Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Fractal fluctuations in exploratory movements predict differences in dynamic touch capabilities between children with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and typical development

  • Bruna S. Avelar,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Graduate Program in Rehabilitation Science, School of Physical Education, Physical Therapy, and Occupational Therapy, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais (UFMG), Belo Horizonte, Minas Gerais, Brazil

  • Marisa C. Mancini,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Graduate Program in Rehabilitation Science, School of Physical Education, Physical Therapy, and Occupational Therapy, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais (UFMG), Belo Horizonte, Minas Gerais, Brazil

  • Sergio T. Fonseca,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Graduate Program in Rehabilitation Science, School of Physical Education, Physical Therapy, and Occupational Therapy, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais (UFMG), Belo Horizonte, Minas Gerais, Brazil

  • Damian G. Kelty-Stephen,

    Roles Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, Grinnell College, Grinnell, Iowa, United States of America

  • Débora M. de Miranda,

    Roles Conceptualization, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Pediatrics, School of Medicine, UFMG, Belo Horizonte, Minas Gerais, Brazil

  • Marco Aurélio Romano-Silva,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Mental Health, School of Medicine, UFMG, Belo Horizonte, Minas Gerais, Brazil

  • Priscila A. de Araújo,

    Roles Investigation

    Affiliation Graduate Program in Rehabilitation Science, School of Physical Education, Physical Therapy, and Occupational Therapy, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais (UFMG), Belo Horizonte, Minas Gerais, Brazil

  • Paula L. Silva

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft

    paula.silva@uc.edu

    Affiliations Graduate Program in Rehabilitation Science, School of Physical Education, Physical Therapy, and Occupational Therapy, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais (UFMG), Belo Horizonte, Minas Gerais, Brazil, Center for Cognition, Action & Perception, Department of Psychology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America

Abstract

Children with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) struggle to perform a host of daily activities. Many of these involve forceful interaction with objects and thus implicate dynamic touch. Therefore, deficits in dynamic touch could underlie functional difficulties presented by ADHD children. We investigated whether performance on a dynamic touch task (length perception by wielding) differ between children with ADHD and age-matched controls. We further examined whether this difference could be explained by fractal temporal correlations (wielding dynamics). Forty-two children (ADHD: 21; typically developing: 21) wielded unseen wooden rods and reported their perceived length in the form of magnitude productions. The rods varied in the magnitude of the first principal moment of inertia (I1). Three-dimensional displacements of hand and rod positions were submitted to Detrended Fluctuation Analysis to estimate trial-by-trial temporal correlations. Children with ADHD reported shorter length for rods with higher I1 than their typically developing peers, indicative of reduced sensitivity to mechanical information supporting dynamic touch. Importantly, temporal correlations in wielding dynamics moderated children’s usage of I1. This finding points to a role of exploratory movements in perceptual deficits presented by children with ADHD and, thus, should be considered a new potential target for interventions.

Introduction

Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is characterized by symptoms of inattention and/or hyperactivity-impulsivity associated with reduced independence at home and deficient academic performance at school [13]. In addition to functional limitations associated with cognitive behavioral symptoms, children with ADHD also have difficulties with the performance of perceptual-motor activities, which are key for successful functioning at home and school settings [4]. The present study aimed to determine whether perceptual deficits might be related to difficulties these children have to move objects in ways that promote learning about their properties and effective use.

Performance deficits in children with ADHD commonly appear in the context of daily activities that involve interaction with objects, such as silverware, scissors, tweezers, toothbrush, pencil, or ball [2,57]. To perform these activities successfully, children have to carefully coordinate their muscular forces to manipulate the object in a way that meets the activity requirements. This careful patterning requires keen perception of the object’s geometrical and dynamical properties [8,9], relying on a haptic perceptual subsystem called dynamic touch [9,10,11]. Perception by dynamic touch in turn requires effortful engagement with objects. Therefore, the difficulty in performing motor activities presented by children with ADHD may produce deficits in dynamic touch and vice-versa.

Dynamic touch supports accurate and reliable perception of a number of object properties, such as length [8,1214], width [13], heaviness [15], and center of percussion [16]. Manipulations of the mechanical properties of the hand-held objects have allowed researchers to consistently conclude that the basis for these accomplishments is the invariant resistance offered by hand-held objects to their motion (i.e., the objects’ mass moments). Deficits in dynamic touch can, thus, result when a pathological condition compromises processes that would typically guarantee sensitivity to objects’ mass moments.

To appreciate how an object’s mass moments might constrain perception by dynamic touch, consider the well-studied task of perceiving the length of hand-held rods by wielding. The first principal moment of inertia (I1) of rods constructed of the same homogenous materials relate reliably to their length. If one is to hold a rod firmly at its proximal end while pointing it forward, I1 will be related to the resistance the rod will offer to up-down and side-to-side accelerations [8,12]. Therefore, wielding the rod in these directions can create the mechanical stimulation that supports detection of I1 and, thus, perception of rod length. When the task is performed successfully, participants exploit the lawful relationship that exist between I1 and the length of homogenous rods—Length = a (I1)b, with b = .33 as expected from dimensional analysis.

The perception of length by wielding has been the paradigm of choice to study dynamic touch in children with and without developmental disability. Research has shown that perceptual performance improves with age [17,18], with typically developing (TD) children reaching adult performance between eight and ten years old [19]. Notably, children with clinical conditions, such as cerebral palsy and developmental coordination disorder (DCD), had lower accuracy in perceptual reports than TD children and reduced attunement to I1 [19,20]. It is unknown, however, whether individuals with ADHD show similar deficits. If they do, remediation requires an understanding of what might be limiting the appropriate use of mechanical information.

Understanding inter-individual differences in the use of mechanical information requires focusing on exploratory movements (wielding dynamics). There are multiple means to explore an object to successfully perceive its properties (e.g. moving the wrist, the elbow, and the shoulder or simply holding the object yield similar perceptual reports) [21,22]. Thus, traditional kinematic descriptions of exploratory movements cannot distinguish their effectiveness. Research has shown, however, that if we remove the overall trend in exploratory movements (overt or more static), the long-range correlations of left-over fluctuations (sometimes called “fractality”) predict interindividual differences in perceptual performance [23,24,2527].

Fractal fluctuations in haptic exploration prove to be critical for perceptual learning. They reflect the coordination of sampling information across various times scales: (a) detection in short-term information pick-up; (b) calibration with medium-term rescaling judgments of object properties and (c) attunement at the longer-term as participants change which object variable they attend to [28,29]. Crucial for present purposes, research showed that wielding dynamics (i.e., long-range temporal correlations in exploratory movements) predicts how an individual uses information in dynamic touch tasks on a trial-by-trial basis and, thus, explains inter-individual differences in perceptual performance and perceptual learning [23,24,2527]—even in the case of non-overt postural sway [30].

We proposed to investigate the performance of children with ADHD in dynamic touch and examined whether fractal measures of their exploratory movements explain their differences in dynamic touch from TD children. Initially, we might well anticipate that the perceptual-motor difficulties and, indeed, difficulties following rules [31] could lead children with ADHD to struggle to meet the demands of the basic dynamic touch procedure, e.g., maintaining a firm grasp on the wielded object. Hence, we tracked exploratory movements with motion-capture both on the hand, in accordance with past dynamic-touch research, but also on the rod itself. This elaboration of the standard method would not only anticipate the failure of children with ADHD to maintain firm grasp according to the experimental instructions, but it would also have the added benefit of elaborating early evidence that incidental vibrations of the rod itself provide perceptual information above and beyond wielding behaviors [32,33]. For instance, it may be that the fractal structure of rod vibrations offers a competing source of information, and children with ADHD might even use this source for their dynamic-touch reports. We hypothesized that (1) children with ADHD would be less accurate and reliable in the length perception task compared to TD children, and (2) differences in performance would be explained by reduced reliance on I1, which in turn would be associated with the fractal structure of wielding dynamics.

Materials and methods

Participants

The ethics review committee of the Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais (UFMG, Brazil) approved the study. In compliance with the approved research protocol, informed consent was obtained, in writing, from all participants and their caregivers prior to their inclusion in the study.

Initially, 24 children with diagnosis of ADHD established according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5 [1]) composed the ADHD group and 22 TD children with no history of any pathology or clinical disorder composed the TD group. Specifically, children with ADHD and their parents underwent a semi-structured psychiatric diagnostic interview with the Brazilian version of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL). These children were recruited through convenience sampling from the child psychiatry outpatient clinic at Hospital das Clínicas /Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais (UFMG). TD children were recruited from the local community. Parents of children with ADHD agreed not to administer medication (psychostimulants) for 24 hours prior to their experimental section. UFMG’s ethics review committee approved the study.

All children initially selected to participate in the study were submitted to the Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices (Raven’s CP; [34]). The Raven’s CP was used to document children’s cognitive ability. Only children with average intellectual ability, that is, those able to understand and cooperate with verbal instructions, continued in the study (Table 1). Three children with ADHD were excluded due to their scores on the Raven’s CP. Parents from both groups completed the Brazilian version of the Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham Questionnaire—version IV (SNAP-IV), a behavior rating scale used to assess symptoms of ADHD (subscales: inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity; [35]). Following the diagnostic criteria established by the DSM-V, only children who had at least six items rated "quite a bit" or "very much" in one or more subscales of SNAP-IV were included in the ADHD group. Only children who had less than six items rated "quite a bit" or "very much" in any subscale of SNAP-IV were included in TD group. At this stage, one TD child was excluded and referred for medical evaluation. Table 1 provides a detailed characterization of each group.

Experimental procedures

To perform the experimental task (length perception by wielding), the children sat on a chair next to an opaque black curtain positioned on their right side. Their hand was put through a slit in the curtain until the forearm was comfortably resting on an armrest, with the wrist a few centimeters beyond its edge to allow free movement. An experimenter instructed participants to maintain full grip of the rods at all times and move it using the wrist joint. Participants provided length reports, in the form of magnitude productions, by aligning the visible marker to the perceived position of the tip of the rod using a string and pulley system. This system was positioned on a 2 meters table such that it was easily reachable with their left hand. A tape measure (not visible to participants) was placed end-to-end on the stick so that the experimenter could quantify participants’ perceived length (PL−cm; Fig 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Experimental set up: Occluded rods were wielded by hand as participants adjusted a visible marker to indicate the location of the tip.

Four ProReflex cameras recorded the motions of reflexive markers positioned at the hand and rod, which were used to characterize the exploratory movements underlying task performance.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0217200.g001

Two wooden rods with different lengths were used in the task. The mass distribution of each rod was manipulated by attaching a steel cylinder at positions corresponding to 50%, 70%, and 90% of its length. By crossing the two different lengths with three mass positions, six rods were created differing in the magnitude of their I1, computed with standard methodology, using the wrist as the axis of rotation [12]. Table 2 provides details about the characteristics of the rods. Inertial Length (LI) is the expected length report for each rod assuming appropriate attunement to I1. To compute LI of our experimental rods (Table 2), we employed the power function relating the length of homogenous rods and I1 [Length = a (I1)b] [12,36]. We estimated the values of a (.309) and b (.333) using the values of length and I1 of a series of eight homogeneous rods with diameter and density equal to the experimental rods.

thumbnail
Table 2. Characteristics of the rods: Diameter (m); density (kg/m3); length (m); mass position (%); values of maximum principal moment of inertia—I1 (Kg/m2), and inertial length—LI (m).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0217200.t002

On each experimental trial, a rod was positioned in the child’s hand who was reminded to grasp it firmly and move it around the wrist while adjusting the visible marker until he or she felt it was aligned with the tip of the rod. When the child indicated he or she was done, the experimenter recorded PL and moved the marker back to its initial position. Each rod was presented three times, in random order, yielding a total of 18 trials. To characterize the wielding dynamics, the position of the hand and the position of the rod over time were recorded at a sampling frequency of 100 Hz using the Qualisys motion capture system (Qualisys Inc, Gothenburg, Sweden) with four properly calibrated ProReflex cameras. The cameras monitored the position of two infrared reflective markers placed (1) on the distal extremity of the rod and (2) on the right hand of the child (the anatomical snuffbox region; Fig 1). In each trial, recordings started when the child was given full control of the rod and told to start exploring and finished when he or she indicated the task was completed. Participants were allowed as much time as needed for each trial and could request a break if they felt tired.

Several procedures were used to familiarize the children with the task. Children were given three rods (not used during the experiment) and were instructed on how to use the pulley system to indicate their length, with and without vision. The experimental trials began only after children demonstrated clear understanding of the restrictions on the exploratory procedure (e.g., did not raise the forearm from the armrest during exploration; move the rod through the wrist only), and followed task instructions (e.g., matched the marker on the pulley system with the tip of the rod when visual information about the rod was available). We told children that the rods used for testing would be different from the ones used during practice.

Data analyses

Haptic perceptual performance.

The mean of the three length reports provided for each rod was computed to obtain a measure of PL. We computed the percent average deviation (AD%) of the three reports provided for each rod from PL and percent root mean square error (RMSE%) as measures of reliability and accuracy, respectively (see [37]). The latter measure expresses the average deviations of length reports from LI, or the expected length given I1.

Wielding dynamics.

We characterized wielding dynamics as temporal correlations (or fractality) in the detrended fluctuations in the exploratory movements of the hand and rod. Twenty-one participants in each of the two groups completed 18 trials generating a total of 756 recordings of three-dimensional positions of hand and rod over time. We computed time-series of three-dimensional hand and rod displacements by calculating the Euclidian distance between successive samples in the hand and rod position time-series, respectively (Fig 2). The mean length of the resulting time-series was 1952.77 data points (SE = 1320.76).

thumbnail
Fig 2.

Estimate the fractality of wielding dynamics in each trial of the reflective markers positioned on the hand (left) and the rod (right). (A) 3D position time-series of the hand and rod recorded during wielding (sampling rate = 100Hz). (B) Time-series of Euclidean displacements from the 3D position time-series of the hand and rod (C) Detrended Fluctuation Analysis (DFA) to estimate trial-by-trial alpha reflecting the relationship between magnitude of fluctuation and time-scale.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0217200.g002

To estimate the fractality of wielding dynamics, the 756 displacement time-series of the hand and rod were submitted to Detrended Fluctuation Analysis (DFA [38]; Fig 2). DFA estimates the association between amount of root-mean-square (RMS) variation in a signal (the displacement time-series) with the timescales of measurement. The first step of this analysis is to integrate the original signal, x(t), of length N, by computing the successive cumulative sums of difference scores. This procedure produces a new series y(t): (1) where is the time-series mean. The second step is to fit linear trends, yn(t), to nonoverlapping windows of length n (e.g., 8, 16, 32, 64, etc.) and compute the RMS over each fit. RMS is an average of the residuals around the linear fit and, therefore, captures the magnitude of fluctuation in the signal at the various window sizes. The RMS over each window size (with n < N/4) is described by the following fluctuation function F(n): (2) The third step is to plot F(n), that is, plot RMS against window size (DFA plot). On standard scales, F(n) is a power law. Therefore, in log-log coordinates, a linear trend is observed. The fourth and final step is to compute the slope α of a regression line fitting the DFA plot. The magnitude of α provides an estimate of the temporal correlations or variability structure in the time-series: α ≈ 0.5 indicates no temporal correlations or random pattern (i.e., white noise); α ≈ 1 indicates long-range correlations or persistent structure (i.e., fractal, 1/f, or pink noise); and α ≈ 1.5 indicates highly persistent structure (i.e., Brownian motion; [3941]).

Results and discussion

Dynamic touch performance

To determine whether perceptual performance is affected in children with ADHD, PL was submitted to an Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) with a between subjects’ factor (Group: ADHD vs TD) and a within subjects’ factor (Rod: six levels of I1). The analysis revealed a significant main effect of Group, F(1, 40) = 8.50, p = .006; = .175, with children in the ADHD group showing shorter PL (M = 39.08; SD = 10.67) than children in the TD group (M = 45.25, SD = 11.65). There was also a main effect of Rod, F(5, 200) = 127.44, p < . 0001; = .761. Linear contrast indicated a significant increase in PL as a function of I1, p < .0001; = .860. Finally, there was a significant Group x Rod interaction, F(5, 200) = 2.81, p = .041; = .066. Simple effect analyses showed that differences between groups were statistically significant only for rods 4, F(1, 40) = 5.39, p = .025; 5, F(1, 40) = 7.07, p = .011; and 6, F(1, 40) = 12.04, p = .001 (Fig 3).

thumbnail
Fig 3.

(A) Magnitude of the group difference in perceived length (PL) as a function rods and (B) Magnitude of the group difference in percent average deviation (AD%) and percent root mean square error (RMSE%). Asterisks indicate significant difference (p < 0.05). Vertical lines indicate standard error. ADHD Group: Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; TD Group: Typical development.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0217200.g003

To determine whether differences in performance were indicative of ADHD-related deficits in dynamic touch, AD% and RMSE% were compared between groups. Mann-Whitney tests were employed because both variables were not normally distributed. Results indicated differences between groups in both AD% (U = 129, p = .021) and RMSE% (U = 111, p = .006) (Fig 3). As smaller values of AD% and RMSE% indicate greater reliability and accuracy, respectively, children in the TD group had significantly better reliability and accuracy of PL than children in ADHD group, confirming our first hypothesis.

Dynamic touch in ADHD: Wielding dynamics moderate information use

The scaling exponents estimated for the displacement time series of the rod (αrod) and for the displacement time series of the hand (αhand) are consistent with fractal fluctuations in both groups [ADHD: Mαrod = 1.1 (SD = .11), Mαhand = 1.07 (SD = .10); TD: Mαrod = 1.09 (SD = .23), Mαhand = 1.05 (SD = .09)]. We used linear mixed effect regression model to estimate the contributions of I1, wielding dynamics (fractal scaling of hand and rod), Group (ADHD = 0, Typical = 1), and the interactions between these factors to trial-by-trial variations in length reports. Our model also included trial number to account for any changes in length reports due to experience with the task. All lower and higher order interactions were included.

The expected collinearity between αrod and αhand (r = 0.65, p < 0.05) recommended using Principal Component Analysis such that both variables could figure in the same stable model. This analysis generated two orthogonal variables (PC1 & PC2), each capturing the same proportion of total variance in the original variables. The linear combination representing PC1 (PC1 = 0.95* αrod + 0.31* αhand) indicates that its score is primarily affected by and change in direct proportion to αrod. In contrast, the linear combination representing PC2 (PC2 = -0.31* αrod + 0.95 αhand) indicates that its score is primarily affected by and change in direct proportion to αhand (note the similar weighing). Thus, for the sake of clarity, we will hereafter refer to PC1 as PCrod and to PC2 as PChand. The effect of adding predictors required comparison of deviance between model and data (-2 Log Likelihood or -2LL) between a larger model and a simpler nested model. Change in -2LL is distributed as chi-square with degrees of freedom equal to the difference in the number of parameters between nested models allowing test of statistical significance. Only interactions that significantly improved fit were kept in the final model.

Our final model included the following interactions: PCrod × PChand × Trial, PCrod × I1 × Group, PChand × I1 × Group as well as all component lower-order interactions and main effects (Table 3). All potential higher order interactions linking these two families of interactions failed to improve model fit, suggesting that the interaction PCrod × PChand × Trial was not modified by I1 or Group. This model generates predictions that correlate well with the actual trial-by-trial length reports, r = .82, p < .0001, indicating that the model explains roughly 67.1% of the observed variability in the length reports provided by participants.

thumbnail
Table 3. Linear mixed effect regression model for length reports.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0217200.t003

General effects.

Results of the model replicated usual findings in dynamic touch research: length reports were related to (a) I1 and (b) wielding dynamics, regardless of diagnosis. The model advances current research on dynamic touch because it captures the specific contributions of hand and rod’s movement fractality to length reports. Results show a significant main effect of PChand, such that higher hand’s movement fractality was related to shorter length reports. It is possible that the relationship between hand exploratory movement fractality and length reports is mediated by the mechanical properties of the rods. Specifically, greater fractality of hand’s movement can be expected with rods of lower I1, which are tied to shorter PL. This interpretation is supported by research showing that (a) hand-held objects with lower I1 impose weaker constraints on exploratory movement [42], and (b) these weaker constraints allow fluctuations to distribute over a wider range of time scales and so to embody stronger fractal structure [4346].

No main effect of PCrod on length reports was observed. However, rod’s movement fractality magnifies the effect of hand’s movement fractality on length reports when both change in the same direction (PCrod x PChand), in particular at earlier trials (PCrod x PChand x Trial). It is not clear at the moment what might explain the “magnifying” effect of rod’s movement fractality nor why it decreases with repeated trials. However, the significant interaction effects of I1 with both PCrod and PChand suggests that both rod and hand’s movement fractality are required to account for the use of mechanical information during dynamic touch tasks. Importantly for present purposes, the particular wielding dynamics used to modify the emphasis on I1 differed between ADHD and TD groups (PChand x I1 x Group and PCrod x I1 x Group) and will be addressed in more detail in turn.

Group-specific effects.

The effect of I1 on length reports was significantly different between groups (I1 x Group), suggesting distinct scaling relations between the informational variable and the to-be-perceived property (i.e., different calibration). Children in the TD group changed their perceptual reports more than children with ADHD as a function of changes in I1. As noted, the effect of I1 on length reports was modified by wielding dynamics in different ways by children with and without ADHD. For TD children, the effect of I1 was emphasized when PChand was lower (I1 x PChand x Group) and PCrod was higher (I1 x PCrod x Group) and for children with ADHD, the effect of I1 was emphasized when PChand was higher (I1 x PChand) and PCrod was lower (I1 x PCrod). In conjunction, these results support our second hypothesis and will be addressed in more details in the general discussion.

General discussion

The present study provides empirical evidence that dynamic touch is affected in children with ADHD adding to the growing literature about perceptual deficits in this population (cf. [47] for review). Results reinforce the need to study the implications of perceptual deficits in general and dynamic touch in particular to the challenging functional problems presented by children with ADHD. Children with ADHD underestimated the magnitudes of rod length, in particular for longer rods (Fig 3). Notably, a similar pattern of performance was demonstrated by children with DCD [20]. ADHD and DCD are developmental disorders that present common characteristics such as difficulty in tasks that require manipulation of objects [4,48]. Therefore, deficits in dynamic touch may be at the basis of difficulties in manual skills presented by children with these (commonly co-occurring) medical conditions.

One might interpret the underestimation of object length observed in children with ADHD and DCD as signaling a generally reduced receptivity of mechanical stimulation. This interpretation would be consistent with evidence of lower activation of neural areas related to somatosensory perception [4951] (cf. [48]). However, if deficits in dynamic touch were a simple function of impaired neural receptivity, underestimation would be similar across rods or, at best, differences in performance would be reduced for rods with higher I1 that provide relatively greater magnitudes of mechanical stimulation. The opposite was true for both children with ADHD (present study) and DCD [20]. Thus, this pattern of result would be consistent with differences in how mechanical information supporting dynamic touch is used.

The results of our regression model indeed support this assertion: while children in both groups (ADHD and TD) increased length reports with increases in I1, changes in perceived length associated with changes in I1 were less pronounced in children with ADHD (Table 3). The lower emphasis on I1 can explain their lower accuracy and reliability in the task when compared to TD children (Fig 3). Together, these results suggest that children with ADHD were sensitive to trial-by-trial variations in the value of I1, but were relatively less sensitive to information about the appropriate scaling of I1 to length that becomes available over longer time scales. In other words, these atypical developmental trajectories leave intact those mechanisms supporting information detection (i.e., the more or less instantaneous pick up of information) and rather affect calibration mechanisms refining use of information over time.

We hypothesized that the dynamics of exploratory movements predict differences in the use of information by children with ADHD when compared to their TD peers. Results supported this hypothesis. There were significant differences between groups in how the fractality of exploratory movements of the rod and hand moderated children’s emphasis on I1. In TD children, lower hand fractality and greater rod fractality was related to increased emphasis on I1 (Table 3). In contrast, children with ADHD increased emphasis on I1 when the exploratory movement was characterized by greater hand fractality and lower rod fractality (Table 3).

It is not fully clear at this point what underlies the particular differences between groups in how fractality was related to the use of I1. However, previous studies that investigated the effect of rod vibration on dynamic touch performance provide grounds for an initial speculation [32,33]. Results showed an increased emphasis on I1 over time associated with striking, adding to recent results implicating the role of rod vibrations in perceptual calibration even when instructions to participants only entail firm grasp and wielding [32,33]. As noted, when lower fractality of the hand’s exploratory movement co-occurred with high fractality of the rod’s movement, sensitivity to I1 increased in TD children. This result may indicate that when TD children constrained hand motions during exploration so as to enhance stimulation by richly structured rod vibrations within the hand, their perceptual performance became more tightly coupled to the relevant informational parameter, possibly through a process of self-guided calibration.

Children with ADHD commonly present excessive, mildly disorganized overt motor activity and, thus, are less prone to constrain hand motions and direct their processes of stimulation. It is possible, nonetheless, that in trials where children with ADHD demonstrated higher movement fractality, the less ordered overt motions produced by children with ADHD resulted in a general increase in perceptual sensitivity. In particular, greater fractality in hand motions suggests stronger interactions of overt exploratory activity across scales, which can be expected to promote more efficient information sampling and, thereby, explain the increased emphasis on I1 (e.g., see [41] for a discussion of fractal systems as “space filling”). The implication is that, with the right level of fractality, the seemingly irrelevant motor activity of children with ADHD might play a non-obviously functional role.

A potential functional role of hyperactivity has indeed been previously demonstrated. For example, Sarver et al. [52] have shown that the level of performance in a memory task by children with ADHD was directly proportional to the degree of unrelated motor activity they produced during task performance. It is possible that the heightened motor activity enhanced multi-scale fluctuations that increase perceptual awareness of the environment, freeing up resources for the cognitive task. If this hypothesis is confirmed, cognitive-behavioral interventions designed to directly reduce excessive movements should be reconsidered ([5355]). At a larger scale, beyond the specific scale of ADHD research, converging lines of research in movement science and in biological sciences more generally are pointing increasingly towards clearer appreciation that higher-dimensional, “noisier,” more random or stochastic processes are a key part of healthy developmental trajectories [5659]. The notion of noise supporting biological order is not exactly new [60], but it can be “nonobvious” or seem “unrelated” to researchers not knowing to seek evidence of its role [61], and that evidence can go entirely undetected with modeling that does not make it explicit [62]. The present work is the latest example of how motor variability can be a window on the development of individual differences in perceptual-motor response—a window made clearer in some cases by fractal modeling [25,63,64].

An immediate question for clinical applications of this insight might be: So, if “noisy” movements can support developmental trajectories, can we leverage endogenous fluctuations with unobtrusive noise-based interventions and therapies? The short answer seems to be “yes.” Noise-based stimulation below sensory threshold can make both posture and gait more stable when applied to the plantar surface of the foot [6567] and can make breathing more regular when applied to an infant’s mattress [68,69]. These noise-based stimulations appear to work better for individuals with greater apparent pathology or instability [70,71]. Perhaps, noise-based interventions may provide an unobtrusive but fruitful way to facilitate functional performance of children with ADHD [72].

Conclusion

The present study showed that children with ADHD have deficits in dynamic touch that may be crucial for the organization of effective, task and context sensitive movement patterns. These deficits were manifested as reduced sensitivity to mechanical information that supports perception of object properties by wielding. Importantly, differences in haptic perceptual performance between children with and without ADHD were related to the temporal correlations (degree of fractality) observed in the subtle, seemingly irrelevant fluctuations of exploratory movements. In general lines, this result can be taken to reflect differences in the coordination of multiscale processes involved in information sampling and use (e.g., coordination of the moment-to-moment pick up of information with the longer time-scale processes that support appropriate calibration). Future studies using a learning paradigm can be designed to better gauge the adaptability of perceptual performance of children with ADHD when explicit knowledge of results is provided over time.

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully acknowledge all participants, members of the Núcleo de Investigações sobre a Impulsivade e Atenção (NITIDA), and Isabel Gomes and Gabriela Silvestrini for help in data collection. Financial support for this study was granted by the Brazilian government agencies National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq), Coordination for the Improvement of Higher-Level Personnel (CAPES, Finance Code 001) and by the Research Support Foundation from the State of Minas Gerais (FAPEMIG). DGKS acknowledges the generous internal support from Grinnell College.

References

  1. 1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, Fifth Edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. pp. 1–947.
  2. 2. Dunn L, Coster WJ, Orsmond GI, Cohn ES. Household task participation of children with and without attentional problems. Phys Occupat Ther Pediatrics 2009; 29: 258–273.
  3. 3. Kawabata Y, Tseng WL, Gau SSF. Symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and social and school adjustment: the moderating roles of age and parenting. J Abnormal Child Psychol 2012;40: 177–188.
  4. 4. Kaiser ML, Schoemaker MM, Albaret JM, Geuze RH. What is the evidence of impaired motor skills and motor control among children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)? Systematic review of the literature. Res Developmental Disabil 2015;36: 338–357.
  5. 5. Lavasani NM, Stagnitti K. A study on fine motor skills of Iranian children with attention deficit/hyper activity disorder aged from 6 to 11 years. Occupat Ther Intl 2011;18: 106–114.
  6. 6. Scharoun SM, Bryden PJ, Otipkova Z, Musalek M, Lejcarova A. Motor skills in Czech children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and their neurotypical counterparts. Res Developmental Disabil 2013;34: 4142–4153.
  7. 7. Pan C. Y., Tsai C. L., & Chu C. H. (2009). Fundamental movement skills in children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 39(12), 1694. pmid:19588236
  8. 8. Carello C, Silva PL, Kinsella-Shaw JM, Turvey MT. Muscle-based perception: theory, research and implications for rehabilitation. Brazil J Phys Ther 2008;12: 339–350.
  9. 9. Gibson JJ. The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin; 1966. pp. 1–335.
  10. 10. Turvey MT. Dynamic touch. Am Psychol 1996;51: 1134–1152. pmid:8937263
  11. 11. Turvey MT, Carello C. Dynamic touch. In: Epstein W, Rogers S, editors. Perception of space and motion. New York: Academic Press; 1995. pp. 401–490.
  12. 12. Fitzpatrick P, Carello C, Turvey MT. Eigenvalues of the inertia tensor and exteroception by the “muscular sense”. Neurosci 1994;60: 551–568.
  13. 13. Turvey MT, Burton G, Amazeen EL, Butwill M, Carello C. Perceiving the width and height of a hand-held object by dynamic touch. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perf 1998;24: 35–48.
  14. 14. Carello C, Kinsella-Shaw J, Amazeen EL, Turvey MT. Peripheral neuropathy and object length perception by effortful (dynamic) touch: A case study. Neurosci Lett 2006;405: 159–163. pmid:16884850
  15. 15. Amazeen EL. Perceptual independence of size and weight by dynamic touch. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perf 1999;25: 102–119.
  16. 16. Carello C, Thuot S, Anderson KL, Turvey MT. Perceiving the sweet spot. Percept 1999;28: 307–320.
  17. 17. Kloos H, Amazeen EL. Perceiving heaviness by dynamic touch: An investigation of the size-weight illusion in preschoolers. Brit J Developmental Psychol 2002;20: 171–118.
  18. 18. Fitzpatrick P, Flynn N, Dynamic touch perception in preschool children. Ecol Psychol 2010;22: 89–118.
  19. 19. Ocarino JM, Fonseca ST, Silva PL, Gonçalves GG, Souza TR, Mancini MC. Dynamic touch is affected in children with cerebral palsy. Hum Mov Sci 2014;33: 85–96 pmid:24054355
  20. 20. Wade MG, Tsai CL, Stoffregen T, Chang CH, Chen FC. Perception of object length via manual wielding in children with and without developmental coordination disorder. J Mot Behav 2016;48: 13–19. pmid:25927247
  21. 21. Pagano CC, Fitzpatrick P, Turvey MT. Tensorial basis to the constancy of perceived object extent over variations of dynamic touch. Percept Psychophys 1993;54: 43–54. pmid:8351187
  22. 22. Burton G, Turvey MT. Attentionally splitting the mass distribution of hand-held rods. Percept Psychophys 1991;50: 129–140. pmid:1945735
  23. 23. Palatinus Z, Dixon JA, Kelty-Stephen DG. (2013). Fractal fluctuations in quiet standing predict the use of mechanical information for haptic perception. Ann Biomed Eng 2013;41: 1625–1634. pmid:23188561
  24. 24. Palatinus Z, Kelty-Stephen DG, Kinsella-Shaw J, Carello C, Turvey MT. Haptic perceptual intent in quiet standing affects multifractal scaling of postural fluctuations. J Exper Psychol Human Percept Perf 2014;40: 1808–1818.
  25. 25. Kelty-Stephen DG, Dixon JA. Interwoven fluctuations during intermodal perception: Fractality in head sway supports the use of visual feedback in haptic perceptual judgments by manual wielding. J Exper Psychol Hum Percept Perf 2014;40: 2289–2309.
  26. 26. Stephen DG, Arzamarski R, Michaels CF. The role of fractality in perceptual learning: exploration in dynamic touch. J Exper Psychol Hum Percept Perf 2010;36: 1161.
  27. 27. Stephen DG, Hajnal A. Transfer of calibration between hand and foot: Functional equivalence and fractal fluctuations. Attent Percept Psychophys 2011;73: 1302–1328.
  28. 28. Michaels CF, Arzamarski R, Isenhower RW, Jacobs DM. Direct learning in dynamic touch. J Exper Psychol Hum Percept Perf 2008;34: 944–957
  29. 29. Withagen R, Michaels CF. The role of feedback information for calibration and attunement in perceiving length by dynamic touch. J Exper Psychol Hum Percept Perf 2005;31: 1379–1390.
  30. 30. Hajnal A, Clark JD, Doyon JK, Kelty-Stephen DG. Fractality of body movements predicts perception of affordances: Evidence from stand-on-ability judgments about slopes. J Exper Psychol Hum Percept Perf 2018;44: 836–841.
  31. 31. Barkley RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull 1997;121: 65–94. pmid:9000892
  32. 32. Kelty-Stephen DG, Eddy CL. Self-trained perception need not be veridical: striking can exaggerate judgment by wielding and can transfer exaggeration to new stimuli. Attent Percept Psychophys 2015;77: 1854–1862.
  33. 33. Stephen DG, Arzamarski R. Self-training of dynamic touch: Striking improves judgment by wielding. Attent Percept Psychophys 2009;71: 1717–1723.
  34. 34. Pasquali L, Wechsler S, Bensusan E. Matrizes Progressivas do Raven Infantil: um estudo de validação para o Brasil. Avaliação Psicológica Interam J Psychol Assess 2002;1: 95–110.
  35. 35. Mattos P, Pinheiro MA, Rohde LAP, Pinto D. Apresentação de uma versão em português para uso no Brasil do instrumento MTA-SNAP-IV de avaliação de sintomas de transtorno do déficit de atenção/hiperatividade e sintomas de transtorno desafiador e de oposição. Revista Psiquiatria Rio Grande Sul. Porto Alegre 2006;28: 290–297.
  36. 36. Carello C, Turvey MT. Rotational invariants and dynamic touch. In: Heller M, editor. Touch, representation and blindness. New York: Oxford University Press; 2000. pp. 27–66.
  37. 37. Hajnal A, Fonseca S, Harrison S, Kinsella-Shaw J, Carello C. Comparison of dynamic (effortful) touch by hand and foot. J Mot Behav 2007;39: 82–88. pmid:17428754
  38. 38. Peng CK, Buldyrev SV, Havlin S, Simons M, Stanley HE, Goldberger AL. Mosaic organization of DNA nucleotides. Phys Rev E 1994;49: 1685–1689.
  39. 39. Coey CA, Washburn A, Hassebrock J, Richardson MJ. (2016). Complexity matching effects in bimanual and interpersonal syncopated finger tapping. Neurosci Lett 2016;616: 204–210. pmid:26840612
  40. 40. Ihlen EAF. Introduction to multifractal detrended fluctuation analysis in Matlab. Front Physiol 2012;3: 141. pmid:22675302
  41. 41. Eke A, Herman P, Kocsis L, Kozak LR. (2002). Fractal characterization of complexity in temporal physiological signals. Physiol Meas 2002;23: R1–R38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1088/0967-3334/23/1/201 pmid:11876246
  42. 42. Goodman L, Riley MA, Mitra S, Turvey MT. (2000). Advantages of rhythmic movements at resonance: minimal active degrees of freedom, minimal noise, and maximal predictability. J Mot Behav 2000;32: 3–8. pmid:11008266
  43. 43. Kuznetsov N, Wallot S. (2011). Effects of accuracy feedback on fractal characteristics of time estimation. Front Integr Neurosci 2011;5: 62. pmid:22046149
  44. 44. Stephen DG, Dixon JA. The self-organization of insight: Entropy and power laws in problem solving. J Problem Solving 2009;2: 72–101.
  45. 45. Van Orden G. Voluntary performance. Medicina 2010;46: 581–594. pmid:21252592
  46. 46. Stephen DG, Holden JG, Mirman D, Dixon JA. Multifractal dynamics in the emergence of cognitive structure. Topics Cognit Sci 2012;4: 51–62.
  47. 47. Ghanizadeh A. Sensory processing problems in children with ADHD, a systematic review. Psychiatr Investig 2011;8: 89–94
  48. 48. Goulardins JB, Rigoli D, Licari M, Piek JP, Hasue RH, Oosterlaan J, Oliveira JA. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and developmental coordination disorder: Two separate disorders or do they share a common etiology. Behav Brain Res 2015;292: 484–492 pmid:26168770
  49. 49. McLeod KR, Langevin LM, Goodyear BG, Dewey D. Functional connectivity of neural motor networks is disrupted in children with developmental coordination disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. NeuroImage Clin 2014;4: 566–575. pmid:24818082
  50. 50. Querne L, Berquin P, Vernier-Hauvette MP, Fall S, Deltour L, Meyer ME et al. Dysfunction of the attentional brain network in children with developmental coordination disorder: a fMRI study. Brain Res 2008;1244: 89–102. pmid:18718456
  51. 51. Yu-Feng Z, Yong H, Chao-Zhe Z, Qing-Jiu C, Man-Qiu S, Meng L. et al. Altered baseline brain activity in children with ADHD revealed by resting-state functional MRI. Brain Develop 2007, 29(2): 83–91.
  52. 52. Sarver DE, Rapport MD, Kofler MJ, Raiker JS, Friedman LM. Hyperactivity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Impairing deficit or compensatory behavior?. J Abnormal Child Psychol 2015;43: 1219–1232.
  53. 53. Tryon W, Tryon G, Kazlausky T, Gruen W, Swanson J. Reducing hyper-activity with a feedback actigraph: initial findings. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2006;11: 607–617. pmid:17163228
  54. 54. Barkley RA. Psychosocial treatments for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children. J Clin Psychiatr 2002;63:36–43.
  55. 55. Wells KC, Pelham WE, Kotkin RA, Hoza B, Abikoff HB, Abramowitz A. et al. Psychosocial treatment strategies in the MTA study: Rationale, methods, and critical issues in design and implementation. J Abnormal Child Psychol 2000;28: 483–505.
  56. 56. Sternad D. It’s not (only) the mean that matters: Variability, noise, and exploration in skill learning. Curr Opinion Behav Sci 2018;20: 183–195.
  57. 57. McDonnell M, Ward L. What is stochastic resonance? Definitions, misconceptions, debates, and its relevance to biology. PLoS Comput Biol 2009;5:e1000348. pmid:19562010
  58. 58. McDonnell M, Ward L. The benefits of noise in neural systems: Bridging theory and experiment. Nat Rev Neurosci 2011;12: 415–426. pmid:21685932
  59. 59. Cluff T, Balasubramaniam R. Motor learning characterized by changing Levy distributions. PLoS ONE 2009;4: e5998. pmid:19543399
  60. 60. Turing AM. The chemical basis of morphogenesis. Phil Trans Royal Soc Lond B 1952:237, 37–72
  61. 61. Gottlieb G. On the epigenetic evolution of species-specific perception: The developmental manifold concept. Cognit Develop 2007;17: 1287–1300.
  62. 62. Molenaar PCM. On the implications of the classical ergodic theorems: Analysis of developmental processes has to focus on intra-individual variation. Developmental Psychobiol 2008;50: 60–69.
  63. 63. Ashkenazy Y, Hausdorff JM, Ivanov PCh, Stanley HE. A stochastic model of human gait dynamics. Physica A 2002;316: 662–670.
  64. 64. Carver NS, Bojovic D, Kelty-Stephen DG. Multifractal foundations of visually-guided aiming and adaptation to the prismatic perturbation. Hum Mov Sci 2017;55: 61–72. pmid:28763703
  65. 65. Galica A, Kang H, Priplata A, D’Andrea S, Starobinets O, Sorond F et al. Subsensory vibrations to the feet reduce gait variability in elderly fallers. Gait Posture 2009;30: 383–387. pmid:19632845
  66. 66. Lipsitz LA, Lough M, Niemi J, Travison T, Howlett H, Manor B. A shoe insole delivering subsensory vibratory noise improves balance and gait in healthy elderly people. Arch Phys Med Rehab 2015;96: 432–439.
  67. 67. Priplata A, Niemi J, Harry J, Lipsitz L, Collins J. Vibrating insoles and balance control in elderly people. Lancet 2003;362: 1123–1124. pmid:14550702
  68. 68. Bloch-Salisbury E, Indic P, Bednarek F, Paydarfar D. Stabilizing immature breathing patterns of preterm infants using stochastic mechanosensory stimulation. J Appl Physiol 2009;107: 1017–1027. pmid:19608934
  69. 69. Smith VC, Kelty-Stephen DG, Qureshi Ahmad M, Mao W, Cakert K, Osborne J et al. Stochastic resonance effect on apnea, bradycardia, and oxygenation: A randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics 2015;136: e1561–e1568 pmid:26598451
  70. 70. Priplata A, Patritti BL, Niemi JB, Hughes R, Gravelle DC, Lipsitz LA et al. Noise-enhanced balance control in patients with diabetes and patients with stroke. Ann Neurol 2006;59: 4–12. pmid:16287079
  71. 71. Stephen DG, Wilcox BJ, Niemi JB, Franz J, Kerrigan DC, D’Andrea SE. Baseline-dependent effect of noise-enhanced insoles on gait variability in healthy elderly walkers. Gait Posture 2012;38: 537–540.
  72. 72. Sikström S, Söderlund G. Stimulus-dependent dopamine release in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Rev 2007;114: 1047–1075.