Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Distribution and Abundance of Glucocorticoid and Mineralocorticoid Receptors throughout the Brain of the Great Tit (Parus major)

  • Rebecca A. Senft ,

    senftrebecca@gmail.com

    Affiliation Department of Biology, Swarthmore College, Swarthmore, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • Simone L. Meddle,

    Affiliation The Roslin Institute, The Royal (Dick) School of Veterinary Studies, The University of Edinburgh, Easter Bush, Midlothian, EH25 9RG, United Kingdom

  • Alexander T. Baugh

    Affiliation Department of Biology, Swarthmore College, Swarthmore, Pennsylvania, United States of America

Abstract

The glucocorticoid stress response, regulated by the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, enables individuals to cope with stressors through transcriptional effects in cells expressing the appropriate receptors. The two receptors that bind glucocorticoids—the mineralocorticoid receptor (MR) and glucocorticoid receptor (GR)—are present in a variety of vertebrate tissues, but their expression in the brain is especially important. Neural receptor patterns have the potential to integrate multiple behavioral and physiological traits simultaneously, including self-regulation of glucocorticoid secretion through negative feedback processes. In the present work, we quantified the expression of GR and MR mRNA throughout the brain of a female great tit (Parus major), creating a distribution map encompassing 48 regions. This map, the first of its kind for P. major, demonstrated a widespread but not ubiquitous distribution of both receptor types. In the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVN) and the hippocampus (HP)—the two brain regions that we sampled from a total of 25 birds, we found high GR mRNA expression in the former and, unexpectedly, low MR mRNA in the latter. We examined the covariation of MR and GR levels in these two regions and found a strong, positive relationship between MR in the PVN and MR in the HP and a similar trend for GR across these two regions. This correlation supports the idea that hormone pleiotropy may constrain an individual’s behavioral and physiological phenotype. In the female song system, we found moderate GR in hyperstriatum ventrale, pars caudalis (HVC), and moderate MR in robust nucleus of the arcopallium (RA). Understanding intra- and interspecific patterns of glucocorticoid receptor expression can inform us about the behavioral processes (e.g. song learning) that may be sensitive to stress and stimulate future hypotheses concerning the relationships between receptor expression, circulating hormone concentrations and performance traits under selection, including behavior.

Introduction

The acute glucocorticoid stress response, regulated by the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, enables individuals to cope with challenges encountered in everyday life. The HPA axis facilitates responses through various metabolic changes, including mobilization of glucose to muscles and suppressing long-term processes such as reproduction and growth [1]. These metabolic changes are accomplished through glucocorticoid receptor activation. Although there are both intracellular and membrane-bound receptors, most studies have focused on the role of intracellular mineralocorticoid receptors (MR) and glucocorticoid receptors (GR).

Differential activation of these two receptors among the many tissues expressing them provides modular regulatory control over metabolic processes by altering transcription in the target cells [1,2]. Glucocorticoids (GCs; cortisol in humans, many mammals, and fish; corticosterone in rodents, birds, amphibians; [3]) are the primary endocrine product of the HPA axis and are secreted by the adrenal cortices into general circulation before crossing the cell membrane and binding to the hormone-binding domains of MR and GR receptors in target cells. The receptor-ligand complex then enters the nucleus, homodimerizes, binds to hormone response elements specific to the receptor’s DNA-binding domain, and initiates transcription of target genes. The complete network of genes whose transcription is dependent on GR or MR activation is largely unknown, though a recent study examining GR target genes in mouse liver has identified a large regulatory map with over 300 GR-bound promoters, including genes for other transcriptional regulators and metabolic enzymes [4]. In addition, activity of MR and GR regulates the negative feedback processes of the axis itself, principally by binding to GR in the hypothalamus and pituitary, thereby inhibiting the secretagogues that lead to further elevations in glucocorticoid concentrations [57]. The diverse regulatory network for GR and MR, which is tissue and cell type dependent, is consistent with the wide variety of effects triggered by glucocorticoid release [1]. To better understand this complex regulatory system, an understanding of the distribution and abundance of these receptors across various tissues and cells is necessary. Moreover, because of its upstream location in the HPA axis, receptor expression in the brain has the potential to explain variation, intra- and inter-specifically, in stress physiology. In particular, the hypothalamus and hippocampus (hereafter HP) are two key regions because of their involvement in HPA regulation and roles in mediating diverse behaviors [3].

A handful of published distribution maps have documented MR and GR expression through in situ hybridization in a few species [814]. In songbirds, however, these maps are relatively limited in scale and resolution, exploring only a few regions and largely reporting only presence or absence of MR or GR or both. The mineralocorticoid receptor is reported to be localized to several discrete regions, most notably in HP [810]. In one study, MR was abundant in song-related regions such as the lateral part of the magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium (LMAN) and the robust nucleus of the arcopallium (RA) [14]. Glucocorticoid receptors are known to be more widely distributed, and are known to be abundant in the cerebellum, HP and paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (hereafter PVN; [810]).

The great tit (Parus major) has served as an important model organism in behavioral ecology, particularly in animal personality research [1517] and animal communication [1820]. The HPA axis is known to modulate behavior in songbirds generally (e.g. locomotor [21]; vocal behavior [22]) and thought to contribute to the mechanistic basis of animal personality in this species specifically [2325]). Further development of this species as a model for mechanistic studies of natural behavior could be facilitated by a better understanding of key neuroendocrine traits, including stress hormone receptors. Toward that aim, we quantified MR and GR abundances across 48 regions of interest, including several major song system nuclei (e.g. RA, LMAN, hyperstriatum ventrale, pars caudalis [HVC]).

Although a description of the neural distribution and abundance of MR and GR is itself a useful contribution towards generating hypotheses concerning the behavioral effects of glucocorticoids, it is also of interest as a means of understanding how a hormone may coordinate activity across different regions of the brain. The simultaneous and diverse effects of steroid hormones may connect traits in an individual’s phenotype, liberating or constraining their independence evolution, an idea referred to as hormonal pleiotropy [2429]. Here we make an initial foray into this question by testing how the expression of MR and GR in two regions, the HP and PVN, covary across a sample of 25 individuals. This line of work, especially if expanded to other tissues throughout the body [30], has the potential to illuminate neuroendocrine mechanisms of phenotypic integration and constraint [24,25,31,32].

Methods

Animals

Protocols used in the present study were approved under permit 35–9185.81/G-10/76 by District administration Freiburg Department of Agriculture, Rural areas, Veterinary and Food Administration, Baden-Wuerttemberg, Germany at the Max Planck Inst. Ornithology, Radolfzell to Michaela Hau and Alexander Baugh. One female great tit (Parus major) was used to develop the MR/GR distribution map. This bird was one member of a cohort of twenty-eight great tits collected from a nestbox population in 2009 in Westerheide, NL and held in captivity at the Netherlands Institute for Ecology (Heteren, NL) after reaching nutritional independence. In November 2010, the birds were transported by van to the Max Planck Institute for Ornithology (Radolfzell, Germany). After two weeks of quarantine, they housed singly in outdoor aviaries (3 x 3 x 2 meters high) in alternating male-female adjacencies. This captivity facilitated control of the social environment (birds were housed singly) and the nutritional environment (a diet that including egg, mealworms, fresh vegetables and a custom bird food mixture was provided ad libitum). In total, 28 birds were collected for this study, but after the death of two and a tumor developed in the third, the sample size shrunk to 25 birds. The distribution map was created from one female and expression of GR and MR in HP and PVN was explored across 25 birds.

In situ hybridization

In December 2012 we collected the brain tissue. In less than three minutes from entering the aviary, each bird was captured by hand net and brains were removed by rapid decapitation and frozen on a sheet of sterile aluminum foil in direct contact with a block of dry ice. Brain tissue was transported on dry ice to the Roslin Institute at the University of Edinburgh and mounted with OCT (TissueTek). We completely sectioned the brain coronally with a cryostat (Bright OTF/AS), from caudal to rostral at 15 μm thickness. Sections were thaw-mounted onto polysine pretreated slides (four sections per slide), which were stored at -80°C. We chose a total of 14 slides from the individual creating the basis for the distribution map (7 GR, 7 MR, in pairs distributed throughout the brain) for in situ hybridization. In addition, four slides (2 GR, 2 MR) were process for each of the 24 remaining birds in the cohort.

The protocol used here was previously described [9,10]. Briefly, selected slides were brought to room temperature in sealed slide boxes, fixed with paraformaldehyde, rinsed with PBS and treated with a series of increasingly concentrated ethanol to dehydrate the tissue. After air drying, slides were incubated for two hours at 50°C in a prehybridization solution. Following prehybridization, slides were incubated overnight (16–18 h) with S35-labeled radioactive sense or antisense riboprobes for zebra finch GR or MR [9,10,33] at 55°C. These probes have proven successful with other songbird species including the white-crowned sparrow [33]. Slides were then washed three times in saline-sodium citrate (SSC) for 5 minutes each and subsequently incubated with RNAse-A for one hour at 37°C. This was followed by SSC washes and ethanol dehydration. Slides were then dipped in autoradiographic emulsion and air dried and left to expose in sealed boxes at 4°C for 5 weeks before developing and counterstaining with haematoxylin and eosin. Finally, slides were dehydrated and coverslipped with DPX mountant (Sigma, St. Louis, MO, USA). Slides incubated with sense probes showed no detectable hybridization signal.

Image Collection

One MR distribution map slide had an excessive amount of silver grain expression from a mistake when coverslipping and thus was not photographed or included in subsequent analyses. For each section, we identified regions of interest (ROIs) using the Stereotaxic Canary Atlas and Zebra Finch Atlas [34,35]. We photographed one frame near the center of these ROIs under transmitted bright-field illumination (Zeiss Akioskop and SPOT camera software) at 40X magnification [9]. Additionally, in order to account for nonspecific binding, one off-tissue background frame was captured for each ROI by panning the field of view to the left or right until off tissue [12]. For the distribution map, we took one background image per section. In photographing, light levels were consistent across all samples such that silver grains achieved maximum contrast with the background tissue. We also consistently used the auto-exposure setting.

Quantification

Images were analyzed for silver grain expression in ImageJ (NIH). A custom macro utilized image segmentation to measure area (number of pixels) of silver grains superimposed over cell bodies (Fig 1). From this area, the macro subtracted area of silver grains from a background, off-tissue image in order to control for non-specific binding [12]. This area was then divided by the area of cell bodies, as number of cells differed across regions. The resultant measure is one of relative silver grain expression: area of silver grain expression over cells not attributable to nonspecific binding divided by the total area of cell bodies.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Image segmentation in Image J.

A custom macro segmented images of silver grains over cell bodies into separate cell body (A, top) and silver grain (A, bottom) binary images. Expression was calculated as the pixels of silver grains superimposed over cell bodies subtracting a background level of silver grains all divided by the cell body area (B). This allowed us to control for variation in the number of cell bodies present in different regions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0148516.g001

We binned these values into four categories: zero, low, medium, and high expression. We divided the range of positive silver grain densities into three equal partitions with the fourth category representing expression at or below background levels. This method does not directly compare MR and GR expression to each other, as differences in probe sequence and binding efficacy may affect the precise relationship between silver grains present and actual number of mRNA molecules [36]. Thus we binned mRNA levels for each receptor based on the range of values for that receptor. Finally, the high category was assigned to regions with greater SG than the maximum of the medium range (max expression for any given area was 0.1088 in MR and 0.1503 in GR). Expression level is indicated in the map by size of dot (larger corresponding to higher levels of mRNA).

As a framework for our distribution map, we followed the basic layout used in the Canary Atlas [34]. Using Adobe Illustrator (CS5) ® we custom illustrated the layout of each plate based on our counterstained great tit tissue sections. All representative tissue images for the distribution map were adjusted for contrast, brightness, and hue in Adobe Photoshop (CS5).

Results

Throughout the brain of a female great tit, we were able to sample MR and GR mRNA from 48 unique regions corresponding to six plates throughout the brain (Figs 27). These were roughly plates 6, 12, 17, 21, 28, and 30 in the stereotaxic canary atlas and the following ranges of plates in the zebra finch atlas: 10–11, 19, 25–26, 29–30, 35, 37–39 [34,35]. Because we sampled exhaustively from a single individual, it is necessary to be conservative in the generalizability of the receptor expression data. However, other distribution maps have used small numbers of individuals [37,38]. Additionally, we were able to sample HP and PVN across all 25 individuals and for these regions, the bird chosen for our distribution map is representative of the cohort (Table 1). For representative images of key regions, see Fig 8.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Plate 6 (A4.23–4.41 in the zebra finch atlas).

Legend (left) and map (right). GR (blue, left) and MR (red, right) expression is indicated by dot size. An “X” indicates that the background expression met or exceeded the expression in the region. Area abbreviations can be found in Table 2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0148516.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Plate 12 (~A2.97 in the zebra finch atlas).

Legend (left) and map (right). GR (blue, left) and MR (red, right) expression is indicated by dot size. An “X” indicates that the background expression met or exceeded the expression in the region. Area abbreviations can be found in Table 2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0148516.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. Plate 17 (A2.07–2.25 in the zebra finch atlas).

Legend (left) and map (right). GR (blue, left) and MR (red, right) expression is indicated by dot size. An “X” indicates that the background expression met or exceeded the expression in the region. Area abbreviations can be found in Table 2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0148516.g004

thumbnail
Fig 5. Plate 21 (A1.53–1.71 in the zebra finch atlas).

Legend (left) and map (right). GR (blue, left) and MR (red, right) expression is indicated by dot size. An “X” indicates that the background expression met or exceeded the expression in the region. Area abbreviations can be found in Table 2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0148516.g005

thumbnail
Fig 6. Plate 28 (A0.36 in the zebra finch atlas).

Legend (left) and map (right). GR (blue, left) and MR (red, right) expression is indicated by dot size. An “X” indicates that the background expression met or exceeded the expression in the region. Area abbreviations can be found in Table 2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0148516.g006

thumbnail
Fig 7. Plate 30 (A0.18–0.00 in the zebra finch atlas).

Legend (left) and map (right). GR (blue, left) and MR (red, right) expression is indicated by dot size. An “X” indicates that the background expression met or exceeded the expression in the region. Area abbreviations can be found in Table 2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0148516.g007

thumbnail
Fig 8. Representative photopictomicrographs of MR and GR expression.

Cerebellum (Cb), paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVN), hippocampus (HP), HVC, and nucleus robustus arcopallii (RA). Cell bodies are dark purple, silver grains are black dots. Scale bar indicates 50 μm.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0148516.g008

thumbnail
Table 1. Summary of average MR and GR expression in hippocampus (HP; n = 25) and paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVN; n = 23) across all great tits and in the individual used for the distribution map (DM bird).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0148516.t001

We found the highest GR mRNA expression, in descending order, in the cerebellum, locus coeruleus, nidopallium, apical and densocellular hyperpallium, and paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus. The highest MR, in descending order, was found in the nucleus septalis medialis, nucleus rotundus, cerebellum, trochlear nerve, and parahippocampal area. The lowest GR expression was found in the optic tectum, striatum laterale, and optic tract with no expression found in the optic tectum. For MR, low to no mRNA expression was found in the anterior commissure, optic tract, paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus, lateral part of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, and nucleus tuberis. These values describe lowest and highest sampled regions throughout the plates. For average expression across all plates, see Table 2.

thumbnail
Table 2. Summary of average GR and MR mRNA expression per region (−, +, ++, +++).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0148516.t002

Song regions

We sampled MR and GR mRNA from five nuclei of the song system (1–4 sections per nuclei): HVC, NIf, LMAN, RA, and Area X. Regarding average expression, HVC expressed moderate GR and low MR; RA expressed low GR but moderate MR; and, LMAN, NIf, and Area X expressed low GR and low MR.

Comparison across species

We compared our results to previous distribution maps of MR and GR in other songbird species: the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris), chukar (Alectoris chukar), zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata), and Bengalese finch (Lonchura striata domestica) (Table 3). Because previous distribution maps in these species did not specify relative expression values, we have indicated whether expression was present (P) or absent (A) for those species. For L. domestica, male and female expression was assessed separately.

Consistent with previous studies, we noted high expression of GR in the PVN and the presence of both MR and GR in HP. Unlike in these other species, however, we found relatively low MR in HP. Moreover, we found that MR and GR are present in the cerebellum optic tectum respectively, two regions that have not been reported previously to express these receptors; we note, however, that this apparent species difference could be a sex difference.

MR and GR expression in the PVN and HP across individuals.

For each of twenty-five birds, we assessed MR and GR expression in HP (n = 25) and PVN (n = 23). Average expression and standard deviations, as well as the values for the individual used in the distribution map, are reported in Table 1. There was no effect of sex on expression of MR or GR in either region (all Z<0.9, all p>0.37; Kruskal-Wallis test). We also created linear regressions to examine how receptor expression is correlated across and within regions. We tested all six combinations with MR or GR in the PVN or HP (Fig 9). Expression of MR across these two nuclei was strongly and significantly correlated (R2 = 0.59, F(1,21) = 30.73, β = 0.65, p<0.0001*; Fig 9A). Similarly, there was a trend towards GR expression being correlated across these two nuclei (R2 = 0.15, F(1,21) = 3.63, β = 0.30, p = 0.069; Fig 9B). All others were nonsignificant (all p>0.05) and are thus not shown in Fig 9.

thumbnail
Fig 9. Expression of GR (A) and MR (B) regressions across HP and PVN.

Units are area of silver grains superimposed over cell bodies minus background silver grain area divided by cell body area (see methods). Trendline and tests performed with log-transformed data, though untransformed data are shown. MR levels in HP significantly predicted MR levels in PVN (R2 = 0.5941, p<0.0001). GR levels in HP demonstrated a trend toward predicting GR levels in the PVN (R2 = 0.15, p = 0.069). No other combinations of areas and receptors were significant (all p>0.05).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0148516.g009

Discussion

The distribution and abundance of hormone receptors, even in model organisms, is poorly understood. This is in contrast the surplus of studies that have focused on circulating concentrations of hormone. Understanding which tissues, and in the case of the nervous system, which regions express hormone receptors provides a critical starting point for generating hypotheses about the endocrine and behavioral effects of the hormone of interest. Moreover, even closely-related species may have different patterns of receptor expression [13], which may help explain inter-specific variation in other traits and illustrates the general need for more studies that focus on receptors. Finally, understanding the extent to which steroid hormone receptor expression is coordinated across brain regions has the potential to reveal a mechanism facilitating the functional integration of multiple phenotypic traits (e.g. personality suites; [1517]) and constrain phenotypic independence. For example, recent work has shown that the binding of glucocorticoids to GRs is necessary for maintaining a correlation between immune function and exploratory behavior [39]. Syncing receptor expression in different neural regions could allow glucocorticoid release to simultaneously affect multiple traits, resulting in the development of behavioral syndromes.

The distribution map reported here is one of the most anatomically comprehensive and resolved available for a songbird and is largely congruent with other songbird maps for these receptors. We found the highest GR mRNA expression in the cerebellum, where it is thought to play an important role in synthesis of leukotrienes (inflammatory agents found in immune cells) [40]. High expression levels have also been reported for other songbird species [10]. We also found high GR in the PVN, where it regulates HPA axis activity, and in the nidopallium, where its function has not yet been determined (Table 2). In contrast to a previous songbird map reporting no GR in the optic tectum [9], we found low, but detectable, expression in P. major.

MR was highest in the septalis medialis, nucleus rotundus, cerebellum, trochlear nerve, and parahippocampal area (Table 2). These regions were not examined in previous studies, though possible functions of MR can be hypothesized. For example, the septalis medialis is reported to be involved in sleep, particularly in the production of theta waves and slow wave sleep in mammals [41]. Because previous research has demonstrated homology between these regions in mammals and birds [42], MR expression could potentially play a role in regulation of sleep-wake states [43,44].

We also detected moderate MR mRNA expression in the cerebellum, and it is possible that MR may also play a similar role to GR in the cerebellum in regulating leukotriene synthesis, though future work will need to examine other species and better characterize MR function by identifying the specific DNA response elements to which MR binds.

Surprisingly, we found low MR expression in HP. This finding contrasts with a number of other songbird distribution maps [810], and may reflect inter-specific variation. Distribution maps on members of the primate family have found differences in mRNA levels in HP and other regions among closely-related species such as macaques and squirrel monkeys [13]. Again, such interspecific variation highlights the importance of creating distribution maps for each focal species.

Across our sample of birds, we found a strong positive correlation between MR expression in the PVN and HP. Expression of GR showed a similar, though nonsignificant trend between these two regions. This extends previous work [30], which examined receptor expression across many tissues throughout the body, by looking at specific regions of the brain known to be critical in regulating the HPA axis. Unlike this previous study [30], which found relatively weak correlations among different regions of the body, we found a strong correlation among brain regions. Birds with high MR in PVN also had high MR in HP. Such correlations might derive from these brain regions sharing a similar developmental program to each other compared with more disparate regions throughout the body [45]. Additionally, similar expression patterns could result from the anatomical and functional connectivity between the HP and PVN, as HP input to the PVN regulates the stress response by reducing its duration [46]. Future work should explore other regions identified by the distribution map here to explore how widespread such correlations are, and to assess if one of the receptors (e.g. MR) is generally more coordinated than the other. Such finding would provide support for the idea that neuroendocrine mediated hormonal pleiotropy integrates and coordinates phenotypic traits [2429].

MR and GR in the female song system

The song system in females is greatly underexplored compared to males [10]. It is a common assumption that female songbirds do not often sing in the wild and little work is done to explore the mechanisms and function of female song. However, in some songbirds, such as white-crowned sparrows, females do sing and this event is not rare in the wild [47]. A recent study of over 300 songbird species estimated that females sing in 71% of the studied species [48]. In addition, the authors showed that not only is female song common, it is the ancestral character state. Although great tits and Passerida were excluded from the study, female great tits are known to vocally interact with males during dawn chorus [49].

In addition to a potential role in production of female songs or calls, the song system also appears to be important in song perception. Intact female great tits can discriminate among conspecifics by song alone [50,51]. Lesioning of song system regions (e.g. HVC), however, renders female songbirds unable to discriminate between heterospecific and conspecific song, or between two different conspecific songs [52,53]. Further, the number of neurons and the volume of song system nuclei correlated positively with the selectivity of copulation solicitation displays in female brown-headed cowbirds (Molothrus ater) [54]. The discriminatory abilities conferred by the song system are also essential for maintaining social systems. In M. ater, inactivation of the song system led to decreased preference for high quality conspecific song, causing weaker pair bonds and in turn greater instability of the entire cowbird social network [55].

At present, a few studies have explored the effects of stress on female song recognition and discrimination [5658], with effects that appear to vary depending on the type of stressor. For example, an early-life unpredictable food stressor reduced female preference for conspecific song in European starlings [57]. Additionally, the mechanism by which stress modulates these behavioral (and associated physiological) changes remains unknown. Our results suggest, however, that this would be a fruitful avenue for future research given the strong presence of GR in HVC and MR in RA. One important next step is to describe the developmental timelines of receptor expression [59], in conjunction with circulating concentrations of glucocorticoids [60]. The influence of early life stress (e.g., nutritional stress; [61]) and sex differences on developmental programming of the HPA axis could be examined in the context of long term effects on vocal learning and auditory perception.

We found that most song nuclei expressed low levels of MR and GR with the exception of HVC, which expressed moderate GR and RA, which expressed moderate levels of MR. These findings suggest that these two regions may be particularly sensitive to circulating glucocorticoids. Specifically, HVC may be sensitive to the high concentrations that accompany the stress response, potentially impacting song memory and discrimination abilities in females under stress [5658,61]. Moderate levels of MR in the RA suggest that cells in this region may be particularly sensitive to glucocorticoids at low concentrations, as MR has a higher affinity than GR and is saturated at lower levels. Neurons from RA project onto motor neurons that then control the production of song by constricting the syringeal muscles in birds. Our distribution map suggests that stress-induced increases in glucocorticoids might impact neural activity thereby modulating song production. However, in both RA and HVC, the specific effects of activating MR or GR will depend on which response elements they target in these regions. Future work should aim to identify these response elements and their functional impacts.

In a study on male and female Bengalese finches, Suzuki et al. demonstrated dispersed, nonspecific GR expression and strongly clustered, song region-specific MR expression [14]. This is in contrast to the low receptor expression that we observed in the song system of great tits, which might indicate that these regions are relatively insensitive to changes in glucocorticoid concentrations. We recognize, however, that comparing among studies is challenging because receptor expression might exhibit seasonal variation. The size of song nuclei and glucocorticoid receptor expression, for example, are both known to vary seasonally, with the nadir of expression and song nuclei size both occurring in winter [62,63]. Brain tissue in our study was collected in late fall. It is largely unknown whether expression levels remain constant, either relatively or absolutely, among regions throughout the year or if there are region-specific seasonal variation. In house sparrows, MR expression in HP fluctuates seasonally, as did the relationship between MR and GR expression and secretion of CORT in response to a stressor [64]. This work suggests environmental factors may modulate the stress response through mechanisms other than adjusting MR and GR expression. The importance of such seasonal patterns in receptor expression and in the relationship between MR/GR expression and glucocorticoid secretion requires further comparative study. Lastly, future work should also aim to quantify protein levels [65], in order to examine the time-course and stability of receptor expression.

Conclusions

In the present study, we report the first distribution map of neural MR and GR RNA expression in great tits. We detected high expression of GR mRNA in the PVN and low MR mRNA in the HP. In the female song system, we found moderate GR in HVC, and moderate MR in RA. We explored covariation of MR and GR levels in the HP and PVN. We found a strong, positive relationship for MR in these two regions and a similar trend for GR, extending previous research on inter-tissue patterns of glucocorticoid receptor expression [30]. The present work expands our understanding of which brain regions may be sensitive to circulating glucocorticoids and serves as a basis for future studies exploring how stress sensitivity may influence behavior, including song production and perception.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Val Bishop, who assisted with cryosectioning and in situ hybridization at the Roslin Institute, and Kees van Oers at the Netherlands Institute for Ecology for providing the birds used in this study.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: ATB RAS. Performed the experiments: RAS SLM ATB. Analyzed the data: RAS. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: SLM ATB. Wrote the paper: RAS ATB.

References

  1. 1. Sapolsky RM, Romero LM, Munck AU. How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocr Rev. 2000;21: 55–89. pmid:10696570
  2. 2. Oakley RH, Cidlowski JA. The biology of the glucocorticoid receptor: New signaling mechanisms in health and disease. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2013;132: 1033–1044. pmid:24084075
  3. 3. Nelson RJ. An introduction to behavioral endocrinology. 4th ed. Sunderland: Sinauer Associates; 2005.
  4. 4. Phuc Le P, Friedman JR, Schug J, Brestelli JE, Parker JB, Bochkis IM, et al. Glucocorticoid Receptor-Dependent Gene Regulatory Networks. PLoS Genet. 2005;
  5. 5. De Kloet ER. Brain Corticosteroid Receptor Balance and Homeostatic Control. Front Neuroendocr. 1991;12: 95–164.
  6. 6. Romero ML. Physiological stress in ecology: lessons from biomedical research. Trends Ecol Evol. 2004;19: 249–255. pmid:16701264
  7. 7. Ronchi E, Spencer RL, Krey LC, McEwen BS. Effects of photoperiod on brain corticosteroid receptors and the stress response in the golden hamster (Mesocricetus auratus). Brain Res. 1998;780: 348–351. pmid:9507189
  8. 8. Dickens MJ, Meddle SL, Romero ML. Mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptor mRNA expression in the brain of translocated chukar (Alectoris chukar). Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2011;170: 569–574. pmid:21118689
  9. 9. Dickens M, Romero LM, Cyr NE, Dunn IC, Meddle SL. Chronic stress alters glucocorticoid receptor and mineralocorticoid receptor mRNA expression in the European starling (sturnus vulgaris) brain. J Neuroendocrinol. 2009;21: 832–840. pmid:19686439
  10. 10. Hodgson ZG, Meddle SL, Roberts ML, Buchanan KL, Evans MR, Metzdorf R, et al. Spatial ability is impaired and hippocampal mineralocorticoid receptor mRNA expression reduced in zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) selected for acute high corticosterone response to stress. Proc Biol Sci. 2007;274: 239–245. pmid:17148253
  11. 11. Matsunaga E, Okanoya K. Comparative Gene Expression Analysis Among Vocal Learners (Bengalese Finch and Budgerigar) and Non-Learners (Quail and Ring Dove) Reveals Variable Cadherin Expressions in the Vocal System. Front Neuroanat. 2011.
  12. 12. Patel PD, Lopez JF, Lyons DM, Burke S, Wallace M, Schatzberg AF. Glucocorticoid and mineralocorticoid receptor mRNA expression in squirrel monkey brain. J Psychiatr Res. 2000;34: 383–392. pmid:11165305
  13. 13. Pryce CR. Postnatal ontogeny of expression of the corticosteroid receptor genes in mammalian brains: Inter-species and intra-species differences. Brain Res Rev. 2008;57: 596–605. pmid:17916381
  14. 14. Suzuki K, Matsunaga E, Kobayashi T, Okanoya K. Expression patterns of mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptors in Bengalese finch (Lonchura striata var. domestica) brain suggest a relationship between stress hormones and song-system development. Neuroscience. 2011;194: 72–83. pmid:21851851
  15. 15. Aplin LM, Farine DR, Morand-Ferron J, Cole EF, Cockburn A, Sheldon BC. Individual personalities predict social behaviour in wild networks of great tits (Parus major). Ecol Lett. 2013;16: 1365–1372. pmid:24047530
  16. 16. Dingemanse NJ, Bouwman KM, van de Pol M, van Overveld T, Patrick SC, Matthysen E, Quinn JL. Variation in personality and behavioural plasticity across four populations of the great tit Parus major. J Anim Ecol. 2012;81: 116–126. pmid:21692798
  17. 17. Dingemanse NJ, Both C, Drent PJ, van Oers K, van Noordwijk AJ. Repeatability and heritability of exploratory behaviour in great tits from the wild. Anim Behav. 2002;64: 929–938.
  18. 18. Halfwerk W, Bot S, Buikx J, van der Velde M, Komdeur J, ten Cate C, Slabbekoorn H. Low-frequency songs lose their potency in noisy urban conditions. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2011;108: 14549–14554. pmid:21876157
  19. 19. Hamao S, Watanabe M, Mori Y. Urban noise and male density affect songs in the great tit Parus major. Ethol Ecol Evol. 2011;23: 111–119.
  20. 20. Mockford EJ, Marshall RC. Effects of urban noise on song and response behaviour in great tits. Proc Biol Sci. 2009;276: 2979–2985. pmid:19493902
  21. 21. Breuner CW, Greenberg AL, Wingfield JC. Noninvasive corticosterone treatment rapidly increases activity in Gambel’s white-crowned sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys gambelii). Gen Comp Endocrinol. 1998;111: 386–394. pmid:9707484
  22. 22. Wada H, Breuner CW. Transient elevation of corticosterone alters begging behavior and growth of white-crowned sparrow nestlings. J Exp Biol. 2008;211: 1696–1703. pmid:18456897
  23. 23. Cockrem JF. Stress, corticosterone responses and avian personalities. J Ornithol. 2007;148: 169–178.
  24. 24. Baugh AT, Schaper SV, Hau M, Cockrem JF, de Goede P, van Oers K. Corticosterone responses differ between lines of great tits (Parus major) selected for divergent personalities. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2012;175: 488–494. pmid:22202603
  25. 25. Baugh AT, van Oers K, Naguib M, Hau M. Initial reactivity and magnitude of the acute stress response associated with personality in wild great tits (Parus major). Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2013;189: 96–104. pmid:23665102
  26. 26. Baugh AT, van Oers K, Dingemanse NJ, Hau M. Baseline and stress-induced glucocorticoid concentrations are not repeatable but covary within individual great tits (Parus major). Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2014;208: 154–163. pmid:25218170
  27. 27. Flatt T, Tu MP, Tatar M. Hormonal pleiotropy and the juvenile hormone regulation of Drosophila development and life history. BioEssays. 2005;27: 999–1010. pmid:16163709
  28. 28. Hau M. Regulation of male traits by testosterone: implications for the evolution of vertebrate life histories. BioEssays. 2007;29: 133–144. pmid:17226801
  29. 29. Williams TD. Individual variation in endocrine systems: moving beyond the “tyranny of the Golden Mean.” Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2008;363: 1687–1698. pmid:18048295
  30. 30. Lattin CR, Keniston DE, Reed JM, Romero LM. Are receptor concentrations correlated across tissues within individuals? A case study examining glucocorticoid and mineralocorticoid receptor binding. Endocrinol. 2015;156: 1354–1361.
  31. 31. Ducrest AL, Keller L, Roulin A. Pleiotropy in the melanocortin system, coloration and behavioural syndromes. Trends Ecol Evol. 2008;23: 502–510. pmid:18644658
  32. 32. Sih A, Bell A, Johnson JC. Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends Ecol Evol. 2004;19: 372–378. pmid:16701288
  33. 33. Krause JS, McGuigan MA, Bishop VR, Wingfield JC, Meddle SL. Decreases in mineralocorticoid but not glucocorticoid receptor mRNA expression during the short Arctic breeding season in free-living Gambel’s white-crowned sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys gambelii). J Neuroendocrinol. 2015;27: 66–75. pmid:25411901
  34. 34. Stokes TM, Leonard CM, Nottebohm F. The telencephalon, diencephalon, and mesencephalon of the canary, Serinus canaria, in stereotaxic coordinates. J Comp Neurol. 1974;156: 337–374. pmid:4609173
  35. 35. Nixdorf-Bergweiler BE, Bischof H. A Stereotaxic Atlas Of The Brain Of The Zebra Finch, Taeniopygia Guttata. National Center for Biotechnology Information (US); 2007.
  36. 36. Cole RL, Lechner SM, Williams ME, Prodanovich P, Bleicher L, Varney MA, et al. Differential distribution of voltage-gated calcium channel alpha-2 delta (α2δ) subunit mRNA-containing cells in the rat central nervous system and the dorsal root ganglia. J Comp Neurol. 2005;491: 246–269. pmid:16134135
  37. 37. Dumont Y, Jacques D, Bouchard P, Quirion R. Species differences in the expression and distribution of the neuropeptide Y Y1, Y2, Y4, and Y5 receptors in rodents, guinea pig, and primates brains. J Comp Neurol. 1998;402: 372–384. pmid:9853905
  38. 38. Heppner KM, Kirigiti M, Secher A, Paulsen SJ, Buckingham R, Pyke C, et al. Expression and distribution of glucagon-like peptide-1 receptor mRNA, protein and binding in the male nonhuman primate (Macaca mulatta) brain. Endocrinol. 2015;156: 255–267.
  39. 39. Dosmann A, Brooks KC, Mateo JM. Evidence for a mechanism of phenotypic integration of behaviour and innate immunity in a wild rodent: implications for animal personality and ecological immunology. Anim Behav. 2015;101: 179–189.
  40. 40. Uz T, Dwivedi Y, Qeli A, Peters-Golden M, Pandey G, Manev H. Glucocorticoid receptors are required for up-regulation of neuronal 5-lipoxygenase (5LOX) expression by dexamethasone. FASEB J. 2001;15: 1792–1794. pmid:11481232
  41. 41. Srividya R, Mallick HN, Kumar VM. Sleep changes produced by destruction of medial septal neurons in rats. Neuroreport. 2004;15: 1831–1835. pmid:15257157
  42. 42. Butler AB, Hodos W. Comparative Vertebrate Neuroanatomy: Evolution and Adaptation. 2nd ed. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons; 2005.
  43. 43. Chung S, Son GH, Kim K. Circadian rhythm of adrenal glucocorticoid: its regulation and clinical implications. Biochim Biophys Acta. 2011;1812: 581–591. pmid:21320597
  44. 44. Besedovsky L, Linz B, Born J, Lange T. Mineralocorticoid receptor signaling reduces numbers of circulating human naïve T cells and increases their CD62L, CCR7, and CXCR4 expression. Eur J Immunol. 2014;44: 1759–1769. pmid:24595810
  45. 45. Stiles J, Jernigan TL. The Basics of Brain Development. Neuropsychol Rev. 2010;20: 327–348. pmid:21042938
  46. 46. Aguilera G, Liu Y. The Molecular Physiology of CRH Neurons. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2012;33: 67–84. pmid:21871477
  47. 47. Baptista LF, Trail PW, DeWolfe BB, Morton ML. Singing and its functions in female white-crowned sparrows. Anim Behav. 1993;46: 511–524.
  48. 48. Odom KJ, Hall ML, Riebel K, Omland KE, Langmore NE. Female song is widespread and ancestral in songbirds. Nat Commun. 2014;5: 3379. pmid:24594930
  49. 49. Gorissen L, Eens M. Interactive communication between male and female great tits (Parus major) during the dawn chorus. The Auk. 2004;121: 184–191.
  50. 50. Blumenrath SH, Dabelsteen T, Pedersen SB. Vocal neighbour—mate discrimination in female great tits despite high song similarity. Anim Behav. 2007;73: 789–796.
  51. 51. Lind H, Dabelsteen T, McGregor PK. Female great tits can identify mates by song. Anim Behav. 1996;52: 667–671.
  52. 52. Brenowitz EA. Altered perception of species-specific song by female birds after lesions of a forebrain nucleus. Science. 1991;251: 303–305. pmid:1987645
  53. 53. Del Negro C, Gahr M, Leboucher G, Kreutzer M. The selectivity of sexual responses to song displays: effects of partial chemical lesion of the HVC in female canaries. Behav Brain Res. 1998;96: 151–159. pmid:9821551
  54. 54. Hamilton KS, King AP, Sengelaub DR, West MJ. A brain of her own: a neural correlate of song assessment in a female songbird. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 1997;68: 325–332. pmid:9398592
  55. 55. Maguire SE, Schmidt MF, White DJ. Social Brains in Context: Lesions Targeted to the Song Control System in Female Cowbirds Affect Their Social Network. PLoS ONE. 2013.
  56. 56. Riebel K, Naguib M, Gil D. Experimental manipulation of the rearing environment influences adult female zebra finch song preferences. Anim Behav. 2009;78: 1397–1404.
  57. 57. Farrell TM, Neuert MAC, Cui A, MacDougall-Shackleton SA. Developmental stress impairs a female songbird’s behavioural and neural response to a sexually selected signal. Anim Behav. 2015;102: 157–167.
  58. 58. Schmidt KL, McCallum ES, MacDougall-Shackleton EA, MacDougall-Shackleton SA. Early-life stress affects the behavioural and neural response of female song sparrows to conspecific song. Anim Behav. 2013;85: 825–837.
  59. 59. Wada H, Breuner CW. Developmental changes in neural corticosteroid receptor binding capacity in altricial nestlings. Dev Neurobiol. 2010;70: 853–861. pmid:20629050
  60. 60. Wada H, Salvante KG, Wagner E, Williams TD, Breuner CW. Ontogeny and individual variation in the adrenocortical response of zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata) nestlings. Physiol Biochem Zool. 2009;82: 325–331. pmid:19426109
  61. 61. Kriengwatana B, Wada H, Schmidt KL, Taves MD, Soma KK, MacDougall-Shackleton SA. Effects of nutritional stress during different developmental periods on song and the hypothalamic—pituitary—adrenal axis in zebra finches. Horm Behav. 2014;65: 285–293. pmid:24417905
  62. 62. Breuner CW, Orchinik M. Seasonal Regulation of Membrane and Intracellular Corticosteroid Receptors in the House Sparrow Brain. J Neuroendocrinol. 2001;13: 412–420. pmid:11328450
  63. 63. Deviche P, Gulledge CC. Vocal control region sizes of an adult female songbird change seasonally in the absence of detectable circulating testosterone concentrations. J Neurobiol. 2000;42: 202–211. pmid:10640327
  64. 64. Liebl AL, Shimizu T, Martin LB. Covariation among glucocorticoid regulatory elements varies seasonally in house sparrows. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2013;183: 32–37. pmid:23262276
  65. 65. Medina CO, Lattin CR, McVey M, Romero LM. There is no correlation between glucocorticoid receptor mRNA expression and protein binding in the brains of house sparrows (Passer domesticus). Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2013;193: 27–36. pmid:23892014