Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Informal Child Care and Adolescent Psychological Well-Being: Hong Kong’s “Children of 1997” Birth Cohort

  • Cherry Y. Leung,

    Current address: Department of Community Medicine and School of Public Health, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong.

    Affiliation School of Public Health, Li Ka Shing Faculty of Medicine, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, People’s Republic of China

  • Gabriel M. Leung,

    Current address: Department of Community Medicine and School of Public Health, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong.

    Affiliation School of Public Health, Li Ka Shing Faculty of Medicine, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, People’s Republic of China

  • C. Mary Schooling

    cms1@hkucc.hku.hk

    Current address: Department of Community Medicine and School of Public Health, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong.

    Affiliations School of Public Health, Li Ka Shing Faculty of Medicine, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, People’s Republic of China, CUNY School of Public Health and Hunter College, New York, United States of America

Abstract

Background

Informal child care (child care by untrained family members, relatives or employees in the home) in Western populations is often associated with poorer psychological well-being, which may be confounded by socioeconomic position. We examined the association of informal child care, common in non-Western settings, with adolescent psychological well-being, using Hong Kong’s Chinese “Children of 1997” birth cohort.

Methods

Multivariable linear regression was used to examine the adjusted associations of informal child care (at 0.5, 3, 5 and 11 years) with parent-reported Rutter score for child behavior at 11 years, self-reported Culture-Free Self-Esteem Inventories score at 11 years and self-reported Patient Health Questionnaire-9 depressive symptom score at 13 years. Model comparisons were used to identify the best representation of child care, in terms of a critical period of exposure to informal child care (independent variable) at a specific age, combination of exposures to informal child care at several ages or an accumulation of exposures to informal child care.

Results

Child care was not associated with behavioral problems. A model considering child care at 3 years best represented the association of child care with self-esteem while a model considering child care at 5 years best represented the association of child care with depressive symptoms. Informal child care at 3 years was associated with lower self-esteem (-0.70, 95% confidence interval (CI) -1.26 to -0.14). Informal child care at 5 years was associated with more depressive symptoms (0.45, 95% CI 0.17 to 0.73).

Conclusion

In a developed non-Western setting, informal child care was associated with lower self-esteem and more depressive symptoms.

Introduction

The use of child care is increasing, especially with the surge of employment among women since the 1970s. In Western countries, such as the United States (U.S.), the likelihood of women working during their child’s infancy is the same as when they have an older preschooler [1]. Infancy and early childhood are a crucial age for neurological development. For example, the frontal cortex, where attention, behavior and judgment are controlled, matures most around 2 to 3.5 years [2,3]. Experiences in infancy and early childhood may have long-term impacts on cognition and psychological well-being [4,5]. Psychological well-being is integral to overall health and well-being and should be treated with the same priority as physical health [6,7]; 9.8% of adults are projected to be diagnosed with depression in high income countries by 2030 according to the World Health Organization (WHO) [8]. Poor adolescent psychological well-being, including low self-esteem and depressive symptoms, may presage depression in adulthood [9,10]. Globally, 20% of adolescents are diagnosed with mental illnesses [11], 10–15% with behavioral problems [12], 15–25% with low self-esteem [13,14] and 3–5% with depression [15].

Research into the association of child care with adolescent psychological well-being has been conducted from a number of perspectives mainly in Western settings. Most research has focused on investigating the role of maternal employment in behavior and mental health [1618]. Some research has examined the role of formal child care (child care by trained and untrained caregivers in school or a child care center) [17,19,20]. Other research has examined the role of any type of child care without distinguishing between formal and informal child care (child care by untrained family members, relatives or employees in the home) [2123] in Western settings. Only a few have examined informal care [24,25] as the sole exposure (or independent variable). Non-relative care (non-maternal care excluding grandparents and fathers) was associated with greater risk taking and impulsivity at age 15 [23] whereas maternal employment was found to be the main factor associated with poor cognitive and behavioral outcomes in early childhood [17]. One study concerning grandparent care found an association with greater hyperactivity and peer difficulties [25]. In the West, both informal child care and adolescent psychological well-being are socially patterned, such that low socioeconomic position (SEP) is associated with informal care [2628] and poorer psychological well-being [28,29]. Thus, it is unclear whether observed associations are the result of child care or due to residual confounding arising from an association of low SEP with both informal child care and poor psychological well-being which has not been completely accounted for in the analysis. Informal care is common for children under age 5, estimated at 33 to 53% in the U.S. [27] and 45 to 50% in Britain [30], yet there is limited research focusing on how informal care is related to adolescent psychological well-being.

While there is growing body of evidence about formal child care in Western populations, less consideration has been given to the informal child care common in the rest of the global population, such as Asia, where care is often sought from informal caregivers such as grandparents, uncles, aunts or in-home employed help (including caregivers living with the family) [31,32]. Hong Kong is an economically developed non-Western setting, with a similar social infrastructure and rates of mental illness (16.4% of Hong Kong adolescents are diagnosed with mental illness) [33] to Western Europe and North America but similar to the rest of the global population, especially that of Asia [34], in seeking informal child care. In addition, informal child care is not associated with low SEP in Hong Kong. As such, Hong Kong is an ideal setting to study informal child care because informal child care is common in Hong Kong. The unique attributes of our setting, Hong Kong, means that this study, taking advantage of a large population representative Hong Kong Chinese birth cohort “Children of 1997,” will clarify the role of informal child care at 6 months and 3, 5 and 11 years in psychological well-being in early adolescence (11 to 13 years).

Methods

Ethics statement

Since our participants are children, informed consent was obtained from the parents, next of kin, caretakers or guardians (informants) on behalf of the participants by completing the questionnaire at enrollment as approved by The University of Hong Kong Medical Faculty Ethics Committee. Ethical approval for further studies was obtained from the University of Hong Kong-Hospital Authority Hong Kong West Cluster, Joint Institutional Review Board and/or the Ethics Committee of the Department of Health, Government of the Hong Kong SAR as appropriate.

Participants

Hong Kong’s “Children of 1997” birth cohort is a population representative Chinese birth cohort (n = 8,327) that covered 88.0% of all births in Hong Kong from April 1, 1997 to May 31, 1997, and has been described in detail elsewhere [35]. The study was initially established to investigate the effects of secondhand smoke exposure on infant health. Families were recruited at the first postnatal visit to one of the 49 Maternal and Child Health Centres in Hong Kong, which parents of all newborns were encouraged to attend. Characteristics obtained using a self-administered questionnaire in Chinese at recruitment and subsequent routine visits included maternal and birth characteristics, parental education and early life exposures, such as breastfeeding. Passive follow-up via record linkage was instituted in 2005 to obtain routinely collected information including bi-annual assessments of emotional and behavioral problems using the Revised Parent’s Rutter Scales in Chinese [grade 2, 4 and 6 (i.e., ages 7–8, 9–10 and 11–12 years)] and self-esteem using Form A of the Culture-Free Self-Esteem Inventories for Children by Battle in Chinese [grade 4 (age 9–10 years) onwards] from the Student Health Service (SHS), Department of Health, which provides free annual check-ups for all school students. Active follow-up via direct contact was instituted in 2007, including postal surveys. Survey I, sent in July 2008, then re-sent a second and third time as necessary to non-respondents over the following 9 months, included questions on child care. Survey II, including the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9), was first sent in February 2010 then re-sent a second and third time as necessary over the following 5 months. Non-responses were followed-up via two waves of telephone interviews from November 2010 to April 2011 and from July 2011 to June 2012, and during pilot studies for in-person follow-up (June to August 2011 and June to August 2012). A summary of information collection regarding type of child care and measures of psychological well-being is shown in Table 1.

thumbnail
Table 1. Collection of Information Regarding Child Care and Psychological Well-Being in Hong Kong’s “Children of 1997 Birth Cohort”.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120116.t001

Informal child care exposure (independent variables)

Our primary exposures (independent variables) were exposure to informal child care at 6 months, 3 years, 5 years and 11 years, considered as separate exposures (independent variables) and as an accumulation of exposures (independent variables). Type of child care was obtained from this question in Survey I: ‘Who was your child’s main caregiver at the following different ages: 6 months, 3 years, 5 years, and now (11 years)?’ with responses specified as ‘parent’, ‘grandparent’, ‘domestic helper’ (in-home employed help) and ‘other.’ ‘Other’ included a fill-in-the-blank portion. Most ‘other’ responses were care by other relatives with very little formal care (daycare, nursery and crèche, n = 10 at 6 months and n = 12 at 3 months). Child care in our study was categorized in two ways. First, at each of 6 months and 3, 5 and 11 years, as parental if the child was cared for by either parent or informal if provided by grandparents, in-home employed help, or other relatives in the child’s own home. Those reported to have more than one main caregiver were categorized as informal because parental and informal care were mutually exclusive in this study. Second, informal was further categorized as grandparent, in-home employed help or other.

Outcomes

Emotional and behavioral problems

Emotional and behavioral problems from 9 to <13 years (mean age 11 years) were assessed from the Revised Parent’s Rutter Scales in Chinese for parents [36,37]. Cohen’s kappa to test for item agreement between English and Chinese was excellent (0.81) while the correlation for the scales were high (0.94) [37]; furthermore, the discriminative power for behavioral problems was good [36]. The scales consist of a set of 31 items describing emotional and behavioral difficulties, with each item scored 0 for does not apply, 1 for applies somewhat or 2 for certainly applies. A total score was calculated, where a higher score indicated more emotional and behavioral problems.

Self-esteem

Self-esteem at from 9 to <13 years (mean age 11 years) was assessed from the Form A of the Culture-Free Self Esteem Inventories (SEI) [38] in Chinese for children. The SEI correlates well with other widely used instruments, including Stanley Coopersmith’s Self Esteem Inventory (1967); correlations ranged from 0.72 to 0.93 among primary school students [39], as was found with a study on Chinese children in Hong Kong [40]. A total score (50 items), was calculated, where a lower score indicated lower self-esteem. We excluded unreliable scores with a lie subscale (of the SEI) of 2 or less (n = 158), although a lie subscale of 5 or more has been used to indicate a lack of defensiveness and a reliable self-esteem inventories [38].

Depressive symptoms

Depressive symptoms from 12 to ≤15 years (mean age 13 years) were assessed from the PHQ-9 [41] in Chinese as part of Survey II. The PHQ-9 scale has good sensitivity and specificity in detecting depression in youth [42], is an effective screening tool for the risk of depression among adolescents [43] and has been validated in Chinese adolescents [44]. The scale consists of a set of 9 items describing symptoms and functional impairment, with each item scored 0 for not at all, 1 for several days, 2 for more than half the days or 3 for nearly everyday. A total PHQ-9 score was calculated, where a higher score indicated more depressive symptoms.

Covariates

Potential confounders considered, as classified in Table 2, were sex, mother’s birthplace, highest parental education, highest parental occupation at birth, monthly household income per head at birth, maternal age at birth and parity. These were all self-reported at recruitment, with contradictory, missing or incomplete information clarified in subsequent data collection exercises. Specifically, information from subsequently collected information has been used to correct: 1) sex in a very few cases, where it appeared to be discrepant, 2) to obtain education in a few cases where it was missing from the baseline survey and 3) to improve the classification of mother’s birthplace because the question on the baseline survey was not entirely unambiguous.

thumbnail
Table 2. Characteristics of Respondents (n = 3,679) and Non-Respondents (n = 4,257) to Survey I in 2008–2009 of Hong Kong’s “Children of 1997 Birth Cohort” (Available Case Analysis).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120116.t002

Statistical analysis

We compared the characteristics of respondents and non-respondents to Survey I using Cohen effect sizes. Given exposures to child care are correlated and the role of child care at different ages is not well-defined, we used model comparisons, as in a previous study [45], to ascertain the best way of using the information about child care. We compared models representing child care in terms of: 1) a critical period (i.e., exposure at 6 months, 3 years, 5 years or 11 years), 2) independent exposures at each age considered together and 3) an accumulation of exposures (number of exposures to informal care treated as continuous). We used model fit, from the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), to compare these different representations of child care [46] with behavioral problems, self-esteem and depressive symptoms. A lower AIC indicates a better fitting model. By determining the best fitting model through using the AIC, we can determine whether exposure to informal child care at a certain critical period or at a combination of periods best fits the data. We then used multivariable linear regression to estimate the adjusted association of our independent variable, informal child care exposure, with Rutter, SEI, and depressive symptom scores from which single estimated beta-coefficients with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) are presented. The potential confounders, age of assessment and survey mode for depression were included in the models. PHQ-9 was obtained by postal questionnaire, telephone interview and in-person interview, hence the inclusion of survey mode in the relevant models. As a sensitivity analysis, we also adjusted for maternal depression at the time the participant was 13 years old, which was obtained by maternal self-report from Survey II.

Given that we have a population representative birth cohort with comprehensive baseline data, we used a combination of inverse probability weighting and multiple imputation [47]. Inverse probability weights were estimated using logistic regression with variables that could significantly predict the pattern of missingness for Survey I, including highest parental education, monthly household income per head, highest parental occupation, housing type, maternal age, maternal place of birth, parity and child’s sex, to account for potential differences between those who responded to Survey I and those who did not. We then used multiple imputation to predict missing values of the independent variables and confounders incorporating data on the outcomes (three behavioral and psychological instruments), independent variables, confounders and other factors potentially associated with child care. We summarized the results from 10 imputed datasets into single estimated beta-coefficients with CIs and AIC values adjusted for missing data uncertainty using the ‘Hmisc’ package in R 3.0.1 (R Development Care Team, Vienna, Austria). We also performed a complete case analysis, which excludes individuals with missing data from the analysis, for comparison using Stata version 10 (Stata Corp, College station, Texas).

Results

Of the original 8,327 cohort members, 26 (0.3%) had permanently withdrawn. Of the remaining 8,301, 7,933 were potentially contactable in 2010–2012. The follow-up rates for Rutter score for child behavior was 71% (n = 5,598) and Culture-Free Self-Esteem Inventory score was 87% (n = 6,937); the response rate for PHQ-9 depressive symptom score was 73% (n = 5,797).

Characteristics of respondents compared to non-respondents to Survey I are shown in Table 2. The respondents were slightly different from non-respondents, but the Cohen effect sizes were small (<0.2) [48]. Of the respondents, nearly 50% of participants had informal care at 6 months (49.7%) and 3 years (46.7%). Use of informal care was less common at older ages, 41.0% at 5 years and 23.9% at 11 years. Informal care at all ages was more common among children from families of higher SEP (assessed from parental education, household income per head or parental occupation), with fewer siblings and with Hong Kong born mothers (Table 3). Care by in-home employed help, rather than grandparents, was more common for higher SEP children. Care by in-home employed help, rather than grandparents, was more common at older ages, for children with fewer siblings and for children with Hong Kong born mothers.

thumbnail
Table 3. Baseline Characteristics by Child Care from Hong Kong’s “Children of 1997 Birth Cohort” (Available Case Analysis).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120116.t003

Table 4 shows the best fitting model for exposures to informal child care for each measure of psychological well-being, adjusted for sex, mother’s birthplace, highest parental education, highest parental occupation, monthly household income per head, maternal age at birth, parity, age of assessment and survey mode (PHQ-9 scores). Informal care at 11 years was the best fitting model for the Rutter score, informal care at 3 years was the best fitting model for the self-esteem score and informal care at 5 years was the best fitting model for the PHQ-9 score.

thumbnail
Table 4. AIC Values from Adjusteda Regression Models for Measures of Psychological Well-Being Using Different Representations of Child Care (After Inverse Probability Weighting with Multiple Imputation).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120116.t004

Based on the best fitting models that give the age at which child care had the most impact on each measure of psychological well-being, we further examined these specific associations of informal care with psychological well-being. Associations, adjusted for sex, mother’s birthplace, highest parental education, highest parental occupation, monthly household income per head, maternal age at birth, parity, age of assessment and survey mode (PHQ-9 scores), using the best fitting models are shown in Table 5. Informal care was not associated with the Rutter score. However, informal care at 3 years, compared to parental care, was associated with a lower self-esteem score. Informal care at 5 years was associated with a higher PHQ-9 score; this association remained after further adjustment for maternal depression when the child was about 13 years (data not shown).

thumbnail
Table 5. Adjusteda Association of Informal Care Compared to Parental Care with Psychological Well-Being (After Inverse Probability Weighting with Multiple Imputation).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120116.t005

To elucidate the role of the provider of informal care, we examined the association of different types of informal care with the Rutter score, self-esteem score and PHQ-9 score (S1 Table). Exclusive grandparent care, compared to parental care, at 11 years was associated with a lower Rutter score. A lower self-esteem score was more marked for care by in-home employed help and others at 3 years. Similarly, the association of informal care at 5 years with a higher PHQ-9 score was more marked for care by in-home employed help and others.

As a sensitivity analysis, we also presented results from a complete case analysis adjusted for the same set of confounders (S2 and S3 Tables). The findings were similar to the results from inverse probability weighting with multiple imputation.

Discussion

Using a large population representative birth cohort from the economically developed non-Western setting of Hong Kong where informal child care is common and associated with higher SEP, this study provides new evidence that informal child care, compared to parental care, during childhood is associated with some aspects of poorer adolescent psychological well-being. Informal child care, as compared to parental care, was not clearly associated with behavioral problems, although care by grandparents at 11 years was perhaps associated with better behavior. However, informal child care at 3 years was associated with lower self-esteem, mainly driven by care by in-home employed help, while informal care at 5 years was associated with more depressive symptoms, again mainly driven by exposure to in-home employed help.

Despite our population representative birth cohort, the study has limitations. First, the proportion of respondents for Survey I regarding child care was not high. While we performed a complete case analysis with similar results, our primary analysis employed a combination of inverse probability weighting with multiple imputation since complete case analysis requires more assumptions to be valid [47]. However, there are disadvantages with this method. Since inverse probability weighting only models the probability that an individual has complete data, multiple imputation is used to supplement this by modelling the joint distribution of missing data given the observed data [47]. When the percentage of missing data is high, multiple imputation of independent variables could bias towards the null while multiple imputation of confounders might generate residual confounding. Nonetheless, studies have shown that complete case analysis has more biased results [49]. Second, quality of child care, which may also be associated with child health outcomes [21], was not measured in our study nor was quantity of time spent with each particular caregiver. In Hong Kong, parents or grandparents can be present and overseeing care provided by in-home employed help. Third, maternal depression in infancy and toddlerhood are associated with child behavioral and adjustment problems, whether the depression is due to genetic transmission, observational learning or impaired parenting [50]. Maternal depression during infancy and toddlerhood was not measured. However, adjustment for maternal depression when the participant was 13 years made little difference (data not shown). Fourth, recall bias is a limitation. On the other hand, it is most likely non-differential, impairing precision rather than creating bias. Fifth, our study assessed emotional and behavioral problems, self-esteem and depressive symptoms using screening tools, which increases the chances of reporting bias especially when stereotypes and stigmas exist about mental health, instead of by clinical diagnosis. These questionnaires have been validated in Chinese populations as described in our methods [33,36,51]. Sixth, we used the AIC to compare models, which provides us with the best fitting model inferred from our data and is ideally used for large sample sizes as ours, but ultimately does not provide us with any information if a better model with care at other age groups or combination of age groups exists since it is not specified in our data [52]. Finally, given that formal child care is rarely used in Hong Kong, we could not assess the role of formal as compared to informal child care. Formal child care provided by professionals may have very different effects from informal child care.

There are several possible explanations for our observations. Informal caregivers may have less knowledge about promoting child development and psychological well-being because parents are the typical attendees at well-child primary care visits. Moreover, literacy may be an issue as employed helpers are usually from Southeast Asia. Informal caregivers may have less time to devote to the child as in-home employed caregivers are also usually responsible for running the household. Alternatively, the child may find it difficult to negotiate a number of different caregivers at an early age, given it may not be the same in-home employed help provider throughout childhood, with corresponding effects on self-esteem and vulnerability to depression. Bowlby’s ‘maternal deprivation’ hypothesis, that a child should only be attached to his/her mother during early life, has been disproved as this relationship can be compensated by an attachment to another primary figure [53]. However, while moderate shared caring of children is beneficial, negative immediate and long-term effects are seen when many adults are involved [53]. Better outcomes of psychological well-being as associated with consistent exposure to a particular caregiver, specifically parents and grandparents in our study, support this notion.

From a public health perspective, these results suggest that specific types of informal child care could have negative consequences on adolescent psychological well-being. Prevention could perhaps be achieved by interventions aimed at informal caregivers’ skills or alternatively at ensuring that informal caregivers have sufficient time and energy to devote to the child. Given the importance of psychological well-being and changing attitudes over time with the widespread use of informal care in non-Western settings, further investigation and experimentation could be worthwhile.

Conclusion

In a developed non-Western setting where informal child care is common and associated with higher SEP, informal child care, perhaps particularly by in-home employed help, is prospectively associated with lower self-esteem and higher depressive symptom scores in adolescence. As such, our study suggests early life exposures, particularly the provision of informal child care, may be associated with subsequent psychological well-being.

Supporting Information

S1 Table. Adjusteda Association of Informal Care by Provider Compared to Parental Care with Psychological Well-Being (After Inverse Probability Weighting with Multiple Imputation).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120116.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Adjusteda Association of Informal Care Compared to Parental Care with Psychological Well-Being (Complete Case Analysis).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120116.s002

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Adjusteda Association of Informal Care with Psychological Well-Being by Provider Compared to Parental Care (Complete Case Analysis).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120116.s003

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors thank colleagues at the Student Health Service and Family Health Service of the Department of Health for their assistance and collaboration. They also thank the late Dr. Connie O for coordinating the project and all the fieldwork for the initial study in 1997–8. C. Leung also thanks the University Grants Committee for funding her research through the Hong Kong PhD Fellowship Scheme.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: CMS. Performed the experiments: CYL. Analyzed the data: CYL. Wrote the paper: CYL CMS. Resurrected the birth cohort: GML CMS.

References

  1. 1. U.S. Department of Labor Bureau of Labor Statistics. Employment characteristics of families in 1999; 2000.
  2. 2. Huttenlocher PR, Dabholkar AS. Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. J Comp Neurol. 1997;387: 167–178. pmid:9336221
  3. 3. Rakic P, Bourgeois JP, Goldman-Rakic PS. Synaptic development of the cerebral cortex: implications for learning, memory, and mental illness. Prog Brain Res. 1994;102: 227–243. pmid:7800815
  4. 4. Kolb B, Forgie M, Gibb R, Gorny G, Rowntree S. Age, experience and the changing brain. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1998;22: 143–159. pmid:9579307
  5. 5. Dawson G, Ashman SB, Carver LJ. The role of early experience in shaping behavioral and brain development and its implications for social policy. Development and psychopathology. 2000;12: 695–712. pmid:11202040
  6. 6. Gabe M. Mental health: a report of the Surgeon General. Home Care Provid. 2000;5: 117. pmid:11982112
  7. 7. Department of Health and Human Services. President's New Freedom Commission on Mental Health. Achieving the promise: transforming mental health care in America. Rockville, MD, US: President's New Freedom Commission on Mental Health; 2003.
  8. 8. Mathers CD, Loncar D. Projections of global mortality and burden of disease from 2002 to 2030. PLoS Med. 2006;3: e442. pmid:17132052
  9. 9. Steiger AE, Allemand M, Robins RW, Fend HA. Low and decreasing self-esteem during adolescence predict adult depression two decades later. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2014;106: 325–338. pmid:24467425
  10. 10. Pine DS, Cohen E, Cohen P, Brook J. Adolescent depressive symptoms as predictors of adult depression: moodiness or mood disorder? Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156: 133–135. pmid:9892310
  11. 11. Belfer ML. Child and adolescent mental disorders: the magnitude of the problem across the globe. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49: 226–236. pmid:18221350
  12. 12. Aikins JW, Howes C, Hamilton C. Attachment stability and the emergence of unresolved representations during adolescence. Attach Hum Dev. 2009;11: 491–512. pmid:19946807
  13. 13. Hesketh K, Wake M, Waters E. Body mass index and parent-reported self-esteem in elementary school children: evidence for a causal relationship. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2004;28: 1233–1237. pmid:15314637
  14. 14. McClure AC, Tanski SE, Kingsbury J, Gerrard M, Sargent JD. Characteristics associated with low self-esteem among US adolescents. Acad Pediatr. 2010;10: 238–244 e232. pmid:20605547
  15. 15. Bhatia SK, Bhatia SC. Childhood and adolescent depression. American Academy of Family Physicians. 2007;74: 73–80.
  16. 16. Brooks-Gunn J, Han WJ, Waldfogel J. Maternal employment and child cognitive outcomes in the first three years of life: the NICHD study of early child care. Child Development. 2002;73: 1052–1071. pmid:12146733
  17. 17. Han WJ, Waldfogel J, Brooks-Gunn J. The effects of early maternal employment on later cognitive and behavioral outcomes. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2001;63: 336–354.
  18. 18. Greenstein TN. Maternal employment and child behavioral outcomes—a household economics analysis. Journal of Family Issues. 1993;14: 323–354.
  19. 19. Zachrisson HD, Dearing E, Lekhal R, Toppelberg CO. Little evidence that time in child care causes externalizing problems during early childhood in Norway. Child Development. 2013;84: 1152–1170. pmid:23311645
  20. 20. Lefebvre P, Merrigan P, Verstraete M. Childcare Policy and Cognitive Outcomes of Children: Results from a Large Scale Quasi-Experiment on Universal Childcare in Canada; 2008.
  21. 21. Belsky J, Vandell DL, Burchinal M, Clarke-Stewart KA, McCartney K, Owen MT. Are there long-term effects of early child care? Child Development. 2007;78: 681–701. pmid:17381797
  22. 22. Lamb MA. Nonparental child care: context, quality, correlates, and consequences. In: Damon W, Sigel IE, Renninger KA, editors. Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol 4 Child Psychology in Practice, 5th ed. New York: Wiley; 1998. pp.73–133.
  23. 23. Vandell DL, Belsky J, Burchinal M, Steinberg L, Vandergrift N. Do effects of early child care extend to age 15 years? Results from the NICHD study of early child care and youth development. Child Development. 2010;81: 737–756. pmid:20573102
  24. 24. Vandell DL, McCartney K, Owen MT, Booth C, Clarke-Stewart A. Variations in child care by grandparents during the first three years. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2003;65: 375–381.
  25. 25. Fergusson E, Maughan B, Golding J. Which children receive grandparental care and what effect does it have? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49: 161–169. pmid:17979960
  26. 26. Huston AC, Chang YE, Gennetian L. Family and individual predictors of child care use by low-income families in different policy contexts. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2002;17: 441–469.
  27. 27. Susman-Stillman A, Banghart, P. Demographics of Family, Friend, and Neighbor Child Care in the United States. National Center for Children of Povery at the Mailman School of Public Health, Columbia University and the Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan; 2008.
  28. 28. Richards M, Hatch SL. A Life Course Approach to the Development of Mental Skills. Journals of Gerontology Series B-Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2011;66: 26–35.
  29. 29. Elovainio M, Pulkki-Raback L, Jokela M, Kivimaki M, Hintsanen M, et al. Socioeconomic status and the development of depressive symptoms from childhood to adulthood: a longitudinal analysis across 27 years of follow-up in the Young Finns study. Social Science & Medicine. 2012;74: 923–929.
  30. 30. Bryson CMB, Sibieta L, Butt S. The role of informal childcare: a synthesis and critical review of the evidence—full report. Nuffield Foundation; 2010.
  31. 31. Chan AHN. Live-in Domestic Workers and Their Impact on Hong Kong’s Middle-Class Families. Journal of Family and Economic Issues. 2005;26: 20.
  32. 32. Yunus SA. Childcare Practices in Three Asian Countries. International Journal of Early Childhood. 2005;37: 39–56.
  33. 33. Leung PW, Hung SF, Ho TP, Lee CC, Liu WS, Tang CCP, et al. Prevalence of DSM-IV disorders in Chinese adolescents and the effects of an impairment criterion: a pilot community study in Hong Kong. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;17: 452–461. pmid:18427862
  34. 34. Experiencing Grandparenthood: An Asian Perspective; Mehta KK, Thang LL, editors. London: Springer; 2011.
  35. 35. Schooling CM, Hui LL, Ho LM, Lam TH, Leung GM. Cohort profile: 'Children of 1997': a Hong Kong Chinese birth cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2012;41: 611–620. pmid:21224275
  36. 36. Wong CK. The Rutter Parent Scale A2 and Teacher Scale B2 in Chinese. II. Clinical validity among Chinese children. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 1988;78: 11–17. pmid:3176991
  37. 37. Wong CK. The Rutter Parent Scale A2 and Teacher Scale B2 in Chinese. I. Translation study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 1988;77: 724–728. pmid:3407441
  38. 38. Battle J. Culture-free self-esteem inventories (2nd Edition). Austin, Tex: Pro-Ed; 1992.
  39. 39. Battle J. Culture-Free Self-Esteem Inventories, 2nd ed. Austin, Texas: Pro-Ed; 1992.
  40. 40. Tin SPP. Lifestyle, self-esteem and obesity in children. Ph.D. Thesis, The University of Hong Kong. 2013. Available: http://hub.hku.hk/handle/10722/196449
  41. 41. Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JB. Validation and utility of a self-report version of PRIME-MD: the PHQ primary care study. Primary Care Evaluation of Mental Disorders. Patient Health Questionnaire. JAMA. 1999;282: 1737–1744. pmid:10568646
  42. 42. Richardson LP, McCauley E, Grossman DC, McCarty CA, Richards J, et al. Evaluation of the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 Item for detecting major depression among adolescents. Pediatrics. 2010;126: 1117–1123. pmid:21041282
  43. 43. Allgaier A- K, Pietsch K, Frühe B, Sigl-Glöckner J, Schulte-Körne G. Screening for depression in adolescents: validity of the Patient Health Questionnaire in pediatric care. Depression and Anxiety. 2012;29: 906–913. pmid:22753313
  44. 44. Lee W. Body dissatisfaction, depressive symptoms, and pubertal timing in Hong Kong Chinese. Thesis, The University of Hong Kong. 2008. Available: http://hub.hku.hk/handle/10722/54681
  45. 45. Lin SL, Leung GM, Hui LL, Lam TH, Schooling CM. Is informal child care associated with childhood obesity? Evidence from Hong Kong's "Children of 1997" birth cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2011;40: 1238–1246. pmid:21624932
  46. 46. Guarnaccia PJ, Lopez S. The mental health and adjustment of immigrant and refugee children. Child and adolescent psychiatric clinics of North America. 1998;7: 537–553, viii–ix. pmid:9894054
  47. 47. Seaman SR, White IR, Copas AJ, Li L. Combining multiple imputation and inverse-probability weighting. Biometrics. 2012;68: 129–137. pmid:22050039
  48. 48. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Second edition. Hillsdale: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1988.
  49. 49. Moons KG, Donders RA, Stijnen T, Harrell FE Jr. Using the outcome for imputation of missing predictor values was preferred. J Clin Epidemiol. 2006;59: 1092–1101. pmid:16980150
  50. 50. Downey G, Coyne JC. Children of Depressed Parents—an Integrative Review. Psychological Bulletin. 1990;108: 50–76. pmid:2200073
  51. 51. Yu X, Tam WW, Wong PT, Lam TH, Stewart SM. The Patient Health Questionnaire-9 for measuring depressive symptoms among the general population in Hong Kong. Comprehensive psychiatry. 2012;53: 95–102. pmid:21193179
  52. 52. Burnham KP, Anderson DR. Model Selection and Multimodel Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic Approach New York: Springer-Verlag 2002.
  53. 53. Smith PK, Smith HC, Blades M. Understanding Children's Development. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishers Ltd; 1998.