Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Changes in Topological Organization of Functional PET Brain Network with Normal Aging

  • Zhiliang Liu ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Zhiliang Liu, Lining Ke

    Affiliation State Key Laboratory of Modern Optical Instrumentation, Department of Optical Engineering, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China

  • Lining Ke ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Zhiliang Liu, Lining Ke

    Affiliation Institute of Clinical Anatomy, School of Basic Medical Sciences, Southern Medical University, Guangzhou, China

  • Huafeng Liu,

    Affiliation State Key Laboratory of Modern Optical Instrumentation, Department of Optical Engineering, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China

  • Wenhua Huang ,

    huangwenhua2009@139.com (WHH); zhenghui@zju.edu.cn (ZHH)

    Affiliation Institute of Clinical Anatomy, School of Basic Medical Sciences, Southern Medical University, Guangzhou, China

  • Zhenghui Hu

    huangwenhua2009@139.com (WHH); zhenghui@zju.edu.cn (ZHH)

    Affiliation State Key Laboratory of Modern Optical Instrumentation, Department of Optical Engineering, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China

Abstract

Recent studies about brain network have suggested that normal aging is associated with alterations in coordinated patterns of the large-scale brain functional and structural systems. However, age-related changes in functional networks constructed via positron emission tomography (PET) data are still barely understood. Here, we constructed functional brain networks composed of regions in younger (mean age years) and older (mean age years) age groups with PET data. younger and older healthy individuals were separately selected for two age groups, from a physical examination database. Corresponding brain functional networks of the two groups were constructed by thresholding average cerebral glucose metabolism correlation matrices of regions and analysed using graph theoretical approaches. Although both groups showed normal small-world architecture in the PET networks, increased clustering and decreased efficiency were found in older subjects, implying a degeneration process that brain system shifts from a small-world network to regular one along with normal aging. Moreover, normal senescence was related to changed nodal centralities predominantly in association and paralimbic cortex regions, e.g. increasing in orbitofrontal cortex (middle) and decreasing in left hippocampus. Additionally, the older networks were about equally as robust to random failures as younger counterpart, but more vulnerable against targeted attacks. Finally, methods in the construction of the PET networks revealed reasonable robustness. Our findings enhanced the understanding about the topological principles of PET networks and changes related to normal aging.

Introduction

Function decline and organs aging is an inevitable physiological law of life. As one of the most important organs, aging brain tends to produce some specific alterations in morphological, physiological pathology and functional aspects. It is well known that normal aging is associated with a progressive decline in cognitive performance, including perception, attention, language and memory [1], [2], [3], [4]. Meanwhile, normal senescence is also highly related to some specific encephalopathies, such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD) [5] and Parkinson’s disease (PD) [6]. Therefore, aiming to assess the declining cognitive ability and supply a guiding for age-related encephalopathy in clinical, it is necessary to deeply understand the age-related changes in healthy brain.

An emerging approach for studying human brain system is graph theory which is represented by a set of nodes and links. It has been widely adopted to quantify complex system, e.g., in social sciences, biology or technology [7], [8]. The functional and structural systems of the human brain reveal age-related topological properties of complex networks, such as small-world characteristics, highly connected hubs, modularity, and network robustness [7], [9], [10], [8], [11]. Small-world properties, characterized by a high degree of clustering and a short average distance between any two nodes [12], [13], were analysed to reveal age-related global and local efficiency of information transfer in brain system. Some recent studies reported that, along with the normal aging, small-world network showed changed topological efficiency [10], [14], [15], [16]. For instance, a recent study on functional brain networks suggested that an older age group showed significantly reduced cost efficiency in comparison to a younger group [17]. A similar degeneration process of economical small-world networks was also found in a previous study about AD [18], [19]. Furthermore, changes in modular organization of human brain networks were proven to be associated with normal aging [9], [15]. Simultaneously, highly connected hubs are altered with normal aging, which has been reported in some previous studies [9], [7]. In addition, previous studies also found that normal aging processes significantly affect default mode network (DMN) [20], [21], [22], [23], which is typically deactivated during external stimulation [24], [25]. Thus, a gradually forming evaluated system of brain networks with neural imaging technologies was adopted in assessing the aging brain and provided a guiding for age-related encephalopathy in clinical [7], [8].

However, up to now, few studies have constructed functional network via positron emission tomography (PET) data. Compared with other functional signals, PET can offer a more immediate way to indicate brain activity by offering the index of cerebral glucose metabolism. In this paper, for evaluating the age-related brain changes in normal individuals, the large-scale human brain functional network was constructed by node, defined as regional average cerebral glucose metabolism from PET data. Four main reasons indicate that the definition is reasonable and effective for assessing age-related brain functional changes. Firstly, during the resting state, the level of cerebral glucose metabolism is considered as a reliable index of neural activity [48]. Secondly, synapses are considered as the key sites for transferring information between neurons, and up to of the glucose consumption in the brain is used to maintain a baseline synaptic activity [27]. Thirdly, many studies have reported that the normal aging is accomplished by a decline of synaptic activity, which impacts the cognitive functions [28], [29]. Lastly, effective connectivity between PET regions has been found in previous studies [30], [31]. Thus, the definition of PET nodes can provide a complementary and convincible way to improve the evaluation of brain functional networks.

In the present study, large-scale functional networks ( regions) in two age groups ( older subjects, younger subjects) were constructed by computing the partial correlation matrices of the regional mean intensity values from PET data. Afterwards, we investigated the brain functional topological properties, including small-world characteristics, hub regions and network robustness, revealing the brain functional changes associated with normal aging. Methodological robustness in the construction of PET network was also assessed.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Two hundred and twenty-three healthy human subjects were selected from a physical examination database, and written informed consents for the future research were obtained from all subjects. They were separated into two age groups, older subjects aged years (mean age =  years, male) and younger subjects aged years (mean age =  years, male). Health status of all subjects were evaluated with a normal physical examination before imaging. Individuals with significant chronic or acute disease were excluded from subjects. Other criteria of subjects’ physical condition were as follows: native Mandarin Chinese speaker, right-handed, same average education years, no history of neurological disease, no brain trauma or clinical evidence of cognitive impairment.

PET Data Acquisition

All PET investigations were implemented with Hamamatsu SHR whole-body PET scanner system located at the Medical PET Center of Zhejiang University. The spatial resolution of the scanner is full width at half maximum (FWHM) in sagittal or coronal plane and FWHM in axial plane. There is a patient aperture and an axial field-of-view of in the scanner, which can deal with the whole head. A source for attenuation correction was used in the emission scan after a transmission scanning. All subjects were injected intravenously with fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG) before resting in a dark, quiet room with ears open and eye closed for minutes. Then each subject was scanned lying quietly at rest with eyes closed for minutes. Nyquist frequency was acquired, after a ramp filter with the maximum-likelihood expectation maximization (MLEM) algorithm was used in reconstructions of PET images. Additionally, the acquisitions were performed with the approval of the Health Science Research Ethics Committee of Zhejiang University.

PET Data Preprocessing and Regional Parcellation

After using ImageJ (Wayne Rasband, National Institute of Mental Health, USA) and MRIcro software (http://www.mricro.com), Analyze Formats were acquired from raw PET data. Then the preprocessing was performed using matlab (MathWorks Inc., Notich, MA, USA) and Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM5, Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK) software. Each data was normalized into the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI, McGill University, Montreal, Canada) standard template using SPM5. An isotropic Gaussian kernel with FWHM was used in spatial smoothing to increase the signal-to-noise ratio after normalization. Then the proportional scaling was used for the intensity normalization [32], [33], [34]. Regional parcellation was completed using the anatomically automatic labeled (AAL) template image previously validated by Tzourio-Mazoyer et al. [35]. The brain was divided into 90 anatomical regions of interest ( regions for each hemisphere) using this parcellation. Then we calculated mean intensity values of each region which represented the regional cerebral metabolic rates for glucose.

Partial Correlation Analysis

The functional connection was defined as statistical associations in the intensity values across subjects. Such a connection concept has been introduced by the previous studies [36], [37]. It is reasonable to investigate brain functional systems (networks) by calculating connectivity of the PET regions, since effective connectivity between PET regions has been found previously [30], [31]. So, the analytical procedure was performed by analysing the regional relation obtained above. The interregional correlation matrix (, here ) of each group (Figure 1) was acquired by calculating the partial correlation coefficients across individuals between the mean intensity value of every pair of regions. The conditional dependences of arbitrary two regions partialled out the effects of the other regions defined in the AAL template were represented by the partial correlations between them.

thumbnail
Figure 1. The correlation matrices of two groups.

The graphs show the correlation matrices acquired by calculating partial correlations (left for the older group and right for the younger group). The color bar in the middle indicates the partial correlation coefficient between regions. The rank and row successively represent the 90 brain regions (see Table S1).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088690.g001

Construction of Functional Network

According to the prior studies [32], [7], functional networks of both groups could be acquired from their partial correlation matrices. The partial correlation matrix of each group was converted into a binarized matrix (Figure 2) by setting a threshold. Element of was if the absolute value of the correlation between regions and was smaller than given correlation threshold and otherwise. Topological organization of the human functional networks was represented by the binary matrices. Then a binary graph theoretical analysis [7], [8] was performed in the following.

thumbnail
Figure 2. The binarized matrices () of two groups.

The graphs show the binarized matrices (left for the older group and right for the younger group) which are generated by setting threshold to the correlation matrices. The rank and row successively represent the 90 brain regions (see Table S1). Such a threshold () ensures that the networks of both of the groups have the same number of nodes and links, and also show changed efficiency of information transfer (, , ). In this graph, white and black indicate the and .

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088690.g002

Graph Theoretical Analysis

Sparsity selection.

To perform a graph theoretical analysis, the binarized matrix is described as a network (graph) defined by nodes and edges, where nodes indicate regions and edges indicate undirected links between regions according to nonzero elements of . A fixed sparsity of each network, which was defined as the total number of edges in a graph divided by the maximum possible number of edges, would be calculated after its correlation matrix was thresholded into the binarized matrix. It was stated briefly that same correlation threshold would lead to different number of edges (, or ) between the both resulting graphs because of the difference in the low-level correlations (see Figure 1). Therefore, alterations in the topological organization would not be solely reflected by the between-group difference in network parameters. Hence, a sparsity-specific threshold was set to ensure that the both undirected graphs had the same number of edges () or wiring cost [17], [18]. Because a single and definitive threshold could not be selected currently, graphs with wide range of sparisty () was generated by repeatedly thresholding each correlation matrix, then properties of them were estimated at each threshold value. Then small-world parameters between the two groups were compared as a function of independent sparsity of the precise selection of threshold. The range of sparisty () also ensured that every nodal pairs in both graphs had a connecting path (mentioned below) [38]. Then we estimated network properties including clustering coefficient, path length, global efficiency, nodal centrality and network robustness in the following steps.

Clustering coefficient.

A cluster of node is formed by directly connected nearest neighbours of the node [7]. Clustering coefficient of a node quantifies the number of connections existing in the cluster as a proportion of maximum possible connections [12]. (Figure 3) of a network is defined as the average of over all nodes in a network and indicates the extent of local cliquishness or local efficiency of information transfer [12], [13].

thumbnail
Figure 3. Clustering coefficient () as a function of sparsity.

The graph shows that, at a wide range of sparsity (), the older subjects (red line) have larger value than the younger subjects (black line).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088690.g003

Path length and global efficiency.

Path length between node and node is defined as the minimum number of edges traversed from node to node . (Figure 4) is defined as the average of the all pairs nodes of the network and quantifies the ability of global efficiency of parallel information transfer [13]. Global efficiency (, Figure 5) inversely related to but numerically easier to indicate the global efficiency of parallel information transfer was also estimated. Global efficiency () measure is , means the path length of the node and node . Of note, the problem about the definition of caused by nodal pairs without connecting path, can been eliminated by the range of sparisty (, mentioned above).

thumbnail
Figure 4. Path length () as a function of sparsity.

The graph shows that two groups have same value when sparsity ranges from to and the older group (red line) have larger at .

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088690.g004

thumbnail
Figure 5. Global efficiency () as a function of sparsity.

is numerically easier to indicate the global efficiency than (see Material and Methods). As the sparsity thresholds increase from to , of both groups increase, and younger subjects (black line) have larger values. At high sparsity threshold (), two groups show equal values.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088690.g005

Small-world analysis.

In this study, small-world properties of networks in two groups were examined according to the and measured in the above steps. A small-world network should meet the following criteria: and [12], or [39], [40](see Figure 6), where the and are the mean path length and clustering coefficient of suitable random networks with the same number of nodes, edges, and degree (the degree of a node is the number of connections to that node) as the real network [12], [41]. Random graphs were generated by the random rewiring procedure [42], [43].

thumbnail
Figure 6. Small-world parameters of networks.

The graphs show the changes in (red line), (green line) and (blue line) in the networks of older (left panel) and younger (right panel) groups as a function of sparisty thresholds. At a wide range of sparsity, both networks have , that implies prominent small-world properties (see Materials and Methods). Note that, as the values of sparsity thresholds increase, the and values decrease rapidly, but the values decrease rapidly when sparsity range from to then change slightly.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088690.g006

Betweeness centrality.

The centrality () of node is defined as the number of the shortest paths between all other node pairs pass through it [44]. A node with high value is crucial to efficient communication in the network and is considered as the hub of the network [7]. Here was calculated by using the MatlabBGL package (http://www.stanford.edu/∼dgleich/programs/matlab_bgl/). Then normalized betweenness (, see Figure 7) was measured to estimate nodal characteristics of the networks, where was the average over all nodes in the network.

thumbnail
Figure 7. Betweenness centrality () of two groups.

The below graph shows the comparison (red bar for younger group and blue bar for older group) of normalized betweenness () in each node (region) between two groups. The upper graph shows the regional changes (, ) in normalized betweenness () between two groups. The regions labeled in the upper graph indicate significant changes in between two groups (see Table 4). Note that these results were obtained from the brain networks with a sparsity of . Regions in networks of two groups showing high value () have been listed in the Table 2 and Table 3.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088690.g007

Network robustness.

In this step, a simple analysis about network robustness was performed. Network robustness associated with the stability of a complex network refers to the degree of tolerance against random failures and targeted attacks [7]. In the current studies, robustness (tolerance) of the networks was investigated through removing nodes in the networks [45], [46], [40]. Firstly, to test the the nodal failure tolerance, one node was removed from the networks and changes in the size of the largest connected component were measured. Then other nodes were removed sequentially at random (Figure 8). To address the attack tolerance, the above processes were repeated but we removed the nodes of high value in the targeted position ( nodes of high value were removed from to in abscissa axis, showed in Figure 8). To investigate the comparison of the network robustness between two groups, the procedures were repeated 1000 times for the networks of both groups. Then we calculated the mean relative of largest component (Figure 8). Additionally, in order to investigate statistical differences, the percentile points of each distribution were used as the critical values for a two-tailed test of the null hypothesis with a probability of type I error of , with every number of the removed nodes under two attacks.

thumbnail
Figure 8. Topological robustness in networks of two groups.

The graphs show the relative size of the largest connected component as a function of the fraction of removed nodes by random failures or targeted attacks. As the response to random failures (left panel), the brain network in the older group (red line) is approximately as robust as that in the younger group (black line). Right graph shows that the older network displays remarkably reduced stability against targeted attack compared with the younger. Additionally, the statistical significant differences () of two groups was found with the ranges of and in the right graph.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088690.g008

Statistical Analysis

Correlation differences in statistical analysis.

It is necessary to validate the significance difference of these correlations in two groups with statistical analysis of correlations between pairs of regions. The values approximately normally distributed were generated from correlation coefficients, after Fishers -to- transform. Then the transformed values were compared by statistic to determine the significance of the between-group differences in correlations [47]. A false discovery rate (FDR) procedure [48] was performed to adjust to the the multiple comparisons at a value of .

Statistical differences in topological parameters.

A nonparametric permutation test method was applied to determine statistical significance of the between-group differences. First of all, , , and of the two-groups networks with a given sparsity were separately computed. Secondly, to test the null hypothesis that the group differences might occur by chance, we then randomly reallocated each individual set of regional cerebral glucose metabolism to one or the other of the two groups. Thirdly, after recomputing the correlation matrix and obtaining binarized matrix, we recalculated the network parameters for each randomized group, using the same method. Lastly, this randomization procedure was repeated times and the percentile points of each distribution were used as the critical values for a one-tailed test of the null hypothesis with a probability of type I error of . Then the procedure was repeated at every sparsity threshold value of the networks.

Methodological Robustness Analysis

It is necessary to test the methodological robustness in the construction of networks, because the PET networks are barely constructed by calculating the partial correlation matrices. In this study, the methodological robustness was estimated by reducing sample size in each group. Firstly, individuals were separately removed from the both groups at random, to test methodological robustness against the reductions of samples size in both groups. It is noted that quantities ( nodes) of the removal individuals are limited by the size of the whole sample. After the above step was repeated 50 times, the mean smaller-sample networks ( in older group and in younger group) were obtained. As a comparison to the former networks, the small-world parameters (, and , see Figure 9 A and B), global efficiency (Figure 9 D) and local efficiency (Figure 9 C) in smaller-sample networks were calculated via the above methods. Furthermore, in order to determine statistical significance of the neo-networks differences, the nonparametric permutation test method was applied on and . Methodological robustness was analysed by comparing the small-world properties between networks with different samples.

thumbnail
Figure 9. Small-world properties in smaller-sample networks.

A and B, The graphs show the small-world parameters (, and ) of smaller-sample networks in older (A) and younger (B) groups. At a wide range of sparsity, both networks have , that implies prominent small-world properties (see Materials and Methods). C, This graph shows that older subjects (red line) have larger values than the younger subjects (black line). In the original networks, similar result of was also shown in Figure 3. D, This graph shows the global efficiency () as a function of sparsity. As the sparsity thresholds increase from to , of both groups increase and younger subjects (black line) have larger values.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088690.g009

Results

Correlations of Regions in Two Groups

As is shown in Figure 1, the interregional correlation matrices represent complex correlation patterns of both age groups. Statistical analysis further demonstrates significant between-group correlation differences (, FDR-corrected) in various pairs of regions (Table 1). For instance, older subjects are found to show increased positive correlations in various pairs of cortical regions involved in the frontal, precentral and postcentral. In addition, decreased positive correlations in older group are observed in pairs of regions including hippocampus, amygdala. We also note several changed negative correlations in the older group. All pairs of regions with significant between-group changed correlation coefficients are listed in Table 1.

Small-world Topology Functional Networks

It has been demonstrated in the previous studies [32], [49], [41] that functional network of humans has small-world characteristics. In a small-world network, the nodes of the network have larger local interconnectivity than a random network, but the shortest path length between any pair of nodes is approximately equivalent to a comparable random network [12]. The small-world attributes of the functional networks in two age groups were also examined in the current study. As expected, both functional networks demonstrate small-world characteristics (Figure 6, left for older group and right for younger group, red line) over a wide range of sparsity (). Compared with the matched random networks, they have larger local cliquishness (, green line) but an almost identical path length (, blue line). Using computational modeling simulation approaches, Sporns et al. [50] propose the emergence of small-world topology when networks are evolved for high complexity of dynamic behavior defined as an optimal balance between global integration and local specialization. Therefore, our findings additionally support hypothesis that human brain has evolved into a complex but efficient neural architecture to maximize the power of information processing [41], [51].

Different Small-world Parameters between Two Age Groups

As shown in Figure 3, clustering coefficient () in networks of older group (red line in Figure 3) are larger than those of younger group (black line in Figure 3) over a wide range of sparsity (). Global efficiency (Figure 5) in networks of younger group (red line in Figure 5) are larger than those of older group (black line in Figure 5), when sparsity ranged from to . Both groups show different small-world parameters, reduced global efficiency (, Figure 4 and Figure 5) and increased local cliquishness (Figure 3) in older group. Additional statistical analysis also reveals significant differences () in the values at , values at and at . These results imply that older subjects are probably related to the loss of small-world characteristics in the large-scale functional brain systems. In addition, approximate results were obtained in previous studies about normal aging and even AD [32], [16], [18], [19].

Hub Regions

The functional networks were constructed at a sparsity threshold of to investigate the nodal characteristics of each region in two age groups. After normalized betweenness centrality () of each region (Figure 7) in both networks was measured (see Materials and Methods), hubs were defined as the regions with high betweenness centrality (). In the older group, regions (Table 2) are identified as the hubs because of large values in . In another group, regions are identified as the hubs (Table 3). Our finding of some hub regions (including supplementary motor area, left hippocampus,) is consistent with a previous brain functional network study age-related changes [9].

thumbnail
Table 2. Regions showing high betweenness () in the network of older group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088690.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Regions showing high betweenness () in the network of younger group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088690.t003

Changed Regional Nodal Characteristics between Two Groups

The regions (Figure 7) with between-group changes in betweenness centrality are examined in this study. Compared with the younger subjects, the older show increased betweenness centrality () in 3 regions (ORBmid.L, ORBmid.R, INS.R, listed in Table 4) and decreased betweenness centrality in 4 regions (SFGmed.R, HIP.L, AMYG.R, HES.L, listed in Table 4). Additional statistical analysis reveals significant differences () in betweenness centrality of these regions. The changed nodal characteristics () of each region are also showed in Figure 7 (upper panel). Together, our findings suggest that the roles of regions in managing information are profoundly affected by age [9].

thumbnail
Table 4. Regions showing significant changes in normalized betweenness () between two groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088690.t004

Reduced Network Robustness in Older Subjects

Figure 8 shows the network robustness of two age groups under the targeted attack and random failures. Both groups reveal similar network robustness to the random failures (Figure 8). When the nodes were randomly removed, the sizes of the largest connected component in both groups reduced steadily and approximately (Figure 8, left). Although network robustness of both groups reduced sharply due to the removing of 20 central nodes from to , the younger network displayed remarkably stability against targeted attack compared with the older (Figure 8, right). In addition, the statistical significant differences () of two groups are only found in the targeted-attack procedure. The specific ranges are and .

Small-world Parameters in Smaller-sample Networks

Figure 9 shows the methodological robustness, in response to the decrease of samples size in both age groups. In the smaller-sample networks, small-world characteristics are also revealed according to (red lines) shown in Figure 9 A and B. Simultaneously, larger local efficiency (Figure 9 C) and lower global efficiency (Figure 9 D) in older group are found in smaller-sample networks. Additional statistical analysis reveals significant differences () in the values at all range (), and values at . These findings of two groups are compatible with the former results in the original networks.

Discussion

The current study, for the first time, demonstrates age-related changes in the topological organization of large-scale functional brain networks by utilizing PET data. Our main results are as follows: (1) that the observed data demonstrate age-related alterations in functional correlations among selective subsets of regions, (2) that the global topological organization of functional networks in older subjects are disrupted as indicated by altered small-world parameters, (3) that the regional nodal characteristic (centrality) is changed in older subjects, (4) that the functional network of older group shows reduced network robustness in response to the targeted attack, (5) that the methods to construct the functional PET networks demonstrate reasonable robustness.

Small-world Characteristics and Age-related Changes

Our findings of high global and local efficiency in functional brain networks with both age groups are consistent with some previous studies [32], [7], [17], [52], [53]. Especially, another PET study [32], which compared the properties of whole-brain functional networks of normal, mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and AD individuals by using FDG-PET data, has reported that brain functional PET networks of all show small-world property. Experimental studies [54] and computational modeling simulations approaches [50] have also proposed the emergence of small-world topology when networks evolved for high complexity of dynamic behavior defined as an optimal balance between global integration and local specialization [55]. Thus, Our findings provide additional support for the hypothesis that the human brain has evolved to maximize the cost efficiency of parallel information processing [50], [51].

We also find age-related changes of global and local efficiency (Figure 3, Figure 4 and Figure 5) in the functional networks. The network may develop into a more local and less distributed organization, in the normal processes of brain senescence. This phenomenon suggests a degeneration process with normal aging, which has been reported in some previous studies [7], [41]. It has been proposed that in comparison to small-world networks, the lattice-like networks have a slow signal propagation speed and synchronizability [55]. Many psychiatric and neurological disorders described as dysconnectivity syndromes are associated with the regular topological organization that disturbs the optimal balance of a small-world network [56]. Previous studies have proposed the regular topological organization of brain networks in patients with diseases such as AD or schizophrenia [52], [18]. These convergent evidences from methodologically disparate studies suggest that both AD and schizophrenia are related to abnormal topological organization of structural and functional brain networks [7]. Therefore, our finding about the degeneration process shows that normal aging has high risk for dysconnectivity syndromes.

In particular, the above results in functional brain networks are conformed to a previous study about structural brain networks [16], which the middle group (mean age years) shows higher values in the global efficiency and lower values difference in the local efficiency compared with the old group (mean age years). These evidences may suggest that age-related alterations in cortical functional networks can be related to structural deficits. Honey [57] have found that the spontaneous neuronal dynamics can be structured at multiple temporal scales, proposing a tight association between functional and structural networks. Thus, it could be speculated that the age-related alterations in functional networks shown here are likely to be caused by structural impairments.

Betweenness Centrality and Age-related Alterations

In a complex system, node betweenness represents an crucial metric which can be used to determine the relative importance of a node with a network and identify the pivotal nodes in the network [58]. As indicated above (see Results), and global hub regions (see Table 2, Table 3 and Figure 7) are identified in the older and younger respectively. These hub regions are mainly considered as recently evolved association and primitive limbic regions. It has been proven in the previous study that association regions contribute to the integrity of multiple functional systems, such as memory and attention systems, and are mainly involved in intelligent processing and maintenance of the senior spiritual activity [59]. Meanwhile, limbic regions which are highly interconnected with the prefrontal regions and subcortical regions, are closely related to emotion and a conscious state of mind [59]. Previous studies have reported that identified global hubs were mainly prefrontal and parietal regions, supplying a potential explanation for their well-documented activation by many cognitive functions [7]. In this study, the frontal and parietal regions are also considered as hub regions, especially in older group (see Table 2 and Table 3). Furthermore, although the identified global hubs vary among two age groups, most of these regions are found to show high node betweenness in the functional and structural human brain networks [40], [60], [61], [54], [62]. In addition, it is noted that the substantial discrepancies of identified global hubs between this study and the previous studies can be caused by the different neuroimaging modalities, subjects characteristics and computational methods.

Age-related alterations of hub regions (e.g., SFG and HIP) are also found in this study (see Table 2 and Figure 7). The most of these identified hub regions are association cortices regions ( out of ) in both age groups. This result is consistent with a previous study that association cortices regions tend to be hubs of the brain functional network regardless of age [17]. From younger group to older group, association cortices show significant changed node betweenness (see Table 2, Table 3 and Table 4). These results support the view that age-related changes are characteristic of association cortex as opposed to primary cortex [63]. We find significant changes in node betweenness with decreasing and increasing in normal aging. This result is also similar to the finding by a previous study which indicated both negative and positive age effects on the regional efficiency in cortical regions [61]. Our finding is also consistent with a previous study that the ageing is associated with significantly reduced nodal efficiency in the frontal neocortex [17]. Above results suggest that frontal which manage movement (see Table 2 and Table 3) plays important roles in contacting information of both groups, but the importance of hippocampus closely related to mental activity is reduced in the older group, indicating the relative degradation of the aged mental activities. The similar findings have been reported in previous studies [63], [64], [65]. Overall, our finding demonstrates age-related changes in the nodal ability to manage information flow of PET networks.

In addition, PET investigations have revealed that the precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex and the medial prefrontal cortex, previously shown to be part of the DMN, display an elevated level of metabolic activity [66]. This result is consistent with our study that some elevated regions in DMN are the hubs (e.g. PCG.R, PCUN.R) which show nodal ability to manage the whole-brain PET network. Furthermore, a former study [20] has reported that magnitude of DMN co-activation in some regions (e.g. HIP.L and SFGmed.R) decreases with normal aging. In this study, these regions also show decreased centrality in older group. Hence, our whole-brain PET networks reveal similar regional characteristics to the previous DMN studies.

Topological Vulnerability in Functional Networks in Older Group

It has been demonstrated that small-world brain networks with embedded hubs exhibit surprising resilience to random failures and targeted attacks [40], [46]. Assuming that dynamic behavior of a network is strongly related to its fundamental configuration, it seems reasonable to suppose that the changes in network parameters reflect the disruptions in the general performance of the network such as stability and robustness. This hypothesis is supported by our results that the networks in older group are significantly vulnerable to targeted attacks on its pivotal nodes (hub regions) compared with younger group. The reduced topological stability is associated with senescent functional organization in older group such as small-world architecture, and nodal centrality shown previously. Moreover, former studies have reported the vulnerable topological organization of brain structural cortical networks in patients with AD [19]. Thus, this evidence from our study suggests that normal senescence has risk for AD.

Methodology

In this study, we constructed large-scale human brain functional networks via PET data. It is reasonable to conclude that cerebral glucose metabolism from PET data represent the regional functional activity [26], [27]. Effective connectivity between PET regions has also been revealed in previous studies about investigating brain functional systems [30], [31].

According to results about methodological robustness, the similar small-world parameters (see Figure 9) are obtained in responses to the decrease of the sample size. Small-world properties in both groups (Figure 9 A and B), reduced global efficiency (Figure 9 D) and increased local efficiency (Figure 9 D) in older group are also found. Thus, it is reasonable to consider that this method demonstrates sufficiently reliable. While this study was a cross-sectional study, a longitudinal analysis would also be useful to investigate the changes of functional brain networks with normal aging. In future studies of functional brain network development, younger individuals are expected to be involved in farther experiments.

Conclusion

As mentioned above, by using PET data with graph theory analysis, this study demonstrates age-related changes in the topological organization of large-scale functional brain networks constructed via a robust method. These results indicate that normal senescence has a notable effect on the topological organization of functional brain networks. Our findings are also compatible with previous studies about the small-world properties, hub regions and network robustness of brain functional and structural networks, thus enhancing our understanding of the underlying physiology of normal aging in human brain.

Supporting Information

Table S1.

90 regions of interest included in AAL-atlas.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088690.s001

(PDF)

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: ZHH HFL WHH. Performed the experiments: ZLL LNK. Analyzed the data: ZHH ZLL. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: ZHH HFL ZLL. Wrote the paper: ZLL ZHH.

References

  1. 1. Chiappe P, Siegel LS, Hasher L (2000) Working memory, inhibitory control, and reading disability. Memory & Cognition 28: 8–17.
  2. 2. Madden DJ, Turkington TG, Provenzale JM, Denny LL, Hawk TC, et al. (1999) Adult age differences in the functional neuroanatomy of verbal recognition memory. Human brain mapping 7: 115–135.
  3. 3. Mattay VS, Fera F, Tessitore A, Hariri A, Das S, et al. (2002) Neurophysiological correlates of age-related changes in human motor function. Neurology 58: 630–635.
  4. 4. Aine C, Sanfratello L, Adair J, Knoefel J, Caprihan A, et al. (2011) Development and decline of memory functions in normal, pathological and healthy successful aging. Brain topography 24: 323–339.
  5. 5. McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, et al. (1984) Clinical diagnosis of alzheimer’s disease report of the nincdsadrda work group* under the auspices of department of health and human services task force on alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 34: 939–939.
  6. 6. Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, Lees AJ (1992) Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic parkinson’s disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 55: 181–184.
  7. 7. Bullmore E, Sporns O (2009) Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10: 186–198.
  8. 8. Bassett DS, Bullmore ET (2009) Human brain networks in health and disease. Current opinion in neurology 22: 340.
  9. 9. Meunier D, Achard S, Morcom A, Bullmore E (2009) Age-related changes in modular organization of human brain functional networks. Neuroimage 44: 715.
  10. 10. Micheloyannis S, Vourkas M, Tsirka V, Karakonstantaki E, Kanatsouli K, et al. (2009) The influence of ageing on complex brain networks: a graph theoretical analysis. Human brain mapping 30: 200–208.
  11. 11. Sun J, Tong S, Yang GY (2012) Reorganization of brain networks in aging and age-related diseases. Aging and Disease 3: 181.
  12. 12. Watts D, Strogatz S (1998) The small world problem. Collective Dynamics of Small-World Networks 393: 440–442.
  13. 13. Latora V, Marchiori M (2001) Efficient behavior of small-world networks. Physical Review Letters 87: 198701.
  14. 14. Supekar K, Musen M, Menon V (2009) Development of large-scale functional brain networks in children. PLoS biology 7: e1000157.
  15. 15. Fair DA, Cohen AL, Power JD, Dosenbach NU, Church JA, et al. (2009) Functional brain networks develop from a local to distributed organization. PLoS computational biology 5: e1000381.
  16. 16. Wu K, Taki Y, Sato K, Kinomura S, Goto R, et al. (2012) Agerelated changes in topological organization of structural brain networks in healthy individuals. Human brain mapping 33: 552–568.
  17. 17. Achard S, Bullmore E (2007) Efficiency and cost of economical brain functional networks. PLoS computational biology 3: e17.
  18. 18. Stam C, Jones B, Nolte G, Breakspear M, Scheltens P (2007) Smallworld networks and functional connectivity in alzheimer’s disease. Cerebral Cortex 17: 92–99.
  19. 19. He Y, Chen Z, Evans A (2008) Structural insights into aberrant topological patterns of large-scale cortical networks in alzheimer’s disease. The Journal of neuroscience 28: 4756–4766.
  20. 20. Koch W, Teipel S, Mueller S, Buerger K, Bokde AL, et al. (2010) Effects of aging on default mode network activity in resting state fmri: does the method of analysis matter? Neuroimage 51: 280–287.
  21. 21. Damoiseaux J, Beckmann C, Arigita ES, Barkhof F, Scheltens P, et al. (2008) Reduced resting-state brain activity in the default network in normal aging. Cerebral Cortex 18: 1856–1864.
  22. 22. Fair DA, Cohen AL, Dosenbach NU, Church JA, Miezin FM, et al. (2008) The maturing architecture of the brain’s default network. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105: 4028–4032.
  23. 23. Bluhm RL, Osuch EA, Lanius RA, Boksman K, Neufeld RW, et al. (2008) Default mode network connectivity: effects of age, sex, and analytic approach. Neuroreport 19: 887–891.
  24. 24. Binder JR, Frost JA, Hammeke TA, Bellgowan P, Rao SM, et al. (1999) Conceptual processing during the conscious resting state: a functional mri study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 11: 80–93.
  25. 25. Shulman GL, Corbetta M, Buckner RL, Raichle ME, Fiez JA, et al. (1997) Top-down modulation of early sensory cortex. Cerebral Cortex 7: 193–206.
  26. 26. Petit-Taboue M, Landeau B, Desson J, Desgranges B, Baron J (1998) Effects of healthy aging on the regional cerebral metabolic rate of glucose assessed with statistical parametric mapping. Neuroimage 7: 176–184.
  27. 27. Phelps M, Huang S, Hoffman E, Selin C, Sokoloff L, et al. (1979) Tomographic measurement of local cerebral glucose metabolic rate in humans with (f-18) 2-fluoro-2-deoxy-d-glucose: validation of method. Annals of neurology 6: 371–388.
  28. 28. Pardo JV, Lee JT, Sheikh SA, Surerus-Johnson C, Shah H, et al. (2007) Where the brain grows old: Decline in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal function with normal aging. Neuroimage 35: 1231–1237.
  29. 29. Van Veen V, Krug MK (2009) Schooler JW, Carter CS (2009) Neural activity predicts attitude change in cognitive dissonance. Nature neuroscience 12: 1469–1474.
  30. 30. Horwitz B, Tagamets M, McIntosh AR (1999) Neural modeling, functional brain imaging, and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3: 91–98.
  31. 31. Ferrarelli F, Haraldsson HM, Barnhart TE, Roberts AD, Oakes TR, et al. (2004) [A<sup>17</sup> f]-fluoromethane pet/tms study of effective connectivity. Brain research bulletin 64: 103–113.
  32. 32. Seo EH, Lee DY, Lee JM, Park JS, Sohn BK, et al. (2013) Whole-brain functional networks in cognitively normal, mild cognitive impairment, and alzheimers disease. PloS one 8: e53922.
  33. 33. Gavrilescu M, Shaw ME, Stuart GW, Eckersley P, Svalbe ID, et al. (2002) Simulation of the effects of global normalization procedures in functional mri. Neuroimage 17: 532–542.
  34. 34. Dukart J, Mueller K, Horstmann A, Vogt B, Frisch S, et al. (2010) Differential effects of global and cerebellar normalization on detection and differentiation of dementia in fdg-pet studies. Neuroimage 49: 1490–1495.
  35. 35. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, et al. (2002) Automated anatomical labeling of activations in spm using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the mni mri single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15: 273–289.
  36. 36. Worsley K, Chen J, Lerch J, Evans A (2005) Comparing functional connectivity via thresholding correlations and singular value decomposition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 360: 913–920.
  37. 37. Lerch J, Worsley K, Shaw W, Greenstein D, Lenroot R, et al. (2006) Mapping anatomical correlations across cerebral cortex (macacc) using cortical thickness from mri. Neuroimage 31: 993–1003.
  38. 38. Liu Y, Liang M, Zhou Y, He Y, Hao Y, et al. (2008) Disrupted smallworld networks in schizophrenia. Brain 131: 945–961.
  39. 39. Humphries M, Gurney K, Prescott T (2006) The brainstem reticular formation is a small-world, not scale-free, network. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 273: 503–511.
  40. 40. Achard S, Salvador R, Whitcher B, Suckling J, Bullmore E (2006) A resilient, low-frequency, small-world human brain functional network with highly connected association cortical hubs. The Journal of Neuroscience 26: 63–72.
  41. 41. Sporns O, Chialvo D, Kaiser M, Hilgetag C, et al. (2004) Organization, development and function of complex brain networks. Trends in cognitive sciences 8: 418–425.
  42. 42. Maslov S, Sneppen K (2002) Specificity and stability in topology of protein networks. Science Signalling 296: 910.
  43. 43. Milo R, Shen-Orr S, Itzkovitz S, Kashtan N, Chklovskii D, et al. (2002) Network motifs: simple building blocks of complex networks. Science Signalling 298: 824.
  44. 44. Freeman L (1977) A set of measures of centrality based on betweenness. Sociometry : 35–41.
  45. 45. Albert R, Jeong H, Barabási A (2000) Error and attack tolerance of complex networks. Nature 406: 378–382.
  46. 46. Kaiser M, Hilgetag C (2004) Edge vulnerability in neural and metabolic networks. Biological cybernetics 90: 311–317.
  47. 47. Cohen J, Cohen P (1975) Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum.
  48. 48. Genovese C, Lazar N, Nichols T (2002) Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. Neuroimage 15: 870–878.
  49. 49. Bassett D, Bullmore E (2006) Small-world brain networks. The neuroscientist 12: 512–523.
  50. 50. Sporns O, Tononi G, Edelman G (2000) Theoretical neuroanatomy: relating anatomical and functional connectivity in graphs and cortical connection matrices. Cerebral Cortex 10: 127–141.
  51. 51. Kaiser M, Hilgetag C (2006) Nonoptimal component placement, but short processing paths, due to long-distance projections in neural systems. PloS Computational Biology 2: e95.
  52. 52. Bassett DS, Bullmore E, Verchinski BA, Mattay VS, Weinberger DR, et al. (2008) Hierarchical organization of human cortical networks in health and schizophrenia. The Journal of Neuroscience 28: 9239–9248.
  53. 53. Wang L, Zhu C, He Y, Zang Y, Cao Q, et al. (2009) Altered small-world brain functional networks in children with attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder. Human brain mapping 30: 638–649.
  54. 54. Chen ZJ, He Y, Rosa-Neto P, Germann J, Evans AC (2008) Revealing modular architecture of human brain structural networks by using cortical thickness from mri. Cerebral cortex 18: 2374–2381.
  55. 55. Strogatz SH (2001) Exploring complex networks. Nature 410: 268–276.
  56. 56. Catani M, Ffytche DH (2005) The rises and falls of disconnection syndromes. Brain 128: 2224–2239.
  57. 57. Honey CJ, Kötter R, Breakspear M, Sporns O (2007) Network structure of cerebral cortex shapes functional connectivity on multiple time scales. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104: 10240–10245.
  58. 58. Wang XF, Chen G (2003) Complex networks: small-world, scale-free and beyond. Circuits and Systems Magazine, IEEE 3: 6–20.
  59. 59. Mesulam MM (1998) From sensation to cognition. Brain 121: 1013–1052.
  60. 60. He Y, Dagher A, Chen Z, Charil A, Zijdenbos A, et al. (2009) Impaired small-world efficiency in structural cortical networks in multiple sclerosis associated with white matter lesion load. Brain 132: 3366–3379.
  61. 61. Gong G, Rosa-Neto P, Carbonell F, Chen ZJ, He Y, et al. (2009) Ageand gender-related differences in the cortical anatomical network. The Journal of Neuroscience 29: 15684–15693.
  62. 62. Iturria-Medina Y, Sotero RC, Canales-Rodríguez EJ, Alemán-Gómez Y, Melie-García L, et al. (2008) Studying the human brain anatomical network via diffusion-weighted mri and graph theory. Neuroimage 40: 1064.
  63. 63. Albert ML (1994) Clinical neurology of aging. Oxford University Press, USA.
  64. 64. Reijneveld JC, Ponten SC, Berendse HW, Stam CJ, et al. (2007) The application of graph theoretical analysis to complex networks in the brain. Clinical Neurophysiology 118: 2317–2331.
  65. 65. Jhoo JH, Lee DY, Choo IH, Seo EH, Oh JS, et al. (2010) Discrimination of normal aging, mci and ad with multimodal imaging measures on the medial temporal lobe. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 183: 237–243.
  66. 66. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, et al. (2001) A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 98: 676–682.