motor cortex and less activation within the basal ganglia than healthy individuals [13]. Although our experimental task involved lower limbs, the functional requirement (of performing a bilateral, anti-phasic movement) was similar to that of the aforementioned study, as was the disease severity of the participants [13]. Thus, it is possible that our participants with PD also recruited neural correlates that differed from those of healthy participants. However, the exact changes in neural correlates could not be determined without additional studies. Identifying the neural correlates in individuals with and without PD may help delineate how linear corticomuscular communication is maintained during the ankle movements.

Our observations differed from the results of a previous study, which found decreased beta coherence in PD during sustained isometric wrist extension [20]. Such discrepancy may have been due to the difference in the tasks: sustained isometric contraction compared to cyclical movements. Compared to cyclical movements, sustained contractions may require greater conscious control of the level of muscle activation, thus inducing greater participation by the primary sensorimotor cortex. It has also been shown that, between isometric and dynamic concentric plantarflexion with comparable ankle angles and forces, the motor unit discharge rate is significantly higher during dynamic plantarflexion [79], suggesting that the nature of contraction affects how motor units are recruited. If isometric and dynamic contractions differ substantially in how they are controlled, then it is possible that corresponding corticomuscular communication is differentially vulnerable to PD-related changes during isometric and dynamic contractions. The discrepancy between our findings and the previous study [20] may have also been related to the difference between upper and lower limb muscles, with upper limb muscle receiving stronger corticospinal projections [80]. Because of the stronger projections, upper limb muscles may rely more on corticospinal communication during contractions. Assuming that such communication is reflected in corticomuscular coherence, tasks with greater corticospinal communication may be more affected by PD.

Corticomuscular coherence can be affected in diseases other than PD. It has been reported that stroke can significantly decreases the magnitude of beta corticomuscular coherence on the affected side [81] and shift the location of maximum beta corticomuscular coherence away from the expected location: contralateral sensorimotor cortex [82]. Cerebral palsy has been associated with increased magnitude of beta corticomuscular coherence [83]. We did not observe such phenomena in PD. Although our findings differed from those of previous studies, several discrepancies make the comparison difficult. The main discrepancies are in the experimental task and pathophysiology. The studies on stroke used sustained wrist extension or a gripping task with visual feedback of force production [81,82], and the study on cerebral palsy used externally-cued ballistic hand movements [83]. In the stroke studies, only a small percentage of participants (2 of 6 participants or 3 of 25 participants) showed lesions in the basal ganglia [81,82]. In the cerebral palsy study, it is uncertain how much the interaction between the basal ganglia and the sensorimotor cortex is affected.

Despite reported evidence that aural pacing evokes synchronized periodic fields in the primary auditory cortex [84] and that increased attention or effort increases corticomuscular coherence [44,8592], we did not observe any significant effects of aural pacing on the magnitude of coherence.

This study had several limitations. As we used coherence, our analysis focused on the linear aspect of corticomuscular communication. Because of the complex interconnections that the primary motor cortex forms with adjacent cortical areas and subcortical structures [93], the linear aspect alone probably cannot comprehensively capture how PD affects corticomuscular communication. Indeed, non-linear communication is likely if pathological signals are transmitted from the basal ganglia to the spinal cord as speculated above. Recently, Yang et al. have propose a new method to calculate non-linear corticomuscular coherence [94], with which they have found non-linear corticomuscular coherence during isometric wrist extension and attributed it to somatosensory feedback [95]. Such method may be extended to dynamic movements in the future.

With coherence, we also could not infer the directionality of corticomuscular communication. Although the EEG signal from Cz is likely to consist primarily of electrical cortical activities in the midline cortical structures such as the primary motor and sensory cortices and the supplementary motor area, the signal can also contain activities from the adjacent cortical areas through volume conduction. To determine the sources of the signal from Cz, detailed source localization is required. However, as coherence is a linear measure, it seems more likely that coherence exists via the monosynaptic corticospinal connection rather than the polysynaptic connections for somatosensory feedback.

This study was also limited by the absence of freezing episodes among participants with PD. Such episodes could have been accompanied by observable discrepancies in corticomuscular coherence between healthy participants and participants with PD.

All our participants with PD were responsive to dopaminergic medications and were tested after overnight medication withdrawal in the practically defined off state. However, there could have been some residual effects of dopaminergic medications at the time of testing.

Conclusions

In this study, participants with and without PD performed bilateral, anti-phasic ankle movements. Despite abnormal consistency and symmetry of movement, participants with PD did not significantly differ in the magnitude of corticomuscular coherence from participants without PD. This finding suggests that, for participants with PD, either i) pathological processes outside linear corticomuscular communication contributed to the kinematic abnormalities or ii) PD-related changes in the neural correlates of movement maintained corticomuscular communication but motor performance was still impaired. To delineate whether corticomuscular communication is involved in kinematic abnormalities in PD, future studies should also compare the neural correlates of movement between individuals with and without PD.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Tasnuva Hoque and Utpal Saha for their assistance in performing the experiments with PD.

References

  1. 1. Baltadjieva R, Giladi N, Gruendlinger L, Peretz C, Hausdorff JM. Marked alterations in the gait timing and rhythmicity of patients with de novo Parkinson’s disease. EurJNeurosci. 2006;24: 1815–1820.
  2. 2. Yogev G, Giladi N, Peretz C, Springer S, Simon ES, Hausdorff JM. Dual tasking, gait rhythmicity, and Parkinson’s disease: which aspects of gait are attention demanding? EurJNeurosci. 2005;22: 1248–1256.
  3. 3. Frenkel-Toledo S, Giladi N, Peretz C, Herman T, Gruendlinger L, Hausdorff JM. Effect of gait speed on gait rhythmicity in Parkinson’s disease: variability of stride time and swing time respond differently. JNeuroEngRehabil. 2005;2.
  4. 4. Hausdorff JM, Lowenthal J, Herman T, Gruendlinger L, Peretz C, Giladi N. Rhythmic auditory stimulation modulates gait variability in Parkinson’s disease. EurJNeurosci. 2007;26: 2369–2375.
  5. 5. Hausdorff JM, Schaafsma JD, Balash Y, Bartels AL, Gurevich T, Giladi N. Impaired regulation of stride variability in Parkinson’s disease subjects with freezing of gait. ExpBrain Res. 2003;149: 187–194.
  6. 6. Plotnik M, Giladi N, Balash Y, Peretz C, Hausdorff JM. Is freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease related to asymmetric motor function? AnnNeurol. 2005;57: 656–663.
  7. 7. Plotnik M, Hausdorff JM. The role of gait rhythmicity and bilateral coordination of stepping in the pathophysiology of freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease. 2008;23: S444–S450.
  8. 8. Plotnik M, Giladi N, Hausdorff JM. Bilateral coordination of walking and freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease. EurJNeurosci. 2008;27: 1999–2006.
  9. 9. Halliday DM, Conway BA, Farmer SF, Rosenberg JR. Using electroencephalography to study functional coupling between cortical activity and electromyograms during voluntary contractions in humans. NeurosciLett. Elsevier Ireland Ltd; 1998;241: 5–8.
  10. 10. Ushiyama J, Masakado Y, Fujiwara T, Tsuji T, Hase K, Kimura A, et al. Contraction level-related modulation of corticomuscular coherence differs between the tibialis anterior and soleus muscles in humans. JApplPhysiol. 2012;112: 1258–1267.
  11. 11. Yoshida T, Masani K, Zabjek K, Chen R, Popovic MR. Dynamic Increase in Corticomuscular Coherence during Bilateral, Cyclical Ankle Movements. FrontHuman Neurosci. Frontiers; 2017;11: 200. pmid:28420971
  12. 12. Yoshida T, Masani K, Zabjek K, Chen R, Popovic MR. Dynamic cortical participation during bilateral, cyclical ankle movements: effects of aging. Scientific Reports. Nature Publishing Group; 2017;7: 44658. pmid:28300175
  13. 13. Wu T, Wang L, Hallett M, Li K, Chan P. Neural correlates of bimanual anti-phase and in-phase movements in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2010;133: 2394–2409. pmid:20566485
  14. 14. Plotnik M, Giladi N, Hausdorff JM. A new measure for quantifying the bilateral coordination of human gait: Effects of aging and Parkinson’s disease. ExpBrain Res. 2007;181: 561–570.
  15. 15. Byblow WD, Summers JJ, Thomas J. Spontaneous and intentional dynamics of bimanual coordination in Parkinson’s disease. HumMovSci. 2000;19: 223–249.
  16. 16. Abe K, Asai Y, Matsuo Y, Nomura T, Sato S, Inoue S, et al. Classifying lower limb dynamics in Parkinson’s disease. Brain ResBull. 2003;61: 219–226.
  17. 17. Serrien DJ, Steyvers M, Debaere F, Stelmach GE, Swinnen SP. Bimanual coordination and limb-specific parameterization in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38: 1714–1722. pmid:11099729
  18. 18. Johnson KA, Cunnington R, Bradshaw JL, Phillips JG, Iansek R, Rogers MA. Bimanual co-ordination in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 1998;121: 743–753. pmid:9577398
  19. 19. Van Den Berg C, Beek PJ, Wagenaar RC, Van Wieringen P. C. W. Coordination disorders in patients with Parkinson’s disease: A study of paced rhythmic forearm movements. ExpBrain Res. 2000;134: 174–186.
  20. 20. Salenius S, Avikainen S, Kaakkola S, Hari R, Brown P. Defective cortical drive to muscle in Parkinson’s disease and its improvement with levodopa. Brain. 2002;125: 491–500. pmid:11872607
  21. 21. Pollok B, Krause V, Martsch W, Wach C, Schnitzler A, Südmeyer M. Motor-cortical oscillations in early stages of Parkinson’s disease. JPhysiol. 2012;590: 3203–3212.
  22. 22. Thompson PD, Dick JP, Day BL, Rothwell JC, Berardelli A, Kachi T, et al. Electrophysiology of the corticomotoneurone pathways in patients with movement disorders. 1986;1: 113–117. pmid:3504237
  23. 23. Perretti A, De Rosa A, Marcantonio L, Iodice V, Estraneo A, Manganelli F, et al. Neurophysiological evaluation of motor corticospinal pathways by TMS in idiopathic early-onset Parkinson’s disease. ClinNeurophysiol. 2011;122: 546–549.
  24. 24. Rinne JO, Rummukainen J, Paljarvi L, Rinne UK. Dementia in Parkinson’s disease is related to neuronal loss in the medial substantia nigra. AnnNeurol. 1989;26: 47–50.
  25. 25. Fearnley JM, Lees AJ. Ageing and Parkinson’s disease: substantia nigra regional selectivity. Brain. 1991;114: 2283–2301. pmid:1933245
  26. 26. McGeer PL, Itagaki S, Akiyama H, McGeer EG. Rate of cell death in parkinsonism indicates active neuropathological process. AnnNeurol. 1988;24: 574–576.
  27. 27. Wichmann T, DeLong MR. The basal ganglia. In: Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessell TM, Siegelbaum SA, Hudspeth AJ, editors. Principles of Neural Science. 5 ed. New York: McGraw-Hill; 2013. pp. 960–981.
  28. 28. Obeso JA, Rodriguez-Oroz MC, Rodriguez M, Lanciego JL, Artieda J, Gonzalo N, et al. Pathophysiology of the basal ganglia in Parkinson’s disease. Trends Neurosci. 2000;23: S8–S19. pmid:11052215
  29. 29. Alexander GE, Crutcher MD. Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: neural substrates of parallel processing. Trends Neurosci. 1990;13: 266–271. pmid:1695401
  30. 30. Lang AE, Lozano AM. Parkinson’s disease: second of two parts. NEW ENGLJMED. 1998;339: 1130–1143.
  31. 31. Brazhnik E, Cruz AV, Avila I, Wahba MI, Novikov N, Ilieva NM, et al. State-dependent spike and local field synchronization between motor cortex and substantia nigra in hemiparkinsonian rats. JNeurosci. 2012;32: 7869–7880.
  32. 32. Sharott A, Magill PJ, Harnack D, Kupsch A, Meissner W, Brown P. Dopamine depletion increases the power and coherence of β-oscillations in the cerebral cortex and subthalamic nucleus of the awake rat. EurJNeurosci. 2005;21: 1413–1422.
  33. 33. Williams D, Tijssen M, Van Bruggen G, Bosch A, Insola A, Di Lazzaro V, et al. Dopamine-dependent changes in the functional connectivity between basal ganglia and cerebral cortex in humans. Brain. 2002;125: 1558–1569. pmid:12077005
  34. 34. Kato K, Yokochi F, Taniguchi M, Okiyama R, Kawasaki T, Kimura K, et al. Bilateral coherence between motor cortices and subthalamic nuclei in patients with Parkinson’s disease. ClinNeurophysiol. Elsevier Ireland Ltd; 2015;126: 1941–1950.
  35. 35. Giladi N, Shabtai H, Simon ES, Biran S, Tal J, Korczyn AD. Construction of freezing of gait questionnaire for patients with Parkinsonism. Parkinsonism RelatDisord. 2000;6: 165–170.
  36. 36. Murray MP, Sepic SB, Gardner GM, Downs WJ. Walking patterns of men with parkinsonism. AmJPhysMed. 1978;57: 278–294.
  37. 37. Farina D, Merletti R, Enoka RM. The extraction of neural strategies from the surface EMG: an update. JApplPhysiol. American Physiological Society; 2014;117: 1215–1230.
  38. 38. American Electroencephalographic Society. American electroencephalographic society guidelines for standard electrode position nomenclature. JClinNeurophysiol. 1991;8: 200–202.
  39. 39. Myers LJ, Lowery M, O'Malley M, Vaughan CL, Heneghan C, St Clair Gibson A, et al. Rectification and non-linear pre-processing of EMG signals for cortico-muscular analysis. JNeurosciMethods. 2003;124: 157–165.
  40. 40. Ward NJ, Farmer SF, Berthouze L, Halliday DM. Rectification of EMG in low force contractions improves detection of motor unit coherence in the beta-frequency band. JNeurophysiol. 2013;110: 1744–1750.
  41. 41. Hyvärinen A. Fast and robust fixed-point algorithms for independent component analysis. IEEE TransNeural Networks. Piscataway, NJ, United States: IEEE; 1999;10: 626–634.
  42. 42. Hyvärinen A, Oja E. Independent component analysis: algorithms and applications. Neural Netw. Exeter, United Kingdom: Elsevier Science Ltd; 2000;13: 411–430.
  43. 43. Libenson MH. Electroencephalographic artifacts. Practical Approach to Electroencephalography. Philadelphia: Saunders; 2010. pp. 124–145.
  44. 44. Kilner JM, Baker SN, Salenius S, Hari R, Lemon RN. Human cortical muscle coherence is directly related to specific motor parameters. JNeurosci. 2000;20: 8838–8845.
  45. 45. Trujillo-Ortiz A, Hernandez-Walls R, Barba-Rojo K, Cupul-Magana L. Roystest: Royston’s Multivariate Normality Test. A MATLAB file. [Internet]. http://www.mathworks.com/matlabcentral/fileexchange/loadFile.do?objectId=17811
  46. 46. Ivry RB, Keele SW. Timing functions of the cerebellum. JCognNeurosci. 1989;1: 136–152.
  47. 47. Cerasa A, Hagberg GE, Peppe A, Bianciardi M, Gioia MC, Costa A, et al. Functional changes in the activity of cerebellum and frontostriatal regions during externally and internally timed movement in Parkinson’s disease. Brain ResBull. 2006;71: 259–269.
  48. 48. O'Boyle DJ, Freeman JS, Cody FWJ. The accuracy and precision of timing of self-paced, repetitive movements in subjects with Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 1996;119: 51–70. pmid:8624694
  49. 49. Harrington DL, Haaland KY, Hermanowicz N. Temporal processing in the basal ganglia. Neuropsychology. 1998;12: 3–12. pmid:9460730
  50. 50. Freeman JS, Cody FWJ, Schady W. The influence of external timing cues upon the rhythm of voluntary movements in Parkinson’s disease. JNeurolNeurosurgPsychiatry. BMJ Publishing Group; 1993;56: 1078–1084.
  51. 51. Spencer RMC, Ivry RB. Comparison of patients with Parkinson’s disease or cerebellar lesions in the production of periodic movements involving event-based or emergent timing. Brain Cogn. 2005;58: 84–93. pmid:15878729
  52. 52. Espay AJ, Beaton DE, Morgante F, Gunraj CA, Lang AE, Chen R. Impairments of speed and amplitude of movement in Parkinson’s disease: a pilot study. Wiley Subscription Services, Inc., A Wiley Company; 2009;24: 1001–1008.
  53. 53. Almeida QJ, Wishart LR, Lee TD. Bimanual coordination deficits with Parkinson’s disease: The influence of movement speed and external cueing. 2002;17: 30–37.
  54. 54. Miyachi S, Hikosaka O, Lu X. Differential activation of monkey striatal neurons in the early and late stages of procedural learning. ExpBrain Res. 2002;146: 122–126.
  55. 55. Miyachi S, Hikosaka O, Miyashita K, Kárádi Z, Rand MK. Differential roles of monkey striatum in learning of sequential hand movement. ExpBrain Res. 1997;115: 1–5.
  56. 56. Matsumoto N, Hanakawa T, Maki S, Graybiel AM, Kimura M. Role of nigrostriatal dopamine system in learning to perform sequential motor tasks in a predictive manner. JNeurophysiol. 1999;82: 978–998.
  57. 57. Jueptner M, Weiller C. A review of differences between basal ganglia and cerebellar control of movements as revealed by functional imaging studies. Brain. 1998;121: 1437–1449. pmid:9712006
  58. 58. Wu T, Hallett M. Neural correlates of dual task performance in patients with Parkinson’s disease. JNeurolNeurosurgPsychiatry. 2008;79: 760–766.
  59. 59. Wu T, Liu J, Zhang H, Hallett M, Zheng Z, Chan P. Attention to automatic movements in Parkinson’s disease: Modified automatic mode in the striatum. CerebCortex. Oxford University Press; 2015;25: 3330–3342.
  60. 60. Wu T, Hallett M. A functional MRI study of automatic movements in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2005;128: 2250–2259. pmid:15958505
  61. 61. Brotchie P, Iansek R, Horne MK. Motor function of the monkey globus pallidus: 2. Cognitive aspects of movement and phasic neuronal activity. Brain. 1991;114: 1685–1702. pmid:1884173
  62. 62. Mima T, Hallett M. Corticomuscular coherence: A review. JClinNeurophysiol. 1999;16: 501–511.
  63. 63. Gross J, Tass PA, Salenius S, Hari R, Freund HJ, Schnitzler A. Cortico-muscular synchronization during isometric muscle contraction in humans as revealed by magnetoencephalography. JPhysiol. 2000;527: 623–631.
  64. 64. Castermans T, Duvinage M, Cheron G, Dutoit T. About the cortical origin of the low-delta and high-gamma rhythms observed in EEG signals during treadmill walking. NeurosciLett. 2014;561: 166–170.
  65. 65. Lanciego JL, Luquin N, Obeso JA. Functional neuroanatomy of the basal ganglia. Cold Spring Harbor Perspect Med. Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press; 2012;2.
  66. 66. Hunnicutt BJ, Long BR, Kusefoglu D, Gertz KJ, Zhong H, Mao T. A comprehensive thalamocortical projection map at the mesoscopic level. NatNeurosci. Nature Publishing Group; 2014;17: 1276–1285.
  67. 67. Garcia-Rill E, Houser CR, Skinner RD, Smith W, Woodward DJ. Locomotion-inducing sites in the vicinity of the pedunculopontine nucleus. Brain ResBull. 1987;18: 731–738.
  68. 68. Le Ray D, Juvin L, Ryczko D, Dubuc R. Supraspinal control of locomotion. The mesencephalic locomotor region. Prog Brain Res. 2011;188: 51–70. pmid:21333802
  69. 69. Nutt JG, Bloem BR, Giladi N, Hallett M, Horak FB, Nieuwboer A. Freezing of gait: Moving forward on a mysterious clinical phenomenon. Lancet Neurol. 2011;10: 734–744. pmid:21777828
  70. 70. Eidelberg E, Walden JG, Nguyen LH. Locomotor control in macaque monkeys. Brain. 1981;104: 647–663. pmid:7326562
  71. 71. Goetz L, Piallat B, Bhattacharjee M, Mathieu H, David O, Chabardès S. On the Role of the Pedunculopontine Nucleus and Mesencephalic Reticular Formation in Locomotion in Nonhuman Primates. JNeurosci. 2016;36: 4917–4929. pmid:27147647
  72. 72. Garcia-Rill E. The basal ganglia and the locomotor regions. Brain ResRev. 1986;11: 47–63.
  73. 73. Plaha P, Gill SS. Bilateral deep brain stimulation of the pedunculopontine nucleus for Parkinson’s disease. Neuroreport. 2005;16: 1883–1887. pmid:16272872
  74. 74. Stefani A, Lozano AM, Peppe A, Stanzione P, Galati S, Tropepi D, et al. Bilateral deep brain stimulation of the pedunculopontine and subthalamic nuclei in severe Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2007;130: 1596–1607. pmid:17251240
  75. 75. Tsang EW, Hamani C, Moro E, Mazzella F, Poon YY, Lozano AM, et al. Involvement of the human pedunculopontine nucleus region in voluntary movements. Neurology. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2010;75: 950–959. pmid:20702790
  76. 76. Grillner S, Wallén P, Saitoh K, Kozlov A, Robertson B. Neural bases of goal-directed locomotion in vertebrates-An overview. G S, M A, K O, R S, S P, editors. Brain ResRev. 2008;57: 2–12.
  77. 77. Ryczko D, Dubuc R. Dopamine and the Brainstem Locomotor Networks: From Lamprey to Human. Front Neurosci. Frontiers; 2017;11: 295. pmid:28603482
  78. 78. Rascol O, Sabatini U, Chollet F, Celsis P, Montastruc JL, Marc Vergnes JP, et al. Supplementary and Primary Sensory Motor Area Activity in Parkinson’s Disease: Regional Cerebral Blood Flow Changes During Finger Movements and Effects of Apomorphine. ArchNeurol. 1992;49: 144–148.
  79. 79. Kallio J, Søgaard K, Avela J, Komi PV, Selänne H, Linnamo V. Motor unit firing behaviour of soleus muscle in isometric and dynamic contractions. Lucia A, editor. PLoS ONE. 2013;8: e53425. pmid:23405069
  80. 80. Brouwer B, Ashby P. Corticospinal projections to upper and lower limb spinal motoneurons in man. ElectroencephalogrClinNeurophysiol. 1990;76: 509–519.
  81. 81. Mima T, Toma K, Koshy B, Hallett M. Coherence between cortical and muscular activities after subcortical stroke. Stroke. 2001;32: 2597–2601. pmid:11692023
  82. 82. Rossiter HE, Eaves C, Davis E, Boudrias MH, Park CH, Farmer S, et al. Changes in the location of cortico-muscular coherence following stroke. NeuroImage Clin. 2013;2: 50–55.
  83. 83. Riquelme I, Cifre I, Muñoz MA, Montoya P. Altered corticomuscular coherence elicited by paced isotonic contractions in individuals with cerebral palsy: a case-control study. JElectromyogrKinesiology. Elsevier Ltd; 2014;24: 928–933.
  84. 84. Fujioka T, Trainor LJ, Large EW, Ross B. Internalized timing of isochronous sounds is represented in neuromagnetic beta oscillations. JNeurosci. 2012;32: 1791–1802.
  85. 85. Chakarov V, Naranjo JR, Schulte-Mönting J, Omlor W, Huethe F, Kristeva R. Beta-range EEG-EMG coherence with isometric compensation for increasing modulated low-level forces. JNeurophysiol. 2009;102: 1115–1120.
  86. 86. Kristeva R, Patino L, Omlor W. Beta-range cortical motor spectral power and corticomuscular coherence as a mechanism for effective corticospinal interaction during steady-state motor output. Neuroimage. 2007;36: 785–792. pmid:17493837
  87. 87. Masakado Y, Nielsen JB. Task-and phase-related changes in cortico-muscular coherence. Keio JMed. 2008;57: 50–56.
  88. 88. McClelland VM, Cvetkovic Z, Mills KR. Modulation of corticomuscular coherence by peripheral stimuli. ExpBrain Res. 2012;219: 275–292.
  89. 89. Omlor W, Patino L, Mendez-Balbuena I, Schulte-Mönting J, Kristeva R. Corticospinal beta-range coherence is highly dependent on the pre-stationary motor state. JNeurosci. 2011;31: 8037–8045.
  90. 90. Riddle CN, Baker SN. Digit displacement, not object compliance, underlies task dependent modulations in human corticomuscular coherence. Neuroimage. 2006;33: 618–627. pmid:16963283
  91. 91. Trenado C, Mendez-Balbuena I, Manjarrez E, Huethe F, Schulte-Mönting J, Feige B, et al. Enhanced corticomuscular coherence by external stochastic noise. FrontHuman Neurosci. Frontiers Research Foundation; 2014;8.
  92. 92. Ushiyama J, Katsu M, Masakado Y, Kimura A, Liu M, Ushiba J. Muscle fatigue-induced enhancement of corticomuscular coherence following sustained submaximal isometric contraction of the tibialis anterior muscle. JApplPhysiol. 2011;110: 1233–1240.
  93. 93. Yang Y, Dewald JPA, van der Helm FCT, Schouten AC. Unveiling neural coupling within the sensorimotor system: directionality and nonlinearity. EurJNeurosci. 2017;58: 544. pmid:28887885
  94. 94. Yang Y, Solis-Escalante T, van der Helm FCT, Schouten AC. A Generalized Coherence Framework for Detecting and Characterizing Nonlinear Interactions in the Nervous System. IEEE TransBiomedEng. 2016;63: 2629–2637. pmid:27362753
  95. 95. Yang Y, Solis-Escalante T, van de Ruit M, van der Helm FCT, Schouten AC. Nonlinear Coupling between Cortical Oscillations and Muscle Activity during Isotonic Wrist Flexion. Front Comput Neurosci. Frontiers; 2016;10: 126. pmid:27999537