Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Socio-Ecological Variables Associated with Context-Specific Sitting Time in Belgian Older Adults: A One-Year Follow-Up Study

  • Cedric Busschaert,

    Affiliations Department Movement & Sport Sciences, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium, Research Foundation Flanders (FWO), Brussels, Belgium

  • Anne-Lore Scherrens,

    Affiliations Department Public Health, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium, Department Primary Care, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium

  • Ilse De Bourdeaudhuij ,

    Ilse.Debourdeaudhuij@UGent.be

    Affiliation Department Movement & Sport Sciences, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium

  • Greet Cardon,

    Affiliation Department Movement & Sport Sciences, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium

  • Jelle Van Cauwenberg,

    Affiliations Research Foundation Flanders (FWO), Brussels, Belgium, Department Public Health, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium, Department of Human Biometry and Biomechanics, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium

  • Katrien De Cocker

    Affiliations Department Movement & Sport Sciences, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium, Research Foundation Flanders (FWO), Brussels, Belgium

Abstract

Introduction

Knowledge about variables associated with context-specific sitting time in older adults is limited. Therefore, this study explored cross-sectional and longitudinal associations of socio-demographic, social-cognitive, physical-environmental and health-related variables with sitting during TV viewing, computer use and motorized transport in older adults.

Methods

A sample of Belgian older adults completed structured interviews on context-specific sitting time and associated variables using a longitudinal study design. Objective measurements of grip strength and physical performance were also completed. Complete baseline data were available of 258 participants (73.98±6.16 years) of which 229 participants remained in the study at one year follow-up (retention rate: 91.60%). Cross-sectional correlates (baseline data) and longitudinal predictors (change-scores in relation with change in sitting time) were explored through multiple linear regression analyses.

Results

Per context-specific sitting time, most of the cross-sectional correlates differed from the longitudinal predictors. Increases over time in enjoyment of watching TV (+one unit), encouragement of partner to watch less TV (+one unit) and TV time of partner (+30.0 min/day) were associated with respectively 9.1 min/day (p<0.001), 16.0 min/day (p<0.001) and 12.0 min/day (p<0.001) more sitting during TV viewing at follow-up. Increases over time in enjoyment of using a computer (+one unit), the number of smartphones and tablets (+1) and computer use of the partner (+30.0 min/day) were associated with respectively 5.5 min/day (p < .01), 10.4 min/day (p < .05) and 3.0 min/day (p < .05) more sitting during computer use at follow-up. An increase over time in self-efficacy regarding taking a bicycle or walking was associated with 2.9 min/day (p < .05) less sitting during motorized transport at follow-up.

Conclusions

The results stressed the importance of looking at separate contexts of sitting. Further, the results highlighted the importance of longitudinal research in order to reveal which changes in particular variables predicted changes in context-specific sitting time. Variables at the social-cognitive level were most frequently related to context-specific sitting.

Introduction

Older adults are subject to high levels of total sedentary behaviour, which is defined as any waking activity characterized by an energy expenditure ≤ 1.5 metabolic equivalents (METs) performed in a sitting/reclining posture [1]. According to a recent review, objective measurements of sedentary behaviour showed that older adults sit on average 9.4 hours/day [2]. Sitting receives increasing attention in terms of chronic disease prevention [3], as it has been identified as an independent risk factor, for all-cause mortality, metabolic syndrome, waist circumference, overweight/obesity, increased risk of depression and poor bone health in older adults [46]. For example, it was found that sitting during TV viewing was positively associated with waist circumference among UK older adults [7]. Further, TV viewing was also negatively associated with leg and total body lean mass among community-dwelling older adults [8]. Also, a meta-analysis revealed that prolonged sitting during TV viewing (pooled relative risk: 1.13) and computer or internet use (pooled relative risk: 1.22) were associated with depression [9]. Also, evidence suggested to avoid long periods of uninterrupted sitting time, as more breaks in sitting time were associated with lower triglycerides, waist circumference, Body Mass Index (BMI), and 2-h plasma glucose in adults [10]. Next, replacing sitting time by standing may be beneficial for cardio-metabolic health [11]. However, more research is still needed to explore how many times long periods of sitting should be interrupted (i.e. number of breaks per hour) together with the minimum duration and/or intensity of the break that may be necessary for positive health effects [10].

Sitting is a complex behaviour that occurs in multiple contexts, e.g. TV viewing, computer use and motorized transport [1214]. Additionally, evidence suggested that correlates (cross-sectional) and predictors (longitudinal) might be context-specific, however, more research is warranted to map (differences between) context-specific correlates and predictors in older adults [15]. Finally, focusing on identifying correlates/predictors of context-specific sitting time will be of added value for developing more-effective interventions in the future, as gathering associated variables of total sitting time is too broad to target the complexity of sitting time. Future interventions will be more effective when targeting particular contexts of sitting time. Owen et al. [13] indicated the importance of investigating associated variables of sitting time in particular contexts, as this may lead to more-effective interventions in the future. However, it may be advised to not set up interventions that solely focus on one context of sitting time, as this may not cause meaningful changes in total sitting time.

Socio-ecological models are commonly used to explain sitting time and state that multiple levels of variables influence context-specific sitting time [13]. Owen et al. [13] stressed the need for future research to identify associated variables at multiple levels, including individual, social, organizational/community, environmental and policy levels. Still, knowledge about these variables is limited in older adults compared to other age groups [16, 15]. A systematic review of Chastin et al. [15] within the DEterminants of DIet and Physical ACtivity (DEDIPAC) knowledge hub revealed that evidence on correlates of sitting time in older adults was mostly gathered using cross-sectional studies (n = 22) of which the focus was on leisure time sitting, TV viewing, leisure screen time and sitting in cars. Important to notice is the dearth of information from longitudinal studies investigating associated variables of sitting time which highlight the limited evidence of causal relationships among older adults [15]. In older adults, more time is spent sedentary as age advances [2] and (sedentary) behaviours can change in a relatively short amount of time due to sudden changes in health conditions (becoming ill for example) or living situations (loss of the partner for example. To our knowledge, only three studies previously investigated longitudinal associations with sitting time in this age group [1719]. These three studies only focused on total sitting time or TV viewing. In addition, Chastin et al. [15] highlighted that mostly non-modifiable variables (e.g. socio-demographics) were investigated as potential associated variables of sitting time, which stresses the need for additional research on (multilevel) modifiable variables (e.g. environmental and social level) to inform intervention developers targeting sitting time. Therefore, it is advised to explore motivational and contextual associated variables of sitting time [20]. Furthermore, particularly in older adults, the inclusion of health indicators (pain, fear of falling) as potential variables associated with sitting time is rare, but important [15, 21].

Summarized, it is needed to further explore cross-sectional and longitudinal multilevel variables associated with context-specific sitting time in older adults. The objective of the present longitudinal study with a follow-up period of one year was to investigate multilevel correlates and predictors of context-specific sitting time in older adults who still live independently or in a service flat. First, cross-sectional correlates (i.e. socio-demographic, social-cognitive, physical environmental and health-related variables) of Belgian older adults’ context-specific sitting time at baseline (i.e. TV-viewing, computer use and motorized transport) were identified (aim a). Secondly, it was examined if changes in these socio-demographic, social-cognitive, physical environmental and health-related variables from baseline to follow-up predicted changes in older adults’ context-specific sitting (aim b).

Materials and Methods

Study design

The present study was a longitudinal study with a follow-up period of one year. Baseline data was gathered between September 2013 and April 2014, follow-up data between September 2014 and May 2015.

Subjects and procedures

Older adults’ contact information (full name, address and date of birth of potential participants aged 65 years or older) was gathered via the public service department of the city Sint-Niklaas (metropolitan city with approximately 73,000 inhabitants in Flanders, northern Dutch-speaking part of Belgium). An information letter (background, objectives, practical information about the study, and the announcement of a telephone call from a researcher or visit during the following days) was sent to a random sample of 961 older adults at baseline. From the 961 potential participants, 919 older adults actually received the information letter (42 persons were not reached of which 18 older adults deceased and 24 older adults had moved to another address). After sending the information letter, potential participants were contacted by telephone. The researchers tried to contact the older adults up to three times on different days and different moments during the day [22]. Participants who agreed to participate in the present study completed a questionnaire through a structured interview led by a trained researcher at the participants’ homes [23]. To be included in the study, participants had to be 65 years or older, living independently or in a service flat, Dutch-speaking and be able to stand up (e.g. no recent surgery or other medical condition in which a person cannot stand up). Six hundred sixty-one older adults were not included in the analyses, due to several reasons: not present during the three attempts to contact him/her (n = 59); refused to participate (n = 567); not able to participate due to illness (n = 30); not Dutch-speaking (n = 4); not able to stand up (n = 1). This resulted in a final sample of 258 older adults at baseline (participation rate: 28.07%).

At follow-up, all baseline participants received an information letter of the follow-up measurement (same methodology was used as during baseline). From the 258 participants at baseline, 250 older adults actually received the information letter (7 older adults deceased prior to the follow-up measurement, one moved to another address). Twenty-one older adults did not participate at the follow-up measurement due to several reasons (i.e. not present during the three attempts to contact him/her: n = 1; refused to participate: n = 16; not able to participate due to illness: n = 4). This resulted in a final sample of 229 older adults participating both at baseline and follow-up (retention rate: 91.60%).

Possible differences between the older adults who dropped out and the older adults who remained in the study were analysed by using drop-out analyses. Older adults who dropped out were older (77.55 ± 8.43 years vs. 73.53 ± 5.68 years, p = 0.018) than older adults who remained in the study. No differences were found for BMI (p = 0.280), sex (p = 0.505) and educational level (p = 0.401). Both at baseline and follow-up, participants provided written informed consent. The study protocol was approved by the Ghent University Hospital Ethics Committee.

Measures

In the present study a questionnaire was used that was specifically developed for older adults [23]. This questionnaire assessed total sitting time, context-specific sitting time and its potential socio-demographic, social-cognitive, physical environmental and health-related correlates or predictors. The validity of this questionnaire has been evaluated by comparing the self-reported sitting time to activPAL data and revealed moderate-to-good validity for total sitting time on an average day (ρ = 0.48) [23]. The questionnaire also showed good test-retest reliability (Intraclass Correlation Coefficients (ICC) = 0.80) for total sitting time on an average day [23]. Additionally, every context-specific sitting time (i.e. TV viewing, computer use and motorized transport) included in the present study, showed acceptable-to-good test-retest reliability (ICC = 0.48–0.83) [23]. Furthermore, the test-retest reliability of the included potential predictors of context-specific sitting time was measured and revealed moderate-to-excellent results [23]. However, particular social-cognitive variables, which revealed poor reliability, were excluded from the present analyses (indicated in Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Overview of the included item-specific social-cognitive variables, their mean values at baseline and follow-up and the mean change score.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0167881.t001

Context-specific sitting time.

Three context-specific sitting times (i.e. TV viewing, computer use and motorized transport) were measured identically at baseline and follow-up and were assessed by questioning sitting time in the past seven days (weekday and weekend day separately). The answer categories for sitting during TV viewing and computer use were identical: ‘none’, ‘1–15 min/day’, ‘15–30 min/day’, ‘30–60 min/day’, ‘1–2 hours/day’, ‘2–3 hours/day’, ‘3–4 hours/day’, ‘4–5 hours/day’, ‘5–6 hours/day’, ‘6–7 hours/day’ or ‘more than 7 hours/day’. For sitting during motorized transport, the answer categories were slightly different (‘none’, ‘1–15 min/day’, ‘15–30 min/day’, ‘30–45 min/day’, ‘45–60 min/day’, ‘60–90 min/day’, ‘90–120 min/day’, ‘2–2.5 hours/day’, ‘2.5–3 hours/day’, ‘3–4 hours/day’, ‘4–5 hours/day’, ‘5–6 hours/day’, ‘6–7 hours/day’ or ‘more than 7 hours/day’).

Correlates of context-specific sitting time.

All included correlates of context-specific sitting were measured identically at baseline and follow-up.

Socio-demographic variables. The socio-demographic variables that were assessed were: educational level, sex, family situation, age (determined based on date of birth and date of filling out questionnaire) and residential area (detailed information in Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Overview of the included socio-demographic variables and the recoding used for the cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0167881.t002

Social-cognitive variables. The social-cognitive variables (i.e. attitude, self-efficacy, norm, social norm, social support and modelling) were assessed for sitting during TV viewing, computer use and motorized transport. Importantly, the social-cognitive variables were included as context- and item-specific variables (e.g. attitude 3 TV viewing: ‘I enjoy watching TV for many hours at a time’; attitude 4 TV viewing: ‘watching TV is my way to relax’), as this ensured more detailed information (see Table 1) [24]. The answer categories of the variables measuring attitude, (social) norm, self-efficacy and social support ranged from ‘strongly disagree’ to ‘strongly agree’ (five-point Likert scale). The answer categories concerning modelling (partner) for TV viewing and computer use ranged from ‘none (no TV viewing or computer use)’ to ‘more than 7 hours/day’ (11-point Likert scale). For motorized transport, a dichotomous answer category was used (i.e. active transport or motorized transport).

Physical environmental variables. For each context of sitting time identical physical environmental variables were assessed both at baseline and follow-up. Participants were asked to register the number of equipment regarding TV viewing and computer use available in the household that they operate themselves. For TV-viewing, the variable ‘TV set’ was calculated by summing the number of televisions and the variable ‘other TV-viewing equipment’ by summing the number of laptops, desktops, smartphones and tablets. For computer use, the variable ‘computer equipment’ was calculated by summing the number of laptops and desktops. The variable ‘other equipment for computer use’ was obtained by summing the number of smartphones and tablets. The answer categories for both equipment regarding TV-viewing and computer use ranged from ‘none’ to ‘more than 5’ (7-point Likert scale). Two additional physical environmental variables were included in the analyses regarding TV viewing. Participants had to rate the items ‘the remote controller (TV) can always be found closely to me when I need it’ and ‘the couches at our place are comfortable to sit for a long time’, with answer categories ranging from ‘strongly disagree’ to ‘strongly agree’ (five-point Likert scale). For motorized transport, older adults documented the number of all operational motorized vehicles (no electric bicycles) available in the household. A detailed overview of the included physical environmental variables can be found in Table 3.

thumbnail
Table 3. Overview of the included physical environmental variables, their mean values at baseline and follow-up and the mean change scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0167881.t003

Health-related variables. Different health-related variables were assessed using objective (by a trained researcher after filling out questionnaire) or subjective measurements (part of the questionnaire). Older adults rated the following items (five-point Likert scale): general health (‘poor’ to ‘excellent’), to what extent pain interfere with their activities during the past weeks (‘not at all’ to ‘quite a lot’) and fear of falling (‘none of the time’ to ‘all of the time’) [2527]. Participants’ weight up to 0.1 kg (SECA 813 Robusta weight scale) and height up to 0.1 cm (SECA 213 stadiometer) was measured in order to calculate BMI (kg/m2). Further, grip strength (kg) of participants was measured twice, of which the best was included in the analyses, by using a hand dynamometer (Hydraulic Saehan). Participants were explained to stand upright, holding the dynamometer in their dominant hand with the arm held out downwards (without making contact with the body). The ‘Short Physical Performance Battery’ (SPPB) was used to measure the physical performance, consisting of three tests: balance, 3.0 meter walk (instead of 2.4 meter) and a chair stands test [28]. Afterwards, every test was scored (0–4) individually with ‘0’ indicating that the person was not capable or did not perform the test and ‘4’ for the best score, resulting in a final SPPB score (maximum of 12 points). A detailed overview of the included health-related variables can be found in Table 4.

thumbnail
Table 4. Overview of the included health-related variables, their mean values at baseline and follow-up and the mean change scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0167881.t004

Potential covariate of context-specific sitting time.

The short-IPAQ (International Physical Activity Questionnaire) was used to estimate total physical activity (PA) at baseline and follow-up [2931]. Participants reported the frequency (days/week) and time (hours or minutes per day) of their PA during the past seven days, including walking, moderate-intensity PA and vigorous-intensity PA.

Data reduction

Context-specific sitting time.

The midpoint values of the above-mentioned answer categories (i.e. sitting time) were determined in order to calculate a numeric variable for sitting during TV viewing, computer use and motorized transport. Subsequently, daily minutes for an average day were measured by using the following formula: ((sitting time on a weekday * 5) + (sitting time on a weekend day * 2))/7. Afterwards, baseline measurement was subtracted from follow-up measurement (i.e. follow-up minus baseline) to calculate a change-score for all included context-specific sitting times separately.

Correlates of context-specific sitting time.

Potential cross-sectional correlates of sitting during TV viewing, computer use and motorized transport were identified by using the baseline measurements. Changes in socio-demographic, social-cognitive, physical environmental and health-related variables from baseline to follow-up were included as potential longitudinal predictors of changes in context-specific sitting times (subtraction of baseline measurements from follow-up measurements).

The following potential socio-demographic correlates were included in the baseline measurements: educational level, sex, family situation and residential area. In the longitudinal analyses, only a change-score was calculated for family situation, as other above-mentioned variables had a limited variance during one year of follow-up (i.e. residential area) or asking for a change was irrelevant (i.e. sex and educational level). Table 2 gives an overview of the included socio-demographic variables in the cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses (including scoring methods and descriptive statistics [% or mean ± SD]).

Furthermore, all social-cognitive, physical environmental and health-related variables were scored/recoded in the same direction to facilitate interpretation of the results (highest score is most positive answer). A change-score (i.e. follow-up minus baseline) for each of these variables was calculated. Importantly, item-specific values regarding the social-cognitive variables were used to calculate change-scores. More information on the scoring methods and descriptive statistics (mean ± SD) of the social-cognitive variables is provided in Table 1.

Potential covariate of context-specific sitting time.

Walking, moderate-intensity PA and vigorous-intensity PA were processed using the scoring protocol from the IPAQ [32]. Total PA was measured by summing up the three PA variables. Total PA at baseline was included as a covariate in the cross-sectional analyses and a change-score for total PA (i.e. follow-up minus baseline) was included as a covariate in the longitudinal analyses.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses for the cross-sectional and longitudinal research questions were performed in different software programs. First, for the cross-sectional analyses, potential correlates of context-specific sitting time at baseline (aim a) were examined using R Studio Version 0.98.507. Furthermore, SAS version 9.4 software (SAS institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA) was used to examine longitudinal changes in potential predictors related to changes in context-specific sitting time (aim b). Statistical significance was determined at α = 0.05.

Potential cross-sectional correlates of context-specific sitting time at baseline (aim a).

For the cross-sectional analyses, the statistical model used to examine the correlates of TV viewing was different than the models used for computer use and motorized transport due to different distributions of these dependent variables. TV viewing was normally distributed, so standard linear regression analyses were performed. Zero-inflated negative binomial regression models (ZINB) were used to examine the correlates of computer use and motorized transport, as these contexts were positively skewed and included many zero counts. The necessity of using zero-inflated models was confirmed by Vuong tests [33]. Based on Akaike’s Information Criterium (AIC) ZINB models were preferred over zero-inflated poisson (ZIP) models. The interpretation of ZINB models is two-fold as these models estimate simultaneously two regression coefficients per potential correlate. First, a logistic regression is fitted in which the relationship with the probability to be a person who did not sit during computer use or motorized transport is estimated (odds ratio (OR)). Simultaneously, ZINB models examine the relationship between potential correlates and the volume of computer use/motorized transport among those who did sit during computer use or motorized transport (negative binomial regression coefficient, expressed in minutes/week). The exponent of a negative binomial regression coefficient (expB) can be interpreted as a relative increase/decrease in sitting time during computer use or motorized transport associated with a one-unit increase in the correlate. For example, an expB of 1.60 for attitude implies that, among those who did sit during computer use, a one-unit higher score in attitude was associated with 60% more minutes/week in sitting during computer use.

Correlates of context-specific sitting time (i.e. TV viewing, computer use and motorized transport) were identified in four steps, separately for each included context-specific sitting time variable. First, multicollinearity between the quantitative correlates was tested per level (i.e. socio-demographic, social-cognitive, physical environmental and health-related variables). When two correlates belonging to one level showed Pearson correlation coefficients higher than 0.60 with each other [34], the correlate that had the lowest Pearson correlation coefficient with the context-specific sitting time variable was not included in the analyses. Following this procedure, ‘my partner encourages me to spend less time using a computer’ (social support 1 computer use) and ‘my partner encourages me to watch less TV’ (social support 1 TV viewing) were removed. Secondly, for each level, a model was fitted containing all correlates within that level (i.e. four regression models per context-specific sitting time variable). The correlates that showed p<0.10 with the context-specific sitting time variable were included in the next step [35, 36]. In step 3, multicollinearity between the quantitative correlates that yielded a p<0.10 in step 2 was tested. None of these correlates were highly correlated (Pearson r>0.60) with each other. In step 4, the correlates with p<0.10 in step 2 were combined into one model. The results of step 4 were presented and discussed in the current paper. All the cross-sectional analyses were adjusted for total PA at baseline.

Changes in potential predictors related to changes in context-specific sitting time (aim b).

For the longitudinal analyses, the change-scores for TV viewing, computer use and motorized transport were normally distributed and linear regression analyses were performed using PROC CALIS [37]. Full-information maximum likelihood was used as estimation procedure as this effectively utilizes all available information in the data. This procedure is recommended when dealing with missing data on both outcome and predictor variables, as was the case using the present longitudinal data [37]. The longitudinal analyses examined if change-scores of potential predictors predicted changes in context-specific sitting time. In these analyses the same stepwise approach was used as described above to identify the correlates. In step 1, no potential predictors were removed due to multicollinearity. Secondly, for each level separately, multiple regression models were performed containing all change-scores of potential predictors within that level (i.e. four regression models per context-specific sitting time variable). In step 3, none of the remaining change-scores of potential predictors were highly correlated with each other. In step 4, the change-scores of potential predictors with p<0.10 in step 2 were combined into one multiple linear regression model per context-specific sitting time variable. The results of step 4 were presented and discussed in the current paper. All the analyses were adjusted for change in total PA between baseline and follow-up and baseline context-specific sitting time. Furthermore the longitudinal analyses were adjusted for age at baseline, as older adults who dropped out were older than older adults who remained in the study.

Results

Sample characteristics

Table 5 gives an overview of the socio-demographic characteristics, BMI and context-specific sitting at baseline and follow-up.

thumbnail
Table 5. Sample characteristics at baseline and follow-up.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0167881.t005

Aim a: Cross-sectional analyses

The aim was to identify socio-demographic, social-cognitive, physical environmental and health-related correlates of sitting during TV viewing, computer use and motorized transport at baseline. The results of this aim are presented in Table 6.

thumbnail
Table 6. Associations between item-specific correlates and sitting during TV time, computer use and motorized transport: estimates, odds ratios, regression coefficients from the cross-sectional analyses using baseline data.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0167881.t006

Eight variables were significantly related to sitting during TV viewing. A one-unit higher score for ‘I enjoy watching TV for many hours at a time’ (attitude 3), ‘watching TV is my way to relax’ (attitude 4), and ‘my partner thinks I spend too much time watching TV’ (social norm) was associated with respectively 12.7 minutes/day, 13.9 minutes/day and 16.3 minutes/day more sitting during TV viewing. A one-unit higher score for ‘I consider it possible to turn off the TV during weekend days until 5:00 p.m.’ (self-efficacy 2) was associated with 11.6 minutes/day less sitting during TV viewing. Furthermore, 1 minute/day more sitting during TV viewing of the partner was associated with 0.4 minutes/day more sitting during TV viewing of the participant. Next, a one-unit higher score for the number of ‘other TV-viewing equipment’ was associated with 9.5 minutes/day less sitting during TV viewing. Also, a one-unit higher score for ‘general health’ was associated with 16.3 minutes/day less sitting during TV viewing. Lastly, a one-unit higher BMI was associated with 3.0 minutes/day more sitting during TV viewing.

In the logit model for sitting during computer use, no significant relationships were observed. In the negative binomial model, ‘I think using a computer is pleasant’ (attitude 1) was significantly related to sitting during computer use. Among those who did sit during computer use, a one-unit higher score for ‘I think using a computer is pleasant’ (attitude 1) was associated with 66% minutes more sitting during computer use.

One variable of the logit model and two variables of the negative binomial model were significantly related to sitting during motorized transport. The logit model indicated that participants with the possession of one additional motorized vehicle had 98% lower odds of being a person who did not sit during motorized transport. In other words, a one-unit higher score for the number of motorized vehicles was associated with a higher probability to sit during motorized transport. Furthermore, the negative binomial model indicated that among those who did sit during motorized transport, a one-unit higher score for ‘I think that I spend too much time using motorized transport’ (norm) was associated with 14% minutes more sitting during motorized transport. The negative binomial model also indicated that among those who did sit during motorized transport, more active transport of the partner (modelling) was associated with 24% minutes less sitting during motorized transport of the participant.

Aim b: Longitudinal analyses

For aim b, changes in socio-demographic, social-cognitive, physical environmental and health-related predictors from baseline to follow-up related to changes in sitting during TV viewing, computer use and motorized transport were identified. Table 7 presents the change-scores in potential predictors associated with change in context-specific sitting over a one-year follow-up period.

thumbnail
Table 7. Associations between item-specific change-scores of the predictors and the change-scores of sitting during TV time, computer use and motorized transport: regression coefficients from the longitudinal analyses.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0167881.t007

Four change-scores were significantly related to change in sitting during TV viewing. An increase from baseline to follow-up with one unit on the five-point Likert scale for ‘I enjoy watching TV for many hours at a time’ (attitude 3) and ‘my partner encourages me to watch less TV’ (social support 1) was associated with respectively 9.1 minutes/day and 16.0 minutes/day more sitting during TV viewing at follow-up. Furthermore, an increase from baseline to follow-up with 1 minute/day sitting during TV viewing of the partner was associated with 0.4 minutes/day more sitting during TV viewing at follow-up of the participant. Also, an increase from baseline to follow-up with one unit on the five-point Likert scale for ‘my partner thinks I spend too much time watching TV’ (social norm) was associated with 8.3 minutes/day less sitting during TV viewing at follow-up.

Three change-scores were significantly related to change in sitting during computer use. An increase from baseline to follow-up with one unit on the five-point Likert scale for ‘I enjoy using a computer for many hours at a time’ (attitude 3) was associated with 5.5 minutes/day more sitting during computer use at follow-up. Next, an increase from baseline to follow-up in the number of ‘other equipment for computer use’ with one unit was associated with 10.4 minutes/day more sitting during computer use at follow-up. Lastly, an increase from baseline to follow-up with 1 minute/day sitting during computer use of the partner was associated with 0.1 minutes/day more sitting during computer use at follow-up of the participant.

Furthermore, one change-score was significantly related to change in sitting during motorized transport. An increase from baseline to follow-up with one unit on the five-point Likert scale for ‘I consider it possible to take the bicycle or to go by foot spontaneously even if it is possible to use a car’ (self-efficacy 3) was associated with 2.9 minutes/day less sitting during motorized transport at follow-up.

Discussion

The present study investigated cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between socio-ecological variables and sitting during TV viewing, computer use and motorized transport in older adults. The results increase the knowledge on sitting time in older adults, as few studies investigated correlates or predictors of context-specific sitting time at multiple levels (i.e. socio-demographic, social-cognitive, physical environmental and health-related variables) in this age group [15].

An important finding of the present cross-sectional results is the fact that different correlates were identified for the different contexts, i.e. sitting during TV viewing, computer use and motorized transport. A previous Belgian study also identified different associated variables for different outcomes, i.e. driving a car, computer use and TV viewing in older adults [14]. In the present study, social-cognitive correlates were most frequently related to the included context-specific sitting behaviours. For TV viewing, general health and several social-cognitive correlates had the largest impact. A higher score for general health was associated with less sitting during TV viewing (-16.3 minutes/day). Further, attitudes were also identified as an important construct of screen-related sitting time. More enjoyment of watching TV for many hours and finding TV time is a way to relax were positively associated with sitting during TV viewing (respectively +12.7 and +13.9 minutes/day). Previous research already stressed the importance of enjoyment regarding watching TV [38]. Also, among older adults who did sit during computer use, the aspect that this is pleasant was positively associated with sitting during computer use. However, no other correlates were cross-sectionally related to computer use. Next, the social aspect seems important for some contexts (i.e. TV viewing and motorized transport), as modelling of the partner was positively associated with sitting during TV viewing of the participant. Consequently, older adults may watch more TV if they have a partner who watches TV for many hours. On the other hand, previous research also showed that emotional loneliness was positively associated with TV viewing [39]. Among the participants who did sit during motorized transport, more active transport of the partner was associated with less sitting during motorized transport of the participant. In line with this finding, a higher level of modelling (e.g. from family) was associated with higher PA levels (e.g. active transportation) in older adolescents and older adults [4042]. However, for motorized transport, the number of vehicles seems to have the largest positive impact. No other physical environmental correlates were significantly related to the sitting outcomes. Further, in the present study no correlates at the health-related level were identified for sitting during computer use and motorized transport. However, a higher BMI was associated with more sitting during TV viewing (+3.0 minutes/day), which is in line with the conclusions of Chastin et al. [15]. Finally, no socio-demographic correlates were related to any of the different contexts of sitting. The latter is in contrast with findings of previous studies [39, 14, 43, 44]. This may be because variables other than socio-demographics were included in the final step of the analyses. Including these other type of variables (e.g. social-cognitive variables) may have resulted in non-significant relationships for the socio-demographic variables, suggesting that these other variables explain socio-demographic differences in context-specific sitting time.

The longitudinal analyses revealed which changes in particular variables from baseline to follow-up were associated with changes in context-specific sitting time. The results showed that social-cognitive predictors were most frequently related to the included context-specific sitting time variables, especially to TV viewing. No socio-demographic or health-related variables were related to changes in sitting during TV viewing, computer use and motorized transport. Further, different predictors were identified for these three contexts of sitting. However, particular similarities were found between sitting during TV viewing and computer use. For TV viewing and computer use, the largest effect sizes were found for increases from baseline to follow-up for enjoyment when sitting during these particular contexts (+9.1 minutes/day in sitting during TV viewing and +5.5 minutes/day in sitting during computer use). The time their partner spend sitting in these contexts was also associated with more sitting during TV viewing and computer use at follow-up of the participant. An increase of 60 minutes in sitting during TV viewing or computer use of the partner from baseline to follow-up was associated with 22 minutes more sitting during TV viewing and 7 minutes more sitting during computer use of the participant at follow-up. For older adults, it may be advised to replace screen-related sitting time with social and (light) physical demanding activities that older adults also relate with enjoyment. Nevertheless, it will be hard to discourage screen-related sitting time, as changes in enjoyment were strongly related to changes in sitting during TV viewing and computer use. Consequently, some older adults might not want to replace screen-related sitting time with physical demanding activities, as enjoyment is not largely modifiable. In this perspective, Gardner et al. [19] considered the possible constraints of interventions that aim to replace TV time with physical demanding activities. The latter study suggested to integrate light-intensity physical activities during TV time of older adults (i.e. not minimizing TV time, but breaking-up TV time or being physically active while watching TV), such as putting the remote control next to the TV, standing up during advertisement breaks or using an elliptical device (stationary bicycle without a seat) [45, 46] as these kind of activities may also have positive effects on health [10,11]. Further, the longitudinal analyses showed that for computer time, influencing the possession of desktops and laptops might have the largest impact (an increase in the number of devices was associated with +10.4 minutes/day of sitting during computer use). However, reducing the possession of computer devices may not be feasible, so future interventions might better aim to minimize high levels of sitting during computer use together with strategies to break up sitting time [45]. For motorized transport, an increase over time for ‘considering it possible to take the bicycle or to walk instead of using a car’ was associated with less sitting (-2.9 minutes/day) during motorized transport at follow-up. Therefore, intervention developers should increase older adults’ self-efficacy about using active transport in order to minimize sitting during motorized transport in elderly. Regarding the longitudinal analyses, it should be noted that the findings do not comprise causality. The present predictors can be indicators as they may be an indication for behavioural change, however evidence for causality should come from randomized controlled experiments.

Results showed different correlates and predictors for sitting during TV viewing, computer use and motorized transport in older adults, which is in line with a study in Dutch adults [47]. Furthermore, only three variables were found to be related to context-specific sitting in both the cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses, i.e., ‘enjoyment of watching TV for many hours’, ‘social norm’ (TV viewing) and ‘modelling of the partner’ (TV viewing). So, most of the cross-sectional correlates differed from the longitudinal predictors in the present study. This may be explained by the limited variance over time in the predictors. Therefore, additional experimental research is recommended to identify if changing cross-sectional correlates also leads to changes in context-specific sitting time (e.g. intervention studies). Still, the longitudinal predictors are more likely to introduce changes in sitting time compared with the cross-sectional correlates, as changes in these variables were already associated with changes in sitting time. Furthermore, correlates and predictors at the social-cognitive level were most frequently associated with context-specific sitting time, with variables predominantly belonging to the ‘Attitudes-Social influences-Efficacy’ model (ASE model) [48]. Few correlates and predictors were found at the physical environmental and health-related level. However, they had great impact on motorized transport (possession of a vehicle) according to the cross-sectional analyses and for computer use (possession of desktop and laptop) according to the longitudinal analyses. Both cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses revealed that neither grip strength nor physical performance were associated with context-specific sitting time. In line with this finding, no (strong) associations were found between grip strength and TV time, self-reported and objectively measured sitting time [49, 50]. However, Hamer and Stamatakis [51] found that older adults with high levels of TV time (≥ 6hrs/d) had poorer grip strength compared to older adults with lower levels of TV time (< 2hrs/d). Contrarily, using the internet was associated with better grip strength [51]. Nevertheless, the interpretation of internet use was based on a yes/no-question, without quantifying the amount of time [49]. Furthermore, previous research found that objectively measured sitting time was negatively associated with physical performance (total SPPB score) in older adults living in retirement communities, however, no association was found between self-reported sitting time and total SPPB score [52].

A first limitation of the present study is the large number (n = 567; 61.7%) of potential participants who refused to participate at baseline, which may have resulted in self-selection bias (e.g. more motivated people). Secondly, older adults who remained in the study differed in certain characteristics compared to the population of Flemish older adults in 2013, so generalizability may be limited. Older adults living in Flanders had a mean BMI of 26.0 kg/m2 (vs 27.6 kg/m2), 66.7% rated their health as good to very good (vs 78.6%) and 64.1% indicated that pain (almost) never interfered in normal work in the past four weeks (vs 79.5%) [53]. Thus, more research is warranted to verify the generalizability of the present results for older adults with a lower BMI, worse general health and more pain during normal work. As a consequence, the lower participation rates together with the observed differences in characteristics between the sample of older adults in the present study and the general population may have resulted in less precise or biased findings, resulting in less generalizable findings. Thirdly, context-specific sitting was self-reported and not validated yet, as criterion validity (compared to ActivPAL data) was only determined for total sitting time (summation of 12 self-reported contexts of sitting). As a result, the associations found do only apply to the self-reported sitting outcomes and may differ from associations tested with objectively measured sitting outcomes. Lastly, variables included in the level ‘physical environmental variables’ focused particularly on the physical home environment (i.e. screen-related equipment and motorized vehicles). However, it may be possible that the close neighborhood environment has an influence on context-specific sitting time of older adults, as neighborhoods can support or hinder the activities outside the home [35]. Therefore, it may be advised to gather more information on urban environments that encourage older people to go outside, as recently older women indicated that urban design is a barrier for reducing sitting time (e.g. lack of benches on which older adults can rest during walking activities) [21]. Finally, the stepwise analyses approach may not exclude the chance of type I errors when having multiple tests. An important strength of the present study is the longitudinal study design, as to our knowledge limited evidence is available on longitudinal associations between socio-ecological variables and sitting time in older adults. However, future research using a longer follow-up period or additional time points may strengthen the present findings or perhaps highlight new interesting findings (e.g. associations between physical performance and sitting time). Furthermore, the use of context-specific sitting time is of importance to develop future intervention studies aiming to minimize sitting time. Thirdly, the included social-cognitive variables were item-specific, as different social-cognitive variables belonging to one construct (e.g. attitude 1–4) measure slightly different, but all important aspects of the associated construct (e.g. attitude). Therefore, it was relevant to keep these items separately in the analyses for interpretation regarding the development of effective future interventions. Finally, a small drop-out was noticed over a one-year follow-up period.

Conclusions

In general, the present study found different correlates and predictors for sitting during TV viewing, computer use and motorized transport, stressing the importance of looking at separate contexts of sitting. Despite these differences, variables at the social-cognitive level were most frequently related to context-specific sitting. Further, the cross-sectional correlates associated with context-specific sitting mostly differed from their longitudinal predictors of these context-specific sitting behaviours. The longitudinal analyses showed a positive association between attitude and modelling of the partner and context-specific sitting time, especially for TV viewing and computer use. Therefore, it may be advised to set up family-based interventions that focus both on decreasing and interrupting long periods of sitting time. As available evidence is limited about examining predictors of changes in context-specific sitting among older adults, more longitudinal research is warranted to confirm present findings. The present findings can support the development of more-effective interventions to minimize context-specific sitting time among older adults.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all older adults who participated in the present study.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: CB IDB GC JVC KDC.
  2. Formal analysis: CB JVC KDC.
  3. Funding acquisition: IDB.
  4. Investigation: CB AS.
  5. Methodology: CB IDB GC JVC KDC.
  6. Resources: IDB GC KDC.
  7. Software: CB IDB GC JVC KDC.
  8. Supervision: IDB GC KDC.
  9. Writing – original draft: CB AS JVC KDC.
  10. Writing – review & editing: CB AS IDB GC JVC KDC.

References

  1. 1. Sedentary Behaviour RN: Letter to the editor: standardized use of the terms "sedentary" and "sedentary behaviours". Appl Physiol Nutr Metab 2012,37:540–542. pmid:22540258
  2. 2. Harvey JA, Chastin SFM, Skelton DA. How Sedentary are Older People? A Systematic Review of the Amount of Sedentary Behavior. J Aging Phys Activ. 2015;23(3):471–87.
  3. 3. Owen N, Salmon J, Koohsari MJ, Turrell G, Giles-Corti B. Sedentary behaviour and health: mapping environmental and social contexts to underpin chronic disease prevention. Brit J Sport Med. 2014;48(3):174–7.
  4. 4. de Rezende LFM, Rey-Lopez JP, Matsudo VKR, Luiz OD. Sedentary behavior and health outcomes among older adults: a systematic review. Bmc Public Health. 2014;14.
  5. 5. Chastin SF, Mandrichenko O, Helbostadt JL, Skelton DA. Associations between objectively-measured sedentary behaviour and physical activity with bone mineral density in adults and older adults, the NHANES study. Bone. 2014;64:254–62. pmid:24735973
  6. 6. Zhai L, Zhang Y, Zhang DF. Sedentary behaviour and the risk of depression: a meta-analysis. Brit J Sport Med. 2015;49(11):705–9.
  7. 7. Stamatakis E, Davis M, Stathi A, Hamer M. Associations between multiple indicators of objectively-measured and self-reported sedentary behaviour and cardiometabolic risk in older adults. Prev Med. 2012;54(1):82–7. pmid:22057055
  8. 8. Gianoudis J, Bailey CA, Daly RM. Associations between sedentary behaviour and body composition, muscle function and sarcopenia in community-dwelling older adults. Osteoporosis international: a journal established as result of cooperation between the European Foundation for Osteoporosis and the National Osteoporosis Foundation of the USA. 2014.
  9. 9. Zhai L, Zhang Y, Zhang D. Sedentary behaviour and the risk of depression: a meta-analysis. British journal of sports medicine. 2015;49(11):705–9. pmid:25183627
  10. 10. Healy GN, Dunstan DW, Salmon J, Cerin E, Shaw JE, Zimmet PZ et al. Breaks in sedentary time: beneficial associations with metabolic risk. Diabetes Care. 2008;31(4):661–6. pmid:18252901
  11. 11. Healy GN, Winkler EA, Owen N, Anuradha S, Dunstan DW. Replacing sitting time with standing or stepping: associations with cardio-metabolic risk biomarkers. European heart journal. 2015.
  12. 12. Chastin SFM, Schwarz U, Skelton DA. Development of a Consensus Taxonomy of Sedentary Behaviors (SIT): Report of Delphi Round 1. Plos One. 2013;8(12).
  13. 13. Owen N, Sugiyama T, Eakin EE, Gardiner PA, Tremblay MS, Sallis JF. Adults' Sedentary Behavior Determinants and Interventions. Am J Prev Med. 2011;41(2):189–96. pmid:21767727
  14. 14. Van Cauwenberg J, Van Holle V, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Owen N, Deforche B. Diurnal Patterns and Correlates of Older Adults' Sedentary Behavior. Plos One. 2015;10(8):e0133175. pmid:26244676
  15. 15. Chastin SF, Buck C, Freiberger E, Murphy M, Brug J, Cardon G et al. Systematic literature review of determinants of sedentary behaviour in older adults: a DEDIPAC study. The international journal of behavioral nutrition and physical activity. 2015;12:127. pmid:26437960
  16. 16. Dogra S, Stathokostas L. Correlates of extended sitting time in older adults: an exploratory cross-sectional analysis of the Canadian Community Health Survey Healthy Aging Cycle. International journal of public health. 2014.
  17. 17. van der Berg JD, Bosma H, Caserotti P, Eiriksdottir G, Arnardottir NY, Martin KR et al. Midlife Determinants Associated with Sedentary Behavior in Old Age. Med Sci Sport Exer. 2014;46(7):1359–65.
  18. 18. Balboa-Castillo T, Leon-Munoz LM, Graciani A, Rodriguez-Artalejo F, Guallar-Castillon P. Longitudinal association of physical activity and sedentary behavior during leisure time with health-related quality of life in community-dwelling older adults. Health Qual Life Out. 2011;9.
  19. 19. Gardner B, Iliffe S, Fox KR, Jefferis BJ, Hamer M. Sociodemographic, behavioural and health factors associated with changes in older adults' TV viewing over 2 years. Int J Behav Nutr Phy. 2014;11.
  20. 20. Brug J, Chinapaw M. Determinants of engaging in sedentary behavior across the lifespan; lessons learned from two systematic reviews conducted within DEDIPAC. The international journal of behavioral nutrition and physical activity. 2015;12:134. pmid:26453317
  21. 21. Chastin SF, Fitzpatrick N, Andrews M, Dicroce N. Determinants of sedentary behavior, motivation, barriers and strategies to reduce sitting time in older women: a qualitative investigation. International journal of environmental research and public health. 2014;11(1):773–91. pmid:24402064
  22. 22. Van Holle V, Van Cauwenberg J, Van Dyck D, Deforche B, Van de Weghe N, De Bourdeaudhuij I. Relationship between neighborhood walkability and older adults' physical activity: results from the Belgian Environmental Physical Activity Study in Seniors (BEPAS Seniors). The international journal of behavioral nutrition and physical activity. 2014;11:110. pmid:25148845
  23. 23. Busschaert C, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Van Holle V, Chastin SF, Cardon G, De Cocker K. Reliability and validity of three questionnaires measuring context-specific sedentary behaviour and associated correlates in adolescents, adults and older adults. The international journal of behavioral nutrition and physical activity. 2015;12(1):117.
  24. 24. Van Dyck D, Cardon G, Deforche B, Owen N, De Cocker K, Wijndaele K et al. Socio-demographic, psychosocial and home-environmental attributes associated with adults' domestic screen time. Bmc Public Health. 2011;11.
  25. 25. Walters SJ, Munro JF, Brazier JE. Using the SF-36 with older adults: a cross-sectional community-based survey. Age Ageing. 2001;30(4):337–43. pmid:11509313
  26. 26. Haywood KL, Garratt AM, Fitzpatrick R. Quality of life in older people: a structured review of generic self-assessed health instruments. Qual Life Res. 2005;14(7):1651–68. pmid:16119178
  27. 27. Ware JE, Kosinski M, Keller SD. SF-36 Physical and mental health summary scales: a user manual and interpretation guide. 1994.
  28. 28. Guralnik JM, Ferrucci L, Simonsick EM, Salive ME, Wallace RB. Lower-Extremity Function in Persons over the Age of 70 Years as a Predictor of Subsequent Disability. New Engl J Med. 1995;332(9):556–61. pmid:7838189
  29. 29. Tran DV, Lee AH, Au TB, Nguyen CT, Hoang DV. Reliability and validity of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire-Short Form for older adults in Vietnam. Health promotion journal of Australia: official journal of Australian Association of Health Promotion Professionals. 2013;24(2):126–31.
  30. 30. Tomioka K, Iwamoto J, Saeki K, Okamoto N. Reliability and Validity of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ) in Elderly Adults: The Fujiwara-kyo Study. J Epidemiol. 2011;21(6):459–65. pmid:21946625
  31. 31. Kolbe-Alexander TL, Lambert EV, Harkins JB, Ekelund U. Comparison of two methods of measuring physical activity in South African older adults. J Aging Phys Act. 2006;14(1):98–114. pmid:16648654
  32. 32. Guidelines for Data Processing and Analysis of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ)–Short and Long Forms. 2005. https://sites.google.com/site/theipaq/scoring-protocol.
  33. 33. Cheung YB. Zero-inflated models for regression analysis of count data: a study of growth and development. Stat Med. 2002;21(10):1461–9. pmid:12185896
  34. 34. De Bourdeaudhuij I, Lefevre J, Deforche B, Wijndaele K, Matton L, Philippaerts R. Physical activity and psychosocial correlates in normal weight and overweight 11 to 19 year olds. Obes Res. 2005;13(6):1097–105. pmid:15976153
  35. 35. De Bourdeaudhuij I, Sallis J. Relative contribution of psychosocial variables to the explanation of physical activity in three population-based adult samples. Preventive medicine. 2002;34(2):279–88. pmid:11817925
  36. 36. Busschaert C, Cardon G, Van Cauwenberg J, Maes L, Van Damme J, Hublet A et al. Tracking and Predictors of Screen Time From Early Adolescence to Early Adulthood: A 10-Year Follow-up Study. The Journal of adolescent health: official publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine. 2015.
  37. 37. Allison DP. Handling Missing Data by Maximum Likelihood. Statistical Horizons, Haverford, PA, USA. 2012;http://statisticalhorizons.com/wp-content/uploads/MissingDataByML.pdf.
  38. 38. Salmon J, Owen N, Crawford D, Bauman A, Sallis JF. Physical activity and sedentary behavior: A population-based study of barriers, enjoyment, and preference. Health Psychol. 2003;22(2):178–88. pmid:12683738
  39. 39. Van Cauwenberg J, De Donder L, Clarys P, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Owen N, Dury S et al. Relationships of individual, social, and physical environmental factors with older adults' television viewing time. J Aging Phys Act. 2014;22(4):508–17. pmid:24231688
  40. 40. Griffith DM, King A, Allen JO. Male Peer Influence on African American Men's Motivation for Physical Activity: Men's and Women's Perspectives. Am J Mens Health. 2013;7(2):169–78. pmid:23160732
  41. 41. Thogersen-Ntoumani C. An ecological model of predictors of stages of change for physical activity in Greek older adults. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2009;19(2):286–96. pmid:18282227
  42. 42. Deforche B, Van Dyck D, Verloigne M, De Bourdeaudhuij I. Perceived social and physical environmental correlates of physical activity in older adolescents and the moderating effect of self-efficacy. Prev Med. 2010;50:S24–S9. pmid:19818363
  43. 43. Meneguci J, Sasaki JE, Santos AD, Scatena LM, Damiao R. Socio-demographic, clinical and health behavior correlates of sitting time in older adults. Bmc Public Health. 2015;15.
  44. 44. Kikuchi H, Inoue S, Sugiyama T, Owen N, Oka K, Shimomitsu T. Correlates of prolonged television viewing time in older Japanese men and women. Bmc Public Health. 2013;13.
  45. 45. Gardiner PA, Eakin EG, Healy GN, Owen N. Feasibility of Reducing Older Adults' Sedentary Time. Am J Prev Med. 2011;41(2):174–7. pmid:21767725
  46. 46. Rovniak LS, Denlinger L, Duveneck E, Sciamanna CN, Kong L, Freivalds A et al. Feasibility of using a compact elliptical device to increase energy expenditure during sedentary activities. Journal of science and medicine in sport / Sports Medicine Australia. 2014;17(4):376–80.
  47. 47. Uijtdewilligen L, Singh AS, Chinapaw MJ, Twisk JW, van Mechelen W. Person-related determinants of TV viewing and computer time in a cohort of young Dutch adults: Who sits the most? Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2014.
  48. 48. Ajzen I. The Theory of Planned Behavior. Organ Behav Hum Dec. 1991;50(2):179–211.
  49. 49. Keevil VL, Wijndaele K, Luben R, Sayer AA, Wareham NJ, Khaw KT. Television Viewing, Walking Speed, and Grip Strength in a Prospective Cohort Study. Med Sci Sport Exer. 2015;47(4):735–42.
  50. 50. Bann D, Hire D, Manini T, Cooper R, Botoseneanu A, McDermott MM et al. Light Intensity Physical Activity and Sedentary Behavior in Relation to Body Mass Index and Grip Strength in Older Adults: Cross-Sectional Findings from the Lifestyle Interventions and Independence for Elders (LIFE) Study. Plos One. 2015;10(2):e0116058. pmid:25647685
  51. 51. Hamer M, Stamatakis E. Screen-based sedentary behavior, physical activity, and muscle strength in the English longitudinal study of ageing. Plos One. 2013;8(6):e66222. pmid:23755302
  52. 52. Rosenberg DE, Bellettiere J, Gardiner PA, Villarreal VN, Crist K, Kerr J. Independent Associations Between Sedentary Behaviors and Mental, Cognitive, Physical, and Functional Health Among Older Adults in Retirement Communities. The journals of gerontology Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences. 2015.
  53. 53. Scientific Institute of Public Health, Operational Direction Public Health and Surveillance (HISIA: Belgian Health Interview Survey—Interactive Analysis). https://hisia.wiv-isp.be/SitePages/Home.aspx.