Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

An Allele of Sequoia Dominantly Enhances a Trio Mutant Phenotype to Influence Drosophila Larval Behavior

  • Kathryn E. Dean ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Kathryn E. Dean, April Fields, Marcus J. Geer, Eric C. King, Brian T. Lynch, Rohan R. Manohar, Julianne R. McCall, Katherine C. Palozola, Yan Zhang

    Current address: Radiology Department, New York Presbyterian Weill Cornell Medical Center, New York, New York, United States of America

    Affiliation Department of Biology, Denison University, Talbot Hall of Biological Science, Granville, Ohio, United States of America

  • April Fields ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Kathryn E. Dean, April Fields, Marcus J. Geer, Eric C. King, Brian T. Lynch, Rohan R. Manohar, Julianne R. McCall, Katherine C. Palozola, Yan Zhang

    Current address: Department of Physiology and Biophysics, Case Western Reserve University School of Medicine, Cleveland, Ohio, United States of America

    Affiliation Department of Biology, Denison University, Talbot Hall of Biological Science, Granville, Ohio, United States of America

  • Marcus J. Geer ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Kathryn E. Dean, April Fields, Marcus J. Geer, Eric C. King, Brian T. Lynch, Rohan R. Manohar, Julianne R. McCall, Katherine C. Palozola, Yan Zhang

    Current address: University of Toledo College of Medicine, Toledo, Ohio, United States of America

    Affiliation Department of Biology, Denison University, Talbot Hall of Biological Science, Granville, Ohio, United States of America

  • Eric C. King ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Kathryn E. Dean, April Fields, Marcus J. Geer, Eric C. King, Brian T. Lynch, Rohan R. Manohar, Julianne R. McCall, Katherine C. Palozola, Yan Zhang

    Current address: Peace Corps Branch Office, Nampula, Dar Es Salaam, Nampula, Mozambique

    Affiliation Department of Biology, Denison University, Talbot Hall of Biological Science, Granville, Ohio, United States of America

  • Brian T. Lynch ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Kathryn E. Dean, April Fields, Marcus J. Geer, Eric C. King, Brian T. Lynch, Rohan R. Manohar, Julianne R. McCall, Katherine C. Palozola, Yan Zhang

    Current address: Department of Biological Science, Oakland University, Rochester, Michigan, United States of America

    Affiliation Department of Biology, Denison University, Talbot Hall of Biological Science, Granville, Ohio, United States of America

  • Rohan R. Manohar ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Kathryn E. Dean, April Fields, Marcus J. Geer, Eric C. King, Brian T. Lynch, Rohan R. Manohar, Julianne R. McCall, Katherine C. Palozola, Yan Zhang

    Current address: Department of Stem Cell and Regenerative Biology, Harvard University, Bauer Labs, Cambridge, Massachusetts, United States of America

    Affiliation Department of Biology, Denison University, Talbot Hall of Biological Science, Granville, Ohio, United States of America

  • Julianne R. McCall ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Kathryn E. Dean, April Fields, Marcus J. Geer, Eric C. King, Brian T. Lynch, Rohan R. Manohar, Julianne R. McCall, Katherine C. Palozola, Yan Zhang

    Current address: Klinik fuer Paraplegiologie, Heidelberg, Germany

    Affiliation Department of Biology, Denison University, Talbot Hall of Biological Science, Granville, Ohio, United States of America

  • Katherine C. Palozola ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Kathryn E. Dean, April Fields, Marcus J. Geer, Eric C. King, Brian T. Lynch, Rohan R. Manohar, Julianne R. McCall, Katherine C. Palozola, Yan Zhang

    Current address: Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Smilow Center for Translational Research, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, United States of America

    Affiliation Department of Biology, Denison University, Talbot Hall of Biological Science, Granville, Ohio, United States of America

  • Yan Zhang ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Kathryn E. Dean, April Fields, Marcus J. Geer, Eric C. King, Brian T. Lynch, Rohan R. Manohar, Julianne R. McCall, Katherine C. Palozola, Yan Zhang

    Current address: Department of Medicine, Division of Infectious Diseases, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, Virginia, United States of America

    Affiliation Department of Biology, Denison University, Talbot Hall of Biological Science, Granville, Ohio, United States of America

  • Eric C. Liebl

    liebl@denison.edu

    Affiliation Department of Biology, Denison University, Talbot Hall of Biological Science, Granville, Ohio, United States of America

Abstract

The transition of Drosophila third instar larvae from feeding, photo-phobic foragers to non-feeding, photo-neutral wanderers is a classic behavioral switch that precedes pupariation. The neuronal network responsible for this behavior has recently begun to be defined. Previous genetic analyses have identified signaling components for food and light sensory inputs and neuropeptide hormonal outputs as being critical for the forager to wanderer transition. Trio is a Rho-Guanine Nucleotide Exchange Factor integrated into a variety of signaling networks including those governing axon pathfinding in early development. Sequoia is a pan-neuronally expressed zinc-finger transcription factor that governs dendrite and axon outgrowth. Using pre-pupal lethality as an endpoint, we have screened for dominant second-site enhancers of a weakly lethal trio mutant background. In these screens, an allele of sequoia has been identified. While these mutants have no obvious disruption of embryonic central nervous system architecture and survive to third instar larvae similar to controls, they retain forager behavior and thus fail to pupariate at high frequency.

Introduction

Connecting specific neural networks to specific behaviors or skills is a long-term goal of neurobiology. Toward that end model systems such as Drosophila have been useful, allowing specific neural structures as well as specific gene products to be linked to complex behaviors such as courtship and mating [1][3]. Another classic behavior that has been well studied in Drosophila is the foraging behavior of third instar larvae. As third instar larvae prepare to pupate they switch from being foragers, which are focused on feeding and are photo-phobic, to wanderers, which are not focused on feeding and are photo-neutral [4][7]. Several gene products have been implicated in the behavioral switch from foragers to wanderers; these include neuropeptides [5], enzymes involved in neuropeptide metabolism [8], ion channels [9], a mitochondrial DNA polymerase [10], and a scaffolding protein [11].

Identifying dominant second-site enhancers of a sensitized genetic background via modifier screens is a powerful technique to dissect signaling networks [12]. For example, the Abl tyrosine kinase is a regulator of axon outgrowth [13], and such second-site modifier screens have identified numerous dominant enhancers of the abl mutant phenotype with strong phenotypic effects on central nervous system (CNS) architecture [14][17]. Trio is a neuronally expressed Rho-Guanine Nucleotide Exchange Factor (GEF) involved in axon pathfinding during early embryogenesis [14], [18][20]. We have used second-site modifier genetics to explore signaling networks that involve Trio. Specifically, we have screened for mutations that dominantly exacerbate the pre-pupal lethality of a weakly lethal trio mutant background. In these screens we have identified an allele of the zinc-finger transcription factor sequoia. However, rather than grossly affecting axon pathfinding during embryogenesis, this genetic combination inhibits the behavioral switch from foragers to wanderers during the third larval instar.

Materials and Methods

Fly stocks and maintenance

All flies were maintained on either standard cornmeal-yeast medium or Formula 4–24 Instant Food (Carolina Biological) in humidified incubators. All fly stocks were obtained from the Bloomington Stock Center except for the trioM89 stock that was generated in our laboratory and has been described previously [14].

Mutagenesis and screening for dominant enhancers of the trio pre-pupal lethality

Males with wild-type second chromosomes and third chromosomes carrying the trioM89 mutation were subjected to 45 Gray of gamma radiation at the Ohio State University Nuclear Reactor Laboratory. Mutagenized second and third chromosomes were captured over the T(2;3) SM6a-TM6B balancer chromosomes which is marked with the dominant phenotypic markers Tubby (Tb), and Curly (Cy). The Tubby phenotype is easily scored in pupae. The Curly phenotype is easily scored in adults. Single males were crossed to +; trios036810/T(2;3) SM6a-TM6B virgin females. Such crosses should produce a Mendelian ratio of 2 Tubby, Curly : 1 non-Tubby, non-Curly (i.e. wild type) animals. Critically, the non-Tubby, non-Curly offspring represent the trio mutant (trios036810/trioM89) class. Progeny were visually screened for synthetic lethality by scoring for a strong reduction in the frequency of these non-Tubby, non-Curly trio mutant (trios036810/trioM89) pupae and adults. Reductions were quantified by dividing the number of non-Tubby (or non-Curly) animals by half the number of Tubby (or Curly) animals to obtain the “percent expected”. Chromosomes showing less than 25% of expected trio mutant pupae were recovered, retested and maintained as balanced stocks.

Quantifying larvae

The Tubby phenotypic marker was used to score larvae. Crosses were carried out on 4–24 Instant food (Carolina Biological) and progeny larvae were recovered by floatation after re-suspending the food plug in 3 M NaCl. “Percent expected” was calculated as described above.

Mapping and cloning the 9.17 mutation

The 9.17-containing second chromosome was recovered in unbalanced females over the wgSp-1, Bl1, Lrm, Bc1, Pu2 mapping chromosome and multiple (>15) recombinants across each phenotypically scorable interval were recovered and tested for the presence of the 9.17 mutation by assaying for its enhancer activity.

Genomic libraries were made with the CopyControl Fosmid Library Production kit (Epicenter Biotechnologies). Libraries were screened with DIG-probes using standard protocols. All sequencing was done at the Ohio State Plant-Microbe Genomics Facility. Contigs were aligned using Sequencher (Gene Codes Corporation), and BLAST alignments through FlyBase (www.flybase.org).

Antibody staining of embryos

Antibody staining was carried out on 0–24 hr embryos that were collected, fixed and stained as described previously [14]. Monoclonal antibody BP102 was obtained from the Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank (University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA). Mouse monoclonal anti-beta-galactosidase antibody (Promega, Madison WI) was used to detect lacz expression from enhancer trap-containing balancer chromosomes to distinguish embryos' genotypes.

Food dispersal assays

Food dispersal assays were carried out similar to those described in Moncalvo and Campos [6]. Synchronized larvae were generated by crossing animals of the appropriate genotype in large cages (Genesee Scientific). Embryos were collected in 2-hour windows on 1.8% agar plates. These plates were removed from the cages and aged for an additional 24 hours at 25°C after which all hatched larvae were removed with a mild suction apparatus. The cleared plates were returned to the 25°C incubator for 3 hours, after which all hatched first instar larvae (age  = 0–3 hours after hatching) were removed and transferred to 100 mm dishes containing 40 ml of 4–24 instant food (Carolina Biological). Larvae were seeded at the defined density of 100 larvae per plate. These synchronized first instar larvae were then aged at 25°C to the appropriate stage (63–66 hours after hatching for young third instar; 90–93 hours after hatching for older third instar) and isolated by re-suspending the food in 3 M NaCl. The buoyant, floating larvae were skimmed off the surface, washed with distilled water and starved for 60 minutes at 25°C on moist filter paper. Animals were then transferred to the middle of a 25 mm×10 mm plug of 4–24 instant food placed on a 3% agar base in a 100 mm petri dish. After 90 minutes at 25°C, the food plug was scooped up and larvae within recovered by floatation with 3 M NaCl. Larvae that had migrated off the food plug were recovered with a paintbrush. Larvae were rinsed, transferred to 95% ethanol and phenotypically classified using the Tubby marker.

Larval crawling assays

Larval crawling assays were carried out similar to those described in Nichols et al., [21]. Synchronized larvae were generated as described above. Individual larvae were then transferred to 100 mm dishes with 2% agar over 2 mm grid paper. The number of lines the larva's mouth crossed in 60 seconds was recorded.

Larval mass determination

Synchronized larvae were generated as described above. The mass of individual larvae was determined by duplicate measurements with an Ohaus Discovery DV215CD balance, accurate to 0.01 mg.

Larval instar determination

The mouth hooks from individual larvae were dissected under 80% glycerol and examined with 400× magnification. The overall morphology was compared to that shown in Strasburger [22] as referenced in Ashburner [23], and the number of small teeth on the front part of the armature were counted [24].

Statistical analysis

Data was analyzed using the JMP 10 statistical software package (SAS Institute) and consisted of Chi-squared tests, t-tests, one-way ANOVAs and Tukey-Kramer HSD pairwise analyses.

Results

Isolation of the 9.17 mutation as a dominant enhancer of the trio pre-pupal lethality

trios036810 is a hypomorphic allele of the Trio Rho-GEF generated by P-element mutagenesis [14], [18], [20]. trioM89 is an EMS-induced point mutation (L1412F) that disrupts a conserved residue in Trio's first GEF-domain [14]. Animals of the genotype trios036810/trioM89 constitute a genetic background sensitized for Trio activity, as less than expected but still a significant number of these animals survive to both the pupae and adult stages [14] (Table 1). In this sensitized background, gamma ray-mutagenized second and third chromosomes were screened for their ability to dominantly exacerbate trio's pre-pupal lethality. The 9.17 mutation was isolated in this screen as animals heterozygous mutant for 9.17 and homozygous mutant for trio (9.17/+; trios036810/trioM89) demonstrated only 23.9% expected survival to pupae and 5.6% expected survival to adults (Table 1). 9.17's dominant effect was dependent on the trio mutant background, as animals heterozygous mutant for 9.17 and heterozygous mutant for trio showed robust viability (Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Dosage-sensitive modification of the trio mutant phenotypea.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0084149.t001

Molecular characterization of the 9.17 mutation

Mutation 9.17 was mapped to the second chromosome. Meiotic recombinational mapping that followed 9.17's dominant enhancement of the trio mutant phenotype placed 9.17 at 64 cM. Scanning across this region with overlapping chromosomal deficiencies uncovered a recessive lethal lesion on the 9.17 chromosome in this exact area (between bases 9,047,084 and 9,106,852 based on the failure of the 9.17 chromosome to complement both Df(2R)Exel8057 and Df(2R)BSC273) [25], [26]. Genes likely to be involved in axon pathfinding and/or Rho family-dependent signal transduction or such genes' potential cis-regulatory sequences that lie within this region include Fak-like tyrosine kinase (PR2), a Rac-dependent tyrosine kinase [27], Neurospecific receptor kinase (Nrk), a transmembrane receptor tyrosine kinase expressed on neurons [28] and sequoia (seq), a zinc-finger transcription factor involved in regulating axon and dendrite morphology [29].

A fosmid-based genomic library was generated from the balanced 9.17 stock and clones from the 9.17 chromosome within this interval were distinguished from clones from the balancer chromosome based on diagnostic SNPs. From these clones candidate genes' (PR2, Nrk, seq) exons and intron/exon boundaries were fully sequenced. All sequences were wild type except for a 23 base-pair deletion that was discovered within sequoia's second coding exon. Conceptual translation showed this deletion would cause a frameshift resulting in 260 novel amino acids following Q561, but leaving Sequoia's zinc fingers intact (Fig. 1). Parallel cloning and sequencing of 9.17's parental chromosome showed the sequoia gene to be wild type. Thus as 1) this small deletion co-mapped with 9.17's dominant enhancement of the trio mutant phenotype, 2) a 23 base pair deletion is consistent with the gamma ray mutagenesis used to create 9.17, and 3) the lesion is absent from the parental chromosome, we have identified the 9.17 strong dominant enhancer of the trio pre-pupal lethality as seq9.17.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Schematic structures of wild type Sequoia and Sequoia9.17.

Sequoia+ is 882 residues long and contains two C2H2 zinc fingers (D375 to K476) shown as dark vertical lines. The 23 base pair deletion in seq9.17 causes a frameshift after Q561, resulting in 260 unique residues (striped box). Sequoia9.17 retains the two CH2H2 zinc fingers.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0084149.g001

Genetic characterization of the seq9.17 allele

A chromosomal deletion that removes seq (Df(2R)BSC273[25]) and the point alleles seqvr5-5 (Q641STOP[29]) and seqvr5-48 [30] all failed to complement seq9.17 in the trio+ background as judged by adult viability; crossing these seq alleles balanced over the CyO balancer chromosome with each other gave no straight-winged offspring. However, neither Df(2R)BSC273, seqvr5-5, nor seqvr5-48 behaved as a strong dominant enhancer of the trio pre-pupal lethailty, with approximately 50% of the expected pupae developing in all cases (Table 1). Thus while heterozygosity for a seq null deletion or for the recessive lethal seq alleles seqvr5-5 or seqvr5-48, in the trios036810/trioM89 mutant background, reduced pupal and adult viability as compared to controls, this enhancement is much less than that seen with seq9.17 (Table 1). Therefore seq9.17 behaves differently than all other seq alleles tested.

Phenotypic characterization of seq9.17's dominant enhancement of the trio pre-pupal lethality

The dominant enhancement of sensitized genetic backgrounds involving genes coding for proteins within axon pathfinding signaling networks often results in strong embryonic CNS phenotypes (e.g. [14][17]). However, the embryonic CNS of animals heterozygous mutant for seq9.17 and homozygous mutant for trio (seq9.17/+; trios036810/trioM89) was indistinguishable from simple trio mutants (trios036810/trioM89) when visualized with mAb BP102 (x12 = 1.01, p = 0.48; Table 2). As seq9.17 was identified by its ability to dominantly reduce pupation of trio mutant animals, yet those animals had structurally normal embryonic CNSs, we then examined the survival of those animals to larval stages. While survival to pupae was dramatically reduced by the presence of the seq9.17 mutation (67.9% of expected for trios036810/trioM89 vs. 23.9% of expected for seq9.17/+; trios036810/trioM89; x12 = 85.29, p<0.0001; Table 2), survival to third instar larvae was indistinguishable between the two genotypes (75.5% of expected vs. 67.6% of expected respectively; x12 = 1.71, p = 0.19; Table 2). Even after 10 days at 25°C, no seq9.17/+; trios036810/trioM89 pupae were found “hidden” within or under the food plug; these larvae are retarded in their advancement to pupae. In an effort to understand this reduced ability of seq9.17/+; trios036810/trioM89 larvae to pupate, we examined several aspects of these larvae.

thumbnail
Table 2. Characterizing the lethal phase of the seq9.17 dominant enhancement.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0084149.t002

The general locomotive ability, as determined by larval crawling assays, was similar between +/+; trios036810/trioM89 and seq9.17/+; trios036810/trioM89 for both young third instar larvae (63–66 hours after hatching; t28.8 = −1.64, p = 0.11; Table 3) and old third instar larvae (90–93 hours after hatching; t43.5 = −1.54, p = 0.13; Table 4). The average mass of these young third instar larvae was similar (t25.8 = −1.62, p = 0.12; Table 3), but the old (90–93 hours after hatching) seq9.17/+; trios036810/trioM89 larvae were significantly less massive as compared to control +/+; trios036810/trioM89 larvae (t42.8 = −3.58, p = 0.0009; Table 4). This reduction in average mass was primarily due to 15% of the seq9.17/+; trios036810/trioM89 old (90–93 hours after hatching) larvae being noticeably smaller; representative animals are shown in Fig. 2. To confirm that these smaller old (90–93 hours after hatching) larvae were third instars, mouth hooks were examined from a representative sample. The mouth hooks' overall morphology (Figure S1) and tooth count on the front part of the armature (between 11 and 12 teeth) confirmed these were third instars [22][24].

thumbnail
Figure 2. Representative third instar larvae used in phenotypic characterizations.

A: 63–66 hours after hatching (forager) larvae. The two animals on the left are seq9.17/+; trios036810/trioM89. The two animals on the right are +/+; trios036810/trioM89. Anterior is up. A millimeter ruler is along the left for scale. B: 90–93 hours after hatching (wanderer) larvae. The two animals on the left are seq9.17/+; trios036810/trioM89. The smaller animal is representative of 15% of this population. The two animals on the right are +/+; trios036810/trioM89. Anterior is up. A millimeter ruler is along the left for scale.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0084149.g002

thumbnail
Table 3. Characterizing the seq9.17 dominant enhancement in 63–66 hours after hatching larvae (foragers).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0084149.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. Characterizing the seq9.17 dominant enhancement in 90–93 hours after hatching larvae (wanderers).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0084149.t004

While a general “failure to thrive” may contribute to a minority of the seq9.17/+; trios036810/trioM89 larvae's inability to pupate, it alone cannot account for the dramatic reduction in this genotype's pupae. Thus we hypothesized that the behavioral switch from foragers to wanderers may be defective in seq9.17/+; trios036810/trioM89 larvae, further reducing their representation as pupae. In food dispersion tests, larvae of defined ages reared at defined densities were placed onto food plugs and tested for their preference to stay and feed or disperse from the plug over a 90-minute test period. All progeny of either control crosses (+; trioM89/T(2;3) SM6a-TM6B X +; trios036810/T(2;3) SM6a-TM6B) or experimental crosses (seq9.17; trioM89/T(2;3) SM6a-TM6B X +; trios036810/T(2;3) SM6a-TM6B) were tested together in one arena, and so the trio heterozygous mutant siblings (the phenotypically Tubby larvae) served as an internal control against which the trio homozygous mutant siblings (the phenotypically non-Tubby, or wild type larvae) were compared.

Young (63–66 hours after hatching) third instar larvae of all genotypes showed typical forager behavior with 95 to 100% of the animals staying within the food plug (Table 3). A majority of the old (90–93 hours after hatching) third instar larvae from control crosses showed typical wanderer behavior. Only 41.5% of the trios036810/trioM89 larvae stayed within the food plug, as did 44.8% of their siblings that had inherited a balancer chromosome, and thus were simply heterozygous mutant for trio (Table 4). A majority of the old (90–93 hours after hatching) third instar larvae from experimental crosses that had inherited a balancer chromosome and thus were simply heterozygous mutant for trio (half of which were also heterozygous mutant for seq9.17) showed typical wanderer behavior in the same assays, with only 35.8% staying within the food plug (Table 4). In contrast, most old (90–93 hours after hatching) seq9.17/+; trios036810/trioM89 third instar larvae retained typical forager behavior, with 92.5% of the animals staying within the food plug (F3,12 = 17.62, p = 0.0001; Table 4). Thus the seq9.17/+; trios036810/trioM89 larvae retained the forager-like feeding behavior at a time (90–93 hours after hatching) when their siblings and all control larvae had transitioned to wanderers.

Discussion

By assaying for a clear reduction in pupariation, this work has isolated a strong dominant enhancer of the trio pre-pupal lethality (Table 1). That enhancer has been identified as a unique allele of the zinc-finger transcription factor Sequoia (Fig. 1). The phenotypic basis of this dosage-sensitive genetic interaction has been characterized as a disruption in normal third instar larval growth and behavior. Fifteen percent of the seq9.17/+; trios036810/trioM89 old (90–93 hours after hatching) larvae are noticeably smaller, and the average mass of larvae of this age and genotype is statistically lower than controls (Fig. 2, Table 4). Additionally, sequoia heterozygous, trio homozygous mutant animals retain forager-like behavior (Table 4). The failure of these larvae to transition to the wanderer stage is likely a strong contributor to their failure to appear as pupae.

The successful transition of third instar larvae from foragers to wanderers must require the successful integration of a variety of external sensory cues and internal hormonal signals. Multiple mutations that interfere with the transition from foragers to wanderers disrupt various aspects of that general signaling network. Inputs from the environment sensed by larvae include food and light. Pickpocket1 (PPK1), a degenerin/epithelial sodium channel subunit, is expressed in class IV multiple dendritic neurons that completely tile the larval body wall [31]. Mutations in PPK1 disrupt responses to food-associated stimuli required during the early third instar and therefor disrupt the forager to wanderer transition [9]. Mutations in tamas (tam) were identified by their disruption of normal responses of foraging third instar larvae to light, and tam mutants disrupt larval and adult visual system development [10]. tam encodes the mitochondrial DNA polymerase catalytic subunit DNApoly-gamma125. The transition from photo-phobic foragers to photo-neutral wanderers has been linked to serotonin signaling in the brain. Reduced serotonin signaling or targeted inactivation of serotonergic neurons increases late third instar larval responses to light [6].

Neuropeptide signals within the CNS have also been identified as essential for the forager to wanderer transition. Mutations in amontillado (amon), which codes for a prohormone processing protease, fail to exhibit wanderer behavior [32], and amon mutant animals have impaired production of a variety of bioactive neuropeptide hormones [8]. Mutations in neuropeptide F (npf), the Drosophila homolog of mammalian neuropeptide Y, result in premature transition to wanderer behavior, demonstrating the importance of NPF in food attraction behavior [5]. Indeed, npf is normally silenced during the transition to wanderers and inappropriately extended production of NPF inhibits the transition to wanderers [5]. Mutations in the RanBPM scaffolding protein cause early wanderer behavior, although the molecular basis for this is unclear. Significantly, RanBPM is highly expressed in the larval Mushroom Body Kenyon cells, and the mutant phenotype is rescued by targeted expression of wild-type RanBPM to the Kenyon neurons [11], linking that specific larval CNS structure to this behavior.

All of the genetic effects on the forager to wanderer transition summarized above are due to simple homozygous mutations. The present work is unique in that its disruption of normal larval behavior is due to a dosage-sensitive genetic interaction between trio and sequoia alleles; both the simple trio mutant animals and the sequoia heterozygoes on their own show normal behavior (Table 1, Table 4), and neither gene has been previously linked to third instar larval behaviors. Dosage-sensitive genetic interactions often indicate close mechanistic, functional interactions between the two gene products or processes controlled by them (e.g. [33][36]).

What structure or activity might underlie the dosage-sensitive interaction between trio and sequoia that disrupts the forager to wanderer transition? Both genes are broadly expressed throughout the CNS and both broadly govern axon and/or dendrite outgrowth and morphology [29], [37]. Therefore, these larvae may have disruptions in neuronal pathways facilitating critical inputs (such as food or light signals), pathways facilitating outputs such as neuropeptide signals, or pathways governing the integration of these inputs and outputs. One convergence between previously identified work on the forager to wanderer transition and trio is the larval Mushroom Body. trio is strongly expressed in most third instar larval Mushroom Body structures, and trio mutant animals have severe, specific defects in the larval vertical and larval medial Mushroom Body lobes [18]. trio has been further linked to adult Mushroom Body development through dosage-sensitive genetic interactions with the L1-type CAM Neuroglian (Nrg), strongly suggesting Trio is a downstream effector of Nrg signaling [38]. As the larval Mushroom Body develops as a series of complexly choreographed waves of axon projections [39], it is not unexpected that Trio plays a critical role in its development. However, while seq is reportedly expressed at moderate levels in the larval CNS [40], this has not been specifically reported for the larval Mushroom Body. Thus the physical basis of the dosage-sensitive interaction between trio and sequoia remains an active area of investigation in our laboratory.

While this work has focused on the failure of seq9.17/+; trios036810/trioM89 larvae to transition from third instar larvae to pupae, the animals that do develop to pupae then fail to eclose as adults at high rates (Table 2). Therefore, the dosage-sensitive genetic interaction between trio and sequoia likely extends to structures and/or processes critical for eclosion.

A long-term goal of neurobiology is to connect specific behaviors to specific genes' activities and specific neural structures. This work has identified two genes, previously tied to embryonic dendrite and axon development, as having a dosage-sensitive genetic interaction impacting a classic larval behavior. Ongoing studies are directed at testing whether this genetic interaction has a structural basis within the central nervous system.

Supporting Information

Figure S1.

Representative mouth hooks from 90–93 hours after hatching (wanderer), seq9.17/+; trios036810/trioM89, third instar larvae. A: Mouth hook from a normal-sized larvae, representing 85% of the population, viewed under 400× magnification. Morphology and tooth count are consistent with third instar larvae. B: Mouth hook from a smaller-sized larvae, representing 15% of the population, viewed under 400× magnification. Morphology and tooth count are consistent with third instar larvae.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0084149.s001

(TIFF)

Acknowledgments

We thank the (now) Reverend Clair M. Hunkins and Mike Zianni for technical assistance. We thank Mark Seeger and Bjoern Brembs and the anonymous Reviewers for critical comments on the manuscript. We also thank Eric Carle for introducing us to the Very Hungry Caterpillar.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: ECL. Performed the experiments: KED AF MJG ECK BTL RRM JRM KCP YZ ECL. Analyzed the data: KED AF MJG ECK BTL RRM JRM KCP YZ ECL. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: ECL. Wrote the paper: ECL.

References

  1. 1. Ferveur J. (2010) Drosophila female courtship and mating behaviors: Sensory signals, genes, neural structures and evolution. Curr Opin Neurobiol 20: : 764–769. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=20934322&site=ehost-live via the Internet.
  2. 2. Villella A, Hall JC. (2008) Neurogenetics of courtship and mating in Drosophila. Adv Genet 62: : 67–184. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=19010254&site=ehost-live via the Internet.
  3. 3. Yamamoto D. (2007) The neural and genetic substrates of sexual behavior in Drosophila. Adv Genet 59: : 39–66. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=17888794&site=ehost-live via the Internet.
  4. 4. Sokolowski MB, Kent C, Wong J (1984) Drosophila larval foraging behavior developmental stages. Anim Behav 32: 645–651 10.1016/S0003-3472(84)80139-6.
  5. 5. Wu Q, Wen T, Lee G, Park JH, Cai HN, et al.. (2003) Developmental control of foraging and social behavior by the Drosophila neuropeptide Y-like system. Neuron 39: : 147–161. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=12848939&site=ehost-live via the Internet.
  6. 6. Moncalvo VGR, Campos AR (2009) Role of serotonergic neurons in the Drosophila larval response to light. BMC Neuroscience 10: 66 10.1186/1471-2202-10-66.
  7. 7. Sawin-McCormack EP, Sokolowski MB, Campos AR (1995) Characterization and genetic analysis of Drosophila melanogaster photobehavior during larval development. J. Neurogenet 10: 119–135.
  8. 8. Wegener C, Herbert H, Kahnt J, Bender M, Rhea JM (2011) Deficiency of prohormone convertase dPC2 (AMONTILLADO) results in impaired production of bioactive neuropeptide hormones in Drosophila. J Neurochem 118: 581–595 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2010.07130.x.
  9. 9. Ainsley JA, Kim MJ, Wegman LJ, Pettus JM, Johnson WA (2008) Sensory mechanisms controlling the timing of larval developmental and behavioral transitions require the Drosophila DEG/ENaC subunit, Pickpocket1. Dev Biol 322: 46–55 10.1016/j.ydbio.2008.07.003.
  10. 10. Iyengar B, Roote J, Campos AR (1999) The tamas gene, identified as a mutation that disrupts larval behavior in Drosophila melanogaster, codes for the mitochondrial DNA polymerase catalytic subunit (DNApol-gamma125). Genetics 153: 1809–1824.
  11. 11. Scantlebury N, Zhao XL, Moncalvo VGR, Camiletti A, Zahanova S, et al. (2010) The Drosophila gene RanBPM functions in the mushroom body to regulate larval behavior. PLoS One 5: e10652 10.1371/journal.pone.0010652.
  12. 12. Guarente L. (1993) Synthetic enhancement in gene interaction: A genetic tool come of age. Trends Genet 9: : 362–366. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=8273152&site=ehost-live via the Internet.
  13. 13. Lanier LM, Gertler FB. (2000) From abl to actin: Abl tyrosine kinase and associated proteins in growth cone motility. Curr Opin Neurobiol 10: : 80–87. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=10679439&site=ehost-live via the Internet.
  14. 14. Liebl EC, Forsthoefel DJ, Franco LS, Sample SH, Hess JE, et al. (2000) Dosage-sensitive, reciprocal genetic interactions between the abl tyrosine kinase and the putative GEF trio reveal trio's role in axon pathfinding. Neuron 26: 107–118.
  15. 15. Liebl EC, Rowe RG, Forsthoefel DJ, Stammler AL, Schmidt ER, et al.. (2003) Interactions between the secreted protein amalgam, its transmembrane receptor neurotactin and the abelson tyrosine kinase affect axon pathfinding. Development 130: : 3217–3226. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=12783792&site=ehost-live via the Internet.
  16. 16. Hill KK, Bedian V, Juang JL, Hoffmann FM. (1995) Genetic interactions between the Drosophila abelson (abl) tyrosine kinase and failed axon connections (fax), a novel protein in axon bundles. Genetics 141: : 595–606. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=8647396&site=ehost-live via the Internet.
  17. 17. Gertler FB, Bennett RL, Clark MJ, Hoffmann FM. (1989) Drosophila abl tyrosine kinase in embryonic CNS axons: A role in axonogenesis is revealed through dosage-sensitive interactions with disabled. Cell 58: : 103–113. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=2502313&site=ehost-live via the Internet.
  18. 18. Awasaki T, Saito M, Sone M, Suzuki E, Sakai R, et al. (2000) The Drosophila trio plays an essential role in patterning of axons by regulating their directional extension. Neuron 26: 119–131.
  19. 19. Bateman J, Shu H, Van Vactor D (2000) The guanine nucleotide exchange factor trio mediates axonal development in the Drosophila embryo. Neuron 26: 93–106.
  20. 20. Newsome TP, Schmidt S, Dietzl G, Keleman K, Asling B, et al. (2000) Trio combines with dock to regulate pak activity during photoreceptor axon pathfinding in Drosophila. Cell 101: 283–294.
  21. 21. Nichols CD, Becnel J, Pandey UB. (2012) Methods to assay Drosophila behavior. The Journal of Visualized Experiments 61: , e3795, DOI:10.3791/3795.
  22. 22. Strasburger EH. (1932) Drosophila melanogaster meig. ein Einfuhrung in den Bau und die Entwicklung. Berlin: Julius Springer.
  23. 23. Ashburner M. (1989) Drosophila: A laboratory handbook. : Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press. 139–298 p.
  24. 24. Alpatov W (1929) Growth and variation of the larvae of Drosophila melanogaster.. The Journal of Experimental Zoology 52: 407.
  25. 25. Cook RK, Christensen SJ, Deal JA, Coburn RA, Deal ME, et al. (2012) The generation of chromosomal deletions to provide extensive coverage and subdivision of the Drosophila melanogaster genome. Genome Biol 13: R21–R21 10.1186/gb-2012-13-3-r21. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=22445104&site=ehost-live via the Internet.
  26. 26. Parks AL, Cook KR, Belvin M, Dompe NA, Fawcett R, et al.. (2004) Systematic generation of high-resolution deletion coverage of the Drosophila melanogaster genome. Nat Genet 36: : 288–292. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=14981519&site=ehost-live via the Internet.
  27. 27. Burbelo PD, Drechsel D, Hall A (1995) A conserved binding motif defines numerous candidate target proteins for both Cdc42 and rac GTPases. J Biol Chem 270: 29071–29074.
  28. 28. Oishi I, Sugiyama S, Liu Z, Yamamura H, Nishida Y, et al. (1997) A novel Drosophila receptor tyrosine kinase expressed specifically in the nervous system. Unique structural features and implication in developmental signaling. J Biol Chem 272: 11916–11923 10.1074/jbc.272.18.11916.
  29. 29. Brenman JE, Gao FB, Jan LY, Jan YN. (2001) Sequoia, a tramtrack-related zinc finger protein, functions as a pan-neural regulator for dendrite and axon morphogenesis in Drosophila. Dev Cell 1: : 667–677. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=11709187&site=ehost-live via the Internet.
  30. 30. Lasko PF, Pardue ML. (1988) Studies of the genetic organization of the vestigial microregion of Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics 120: : 495–502. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=3143621&site=ehost-live via the Internet.
  31. 31. Grueber WB, Jan LY, Jan YN. (2002) Tiling of the Drosophila epidermis by multidendritic sensory neurons. Development 129: : 2867–2878. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=12050135&site=ehost-live via the Internet.
  32. 32. Rayburn LYM, Gooding HC, Choksi SP, Maloney D, Kidd AR, et al.. (2003) Amontillado, the Drosophila homolog of the prohormone processing protease PC2, is required during embryogenesis and early larval development. Genetics 163: : 227–237. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=12586710&site=ehost-live via the Internet.
  33. 33. Gunsalus KC. (2008) A Caenorhabditis elegans genetic-interaction map wiggles into view. J Biol 7: : 8–8. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=18341704&site=ehost-live via the Internet.
  34. 34. Kann MG. (2007) Protein interactions and disease: Computational approaches to uncover the etiology of diseases. Brief Bioinform8: : 333–346. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=17638813&site=ehost-live via the Internet.
  35. 35. Boone C, Bussey H, Andrews BJ. (2007) Exploring genetic interactions and networks with yeast. Nat Rev Genet8: : 437–449. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=17510664&site=ehost-live via the Internet.
  36. 36. Costanzo M, Baryshnikova A, Myers CL, Andrews B, Boone C. (2011) Charting the genetic interaction map of a cell. Curr Opin Biotechnol 22: : 66–74. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=21111604&site=ehost-live via the Internet.
  37. 37. Bateman J, Van Vactor D (2001) The trio family of guanine-nucleotide-exchange factors: Regulators of axon guidance. J Cell Sci 114: 1973–1980.
  38. 38. Goossens T, Kang YY, Wuytens G, Zimmermann P, Callaerts-Vegh Z, et al. (2011) The Drosophila L1CAM homolog neuroglian signals through distinct pathways to control different aspects of mushroom body axon development. Development (Cambridge) 138: 1595–1605 10.1242/dev.052787.
  39. 39. Kurusu M, Awasaki T, Masuda-Nakagawa L, Kawauchi H, Ito K, et al.. (2002) Embryonic and larval development of the Drosophila mushroom bodies: Concentric layer subdivisions and the role of fasciclin II. Development 129: : 409–419. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=11807033&site=ehost-live via the Internet.
  40. 40. Chintapalli VR, Wang J, Dow JAT. (2007) Using FlyAtlas to identify better Drosophila melanogaster models of human disease. Nat Genet 39: : 715–720. Available: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=17534367&site=ehost-live via the Internet.