Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Story time turbocharger? Child engagement during shared reading and cerebellar activation and connectivity in preschool-age children listening to stories

  • John S. Hutton ,

    John1.Hutton@cchmc.org

    Affiliations Division of General and Community Pediatrics, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America, Reading and Literacy Discovery Center, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America

  • Kieran Phelan,

    Affiliation Division of General and Community Pediatrics, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America

  • Tzipi Horowitz-Kraus,

    Affiliations Division of General and Community Pediatrics, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America, Reading and Literacy Discovery Center, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America, Pediatric Neuroimaging Research Consortium, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America, Communication Sciences Research Center, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America, Education Neuroimaging Center, Faculty of Education in Science and Technology, Technion, Haifa, Israel

  • Jonathan Dudley,

    Affiliations Reading and Literacy Discovery Center, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America, Communication Sciences Research Center, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America

  • Mekibib Altaye,

    Affiliations Pediatric Neuroimaging Research Consortium, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America, Communication Sciences Research Center, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America, Division of Radiology, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America

  • Thomas DeWitt,

    Affiliations Division of General and Community Pediatrics, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America, Reading and Literacy Discovery Center, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America

  • Scott K. Holland

    Affiliations Reading and Literacy Discovery Center, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America, Pediatric Neuroimaging Research Consortium, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America, Communication Sciences Research Center, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America, Division of Radiology, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America

Abstract

Expanding behavioral and neurobiological evidence affirms benefits of shared (especially parent-child) reading on cognitive development during early childhood. However, the majority of this evidence involves factors under caregiver control, the influence of those intrinsic to the child, such as interest or engagement in reading, largely indirect or unclear. The cerebellum is increasingly recognized as playing a “smoothing” role in higher-level cognitive processing and learning, via feedback loops with language, limbic and association cortices. We utilized functional MRI to explore the relationship between child engagement during a mother-child reading observation and neural activation and connectivity during a story listening task, in a sample of 4-year old girls. Children exhibiting greater interest and engagement in the narrative showed increased activation in right-sided cerebellar association areas during the task, and greater functional connectivity between this activation cluster and language and executive function areas. Our findings suggest a potential cerebellar “boost” mechanism responsive to child engagement level that may contribute to emergent literacy development during early childhood, and synergy between caregiver and child factors during story sharing.

Research highlights

  • Utilizes functional MRI to explore neural activation and functional connectivity during a story listening task in 4 year-old children, correlated with behaviors during a mother-child reading observation.
  • Explores neurobiological correlates of shared reading behavior intrinsic to the child, whereas studies to date have largely involved parent- or home-related predictors.
  • Suggests a potential neurobiological mechanism of indirect benefits of child interest and engagement in shared reading on emergent literacy skills described in behavioral studies.
  • Proposes a role of cerebellar association areas in narrative processing in young children potentially influenced by level of engagement, consistent with an increasingly recognized role of the cerebellum in cognitive function and learning.

Introduction

Illiteracy is a major public health issue disproportionately affecting low socioeconomic status (SES) populations, its annual cost estimated at over $1.2 trillion worldwide[1]. While an estimated 5–12% of reading difficulty has an organic etiology, notably dyslexia[2], the majority is attributable to inadequate resources, motivation and/or stimulation required to learn to read[1]. Recent reports have found 25% of Australian 9 year-olds scoring “below basic” for reading[3], 20% of 11 year-olds in in Britain[4], and 33 percent of US fourth graders[5], scores skewed markedly lower for low-SES and minority children[5, 6]. The seeds of environmental reading difficulty are planted early, many children arriving at school at a substantial disadvantage in readiness and increasingly unlikely to catch up with peers as academic demands accelerate[6, 7]. Thus, prevention and early intervention offer tremendous cost savings in terms of health and productivity[810].

As reading is an evolutionarily new, invented skill, there is no hardwired reading network in the brain. Instead, beginning in infancy, brain areas and networks adapted for other functions such as vision, language, and working memory are gradually integrated in response to reading exposure and practice.[11] This neurobiological process underlies emergent literacy, defined as “a developmental continuum between pre-reading and reading (involving) skills, knowledge, and attitudes that are precursors to reading and writing[12].” As parents are a child’s “first and most important teachers[13],” cognitive stimulation in the home, exemplified by shared reading[1416], can greatly influence outcomes[17]. Expanding behavioral and neurobiological evidence affirms benefits of shared (especially parent-child) reading on emergent literacy skills and supporting brain networks during early childhood[1820]. Citing this evidence base, the American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) recommends daily shared reading beginning at birth[21], echoed by reading advocacy groups and campaigns[22]. Early interventions designed to improve outcomes often target facets of home reading environment such as access to books, frequency of reading and quality of shared (a.k.a. dialogic) reading, all largely under parent/caregiver control[23]. By contrast, while factors intrinsic to the child such as interest and engagement in shared reading are often collateral objectives assumed to provide long-term benefits, relatively little is known about their influence on emergent literacy, with largely indirect effects described[12, 24, 25] and no neurobiological studies to date. As reading engagement tends to be low in low-SES households[26], often muting benefits of shared reading[27, 28], it is particularly important to understand mechanisms by which this reciprocal process may operate in the developing brain, and their potential influence on emergent literacy in this vulnerable population.

Recent studies have found positive correlation between quantitative and qualitative aspects of home reading environment and increased activation of brain circuits supporting foundational emergent literacy skills, including semantic processing[18, 19], visual imagery[18], receptive and expressive language[19, 29], and social-emotional integration[19]. These circuits reside largely in the left cerebral cortex, within classic language, limbic and association networks[30]. While long thought to solely support motor function, mounting imaging-based evidence over the past three decades has clearly documented a major cerebellar role in cognitive abilities, notably language, executive function and social-emotional processing[31], the majority of cerebral-cerebellar connectivity involving high-level association cortex[31, 32]. Given uniform cellular architecture and contralateral, polysynaptic feedback loops, it has been proposed that the cerebellum plays a similar modulatory (“smoothing”) role for motor and cognitive skills[31, 33], facilitating rehearsal, refinement and learning[31, 34, 35]. Consistent with this view, right-sided cerebellar activation has often been cited in studies of narrative comprehension and reading[36, 37], mapping to left prefrontal and language areas. Association between cerebellar microstructure and reading component skills in school-age children has also been recently described[38]. To what degree cerebellar development and function influence emergent literacy during rapid stages of neurodevelopment prior to kindergarten[39], and their influence by shared reading behaviors, have not been studied.

Neurobiological differences during early childhood predate[40] and can predict behavioral differences, including fundamental emergent literacy skills[4143], in turn predicting long-term outcomes[44, 45]. Thus, neuroimaging has the potential to provide mechanistic insights into traits, abilities (e.g. phonological awareness; [46]) and functional networks[47], synergistic with behavioral evidence and informing eco-bio-developmental models advocated by the AAP and US National Institutes of Health[48]. The objective of our study was to assess the relationship between child engagement during shared (mother-child) reading via direct observation and neural activation utilizing an established fMRI story listening task[49], in a sample of preschool-age children. Given indirect effects described in behavioral studies[12, 24], we hypothesized that children with higher levels of engagement would show greater activation in brain areas supporting attention (dorsal frontal-parietal) and narrative comprehension (left inferior frontal, superior temporal, inferior parietal).

Methods

Ethics

Of the 41 eligible mother-child dyads agreeing to participate by phone, 32 arrived for their study visit where written informed consent was obtained from mothers including a clear opt-out procedure. Families were compensated for time and travel, and our study, including the informed consent procedure and document, was approved by the Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center Institutional Review Board. Consent forms and other potentially identifying study materials were stored in a secure locked cabinet dedicated to this study.

Participants

Our study involved 22 mother-daughter dyads recruited from a longitudinal home injury prevention trial serving low-SES mothers, based at our institution (Cincinnati Home Injury Prevention (CHIP) trial. We identified 168 girls between 3.0 and 4.5 years of age, with a goal to enroll 40 who would be 4 years old (the oldest in the CHIP cohort) during their study visit, for a target sample size of 26 accounting for a 65% MRI success rate[50]. In addition to female gender and age at scanning, inclusion criteria were: full-term gestation, right-handed, native English speakers from monolingual households, no history of head trauma with loss of consciousness or stimulant use, and no standard contraindications to MRI. We chose to sample girls exclusively due to higher MRI success rates in our desired age range (67% versus 41%[51]), and well-described gender dimorphisms negligibly influencing our story listening task at this age[52, 53]. We identified 105 girls who would be approximately 4 years old during our study window (oldest in the cohort). Of these, 55 were unable to be contacted, 5 excluded due to developmental delay, and 4 for concerns about MRI. Of the 41 agreeing to participate, 32 arrived for their visit where written informed consent was obtained as described above. Of these, 22 completed MRI and video observation (69%).

Shared reading observation

Video observations of mother-child reading were obtained in accordance with an explicit protocol, including arrangement of the room and a script for research coordinators. Following MRI, the mother and child were directed to a private waiting room and encouraged to relax while discharge materials were being compiled. Mothers were informed that video would be in use for research purposes, though not specifically for reading assessment, a high-definition webcam unobtrusively mounted utilizing Microsoft Movie Maker software. On a table were clearly arranged: popular magazines, a table tent providing a Wi-Fi password, and a new children’s picture book (The Little Engine That Could, Watty Piper and Loren Long, Philomel Books 2005). If the mother or child did not spontaneously choose the book within 2 minutes, a research coordinator advised them that it was theirs to take home and to read it if they like, with no further coaching. After 15 minutes, the research coordinator entered to conclude the visit.

Child engagement scoring

Video observation scoring was performed by the principal investigator and 2 additional scorers (1 medical student, 1 Master’s-level research coordinator) explicitly trained in dialogic reading methodology via in-person practice sessions and an online module[54], as described in Hutton et al[19]. Child engagement during shared reading was scored via a scale developed by the investigator and reviewed by experts in measure design at our institution. Scores were as follows: 0 –not engaged (persistent attempts to do something else), 1 –somewhat engaged (often tries to do something else, redirected to the story easily), 2 –very engaged (mostly focused on the story, occasional shifts), 3 –extremely engaged (focused for the entire story with almost no shifts). To differentiate them from those with no interest whatsoever in the event of maternal refusal to read, it was agreed that children expressing strong and/or sustained interest in reading should be awarded a minimum score of 1, though actual scoring was at scorer discretion. Scoring from video practice sessions was critiqued and refined until scores reached alignment. Ambiguities or concerns during actual scoring were jointly resolved, and scores were double-entered into a secure REDCap® database[55]. Maternal engagement was also scored for use in a separate study, as described in Hutton, et al[19], including behaviors such as proximity during story sharing (e.g. on lap), use of child-adjusted voice (e.g. train sound effects), and encouraging the child to turn pages.

Functional MRI acquisition during the story listening task

Details of MRI acclimatization techniques including play-based desensitization are described by Vannest, et al[51]. MRI was performed using a 3T Philips Acheiva MRI system equipped with an Avotec audiovisual system and Real Eye, eye-tracking system. MRI acquisition included a 3D anatomical brain image for registration of functional data as well as functional MRI during stimulation with a story listening task described below. For fMRI, a time-series of 165, BOLD-weighted scans covering the entire brain with 38 slices in the axial plane were continuously acquired with voxel size 3.75x3.75 x5 mm at 2-second intervals (TR = 2) during the story listening task. All children were awake and non-sedated during fMRI.

Our story listening task consists of 11 alternating blocks of control and active conditions (6 and 5 respectively), of 30 seconds duration, for a total functional scanning time of 5 minutes 30 seconds[56]. During the active condition, a series of 5 stories of 9–10 sentences each read in a female voice was presented via headphones. The stories were designed by a speech-language pathologist with vocabulary, syntax and content appropriate for preschool-age children (download: https://www.irc.cchmc.org/software/pedaudio.php), utilized in numerous published studies[57, 58]. The control condition consisted of non-speech tones in a range of frequencies simulating human speech, to control for baseline acoustic processing. No visual stimulus was presented other than a blank screen during this task.

Functional MRI analysis

Data pre-processing was performed using FSL software (fMRI-Brain Software Library, Oxford, UK) [59]. We utilized the FEAT (fMRI Expert Analysis Tool, version 6.00) modality of FSL for our BOLD analyses[59]. Slice timing correction, spatial smoothing, intensity normalization and were all applied, followed by magnetic field map correction and registration to the structural 3D images. Following the initial pre-processing stage, functional image time series data were entered into a first-level general linear model for voxel-wise analysis to discern activation during story listening compared to tones for each child. The design matrix was comprised of hemodynamic response function-convolved paradigm timing parameters and six frame-wise motion-parameters (3 translation, 3 rotation) as covariates of no interest, to control for the effect of subject motion. A composite motion index was calculated for each subject and examined for correlations with story task timing, which was found to be very weak in all subjects (r2<0.09), and not correlated with child engagement scores (p>0.05). Magnitude of motion was also not problematic: average relative mean displacement was 0.50 ± 0.46 mm and the average percentage of frames with motion exceeding 2 mm was 5.9 ± 7.3%.

Contrast maps reflecting stories>tones activation for each subject were converted to Z-score maps with statistical threshold of p<0.05, applying a false discovery rate (FDR) correction for multiple voxel comparisons across the brain. These maps were entered into a second-level analysis carried out using the FLAME (FMRIB’s Local Analysis of Mixed Effects) stage 1 modality[6062], with Z-statistic images thresholded at p = 0.01, recently demonstrated to yield conservative results[63]. Thus, we obtained a whole-brain, group mean activation map representing mean neural activation listening to stories, minus activation listening to tones (i.e. activation attributable to the story task, excluding general acoustic processing) across all 22 children.

Linear regression with child engagement scores

A biostatistician uninvolved with video scoring performed all fMRI analyses. General linear regression was performed applying Z-score maps representing the contrast (stories>tones) for each child as the dependent variable and mean child engagement score as the predictor variable, across all 22 children. Increased power achieved via the inclusion of higher-motion children in our second level BOLD analysis (reduced Type 2 error rate) was deemed worthwhile and unlikely to introduce systemic bias or generate spurious findings (Type 1 errors), given weak correlation of subject motion with our stories task, modest magnitude of motion, and no correlation with child engagement scores. Given that our children were female and essentially the same age (4.1 +/- 0.2 years), gender and age were not considered as covariates. Household income and maternal education were considered but excluded, given lack of univariate correlation with activation for either variable. Activation maps (stories>tones) correlated with child engagement scores, along with summary statistics for size, intensity and location of activation clusters in normalized Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinate space[64] were generated, applying a threshold of p<0.05 and FDR correction for multiple comparisons across the brain.

Group-level statistical inference was carried out using FSL’s randomise function,[65] a nonparametric permutation test function providing robust control over false-positive results.[63] Cluster coordinates were translated into neurological cerebellar or Brodmann Areas (BA) via FSLView[59], referencing the Harvard-Oxford Cortical Structural (2mm scale) and probabilistic cerebellar atlases[66].

Task-based functional connectivity analysis

To assess functional connectivity between brain areas correlated with child engagement scores during our story listening task and other brain areas, a biostatistician performed a post hoc seed-to-voxel correlation analysis utilizing the CONN toolbox for SPM[67]. Functional data were preprocessed as described above. Additionally, prior to connectivity analysis, we also applied voxelwise temporal denoising of the BOLD signal via regression of zero- and first-order derivatives of the six motion parameters, regression of the top five principle components each of the average white matter and cerebrospinal fluid BOLD signal, and 0.008 Hz high pass filtering. As connectivity analyses are highly susceptible to bias from head movement, following preprocessing, frames with composite movement >1mm or global mean intensity z-score > +/-6 were demarcated as outliers using the Artifact Detection Tool (ART) in CONN and censored from the data. Children with >10% of outlier frames were excluded from further group analysis, leaving 12 subject data sets remaining for connectivity analysis with average relative mean displacement of 0.15 ± 0.13 mm and average percentage of frames with motion exceeding 2 mm of 0.9 ± 1.7%.

To maximize our power with this relatively small data set, we took a hypothesis-driven approach, informed by strong evidence of right-sided Crus I/II connectivity with language domains[33, 68] and high overlap (over 40%) of our BOLD activation regression map with this anatomical region. Thus, we defined as our “seed,” the time-series corresponding to the average BOLD signal intersecting with right-sided Crus I/II. Fisher-transformed Pearson correlation coefficients were computed between this seed and every other voxel’s BOLD signal. Voxels positively correlated with the seed region were tested for significance at the group level using a one-tailed T-test (height p < 0.001, extent FDR-corrected p < 0.05). Significant cluster coordinates were then translated into neurological Brodmann Areas or cerebellar areas via FSLView and the Talairach Client software tool[69].

Results

Demographic characteristics for our sample are described in Table 1.

Child engagement scores

A total of 6 mother-child dyads initiated shared reading spontaneously (27%; 3 prompted via the child and 3 via the mother), 10 read after prompting by a research coordinator (46%), and 6 did not read despite prompts (27%). Of mothers who read, 12 read the book in its entirety (75%) and 4 partially (25%). Of the 4 that stopped reading, 3 were due to the child losing interest (75%), and 1 due to the mother losing interest. Of the 6 mothers who did not read, 5 were due to maternal distraction (4 by maternal smartphone, 1 by another caregiver), 3 despite multiple entreaties from the child. Ten mothers (45%) used smartphones during the video session, 2 of them for the entire session. Children expressing strong and/or sustained interest in reading yet whose mothers did not read to them (n = 3) were awarded points based on their level of expressed interest (e.g. repeated entreaties, focused browsing of the book independently). All children received an engagement score irrespective of whether their mother agreed to read to them.

Inter-rater reliability for child engagement scores was high (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.87). All children received an engagement score based on their interest and engagement in shared reading, irrespective of whether their mother agreed to read to them, as described above. For each child, the mean of the 3 reviewer scores was used, resulting in a mean of 1.4 (somewhat to very engaged), and standard deviation of 1.0. Significant, positive correlation was found between child engagement scores and proximity during reading, child page turning, parental use of child-adjusted voice and finishing the book, and negatively correlated with maternal smartphone use (p<0.05). A positive trend was found between child and maternal reading engagement scores (p = 0.08). A summary is shown in Table 2 and Fig 1.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Observed child engagement scores.

Histogram and density curve for observed child engagement scores, including mean (dashed line) and standard deviation (sd). Total score for each child was the mean of 3 reviewer scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177398.g001

Group mean activation for the story listening task

Group mean activation for the stories condition compared to baseline tones involved bilateral, left-lateralized cortical and subcortical regions involved with acoustic, phonological and semantic language processing, similar to prior studies involving this task and age range[49, 58]. Fig 2 displays the group mean activation map for the condition of stories>tones for all participants who completed the task (n = 22, display threshold highlights voxels p<0.05, FDR correction).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Group mean activation map for the story listening task.

Group mean BOLD fMRI activation map for our story listening task (stories>tones) in 4 year-old girls (n = 22). All voxels significant at p<0.05 (FDR corrected), slice thickness 5 mm for contiguous slices. Slices range from z = -28 to z = 74 in MNI coordinate space. Color scale ranges from t = 1.25 (cooler) to 4 (hotter). Radiological orientation, left = right, right = left.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177398.g002

Regression of neural activation with child engagement scores

Child engagement scores were positively correlated with higher BOLD activation in right-sided, largely posterior cerebellar areas including (in anatomically descending order) lobule V, generally ascribed a sensorimotor role including with eye movement, extensive activation in lobule VI, Crus I and Crus II (cognitive-association areas), and smaller activation in Vermis VI and bilateral Vermis II, whose ascribed roles include emotional/affective processing[32, 70]. Less extensive, focal activation in right-sided, inferior cerebral areas was also found, involving temporal-occipital fusiform and lingual gyri, supporting higher-order visual processing, semantic association and imagery[71]. These areas are shown in Fig 3 (voxels p<0.05, FDR correction, n = 22). Fig 4 provides orthogonal sagittal, coronal and axial views (origin x = 6, y = -78, z = -26, MNI coordinate space) to more clearly illustrate the anatomical extent of activation clusters.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Regression map for the story listening task (stories>tones activation) with child engagement score applied as explanatory variable.

Regression map for the story listening task (stories>tones; n = 22), with child engagement score as the explanatory variable. Total cluster size 1164 voxels (p<0.05, FDR corrected), with center of gravity at (x = 21, y = -67, z = -25; R posterior cerebellum) in MNI coordinate space and z-score local maxima 3.22–3.93. Shown as 5 mm slices from z = -64 to z = -12 in MNI coordinate space. Color scale from t = 1.25 (cooler) to 4 (hotter). Radiological orientation, left = right, right = left.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177398.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. Tri-planar view of neural activation (stories>tones) for the story listening task with child engagement score as explanatory variable.

Orthogonal tri-planar view (origin x = 6, y = -78, z = -26, MNI coordinate space; right Crus II/Vermis VI) of cerebellar activation for the story listening task (stories>tones; n = 22), with child engagement score as explanatory variable. Total cluster size 1164 voxels (p<0.05, FDR corrected). Color scale t = 1.25 (cooler) to 4 (hotter). Radiological orientation, left = right, right = left, sagittal plane viewed from the right.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177398.g004

Functional connectivity

Post hoc seed-to-voxel functional connectivity mapping was performed in the subset of n = 12 participants whose fMRI data met the more stringent motion limitations imposed for this analysis. Significant positive correlations (one-tailed, height p<0.001, extent FDR-corrected p<0.05) between the seed region in right Crus I/II identified in our BOLD activation analysis was discovered in four discrete clusters located in: 1) right cerebellar lobules VI-VIII spanning into left Crus I/II, considered to support cognitive association[31, 70], 2) left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (BAs 8, 9 and 46) supporting a variety of executive functions[72], 3) right superior frontal gyrus (BA 8; frontal eye fields), supporting top-down attention and gaze control[73], and 4) left angular gyrus (BA 39) ascribed multiple integrative roles, notably complex language processing[74, 75], as shown in Fig 5.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Functional connectivity map showing brain areas functionally connected with the seed activation cluster correlated with child engagement scores.

Seed-to-voxel map of brain areas functionally connected with the seed activation cluster (right Crus I/II) correlated with observed child engagement scores during the story listening task (stories>tones; n = 12). Clusters at MNI coordinates: A) (x = 26, y = -78, z = -36; bilateral crus I/II and right cerebellar lobules VI and VII/VIII), B) (x = -42, y = 42, z = 22; left BA 6, 8, 9), C) (x = 38, y = 30, z = 42; right BA 8), and D) (x = -36, y = -66, z = 26; left BA 39). Color scale t = 3 (cooler) to 15 (hotter). Radiological orientation, left = right, right = left.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177398.g005

Discussion

Shared reading, recommended to initiate at birth[21], is considered among the best means to provide constructive cognitive stimulation promoting healthy brain development during the critical span of early childhood[20, 39, 76]. Most behavioral and neurobiological evidence of benefits of shared reading on emergent literacy skills and supporting brain circuits involve factors within caregiver control, such as access to books, reading frequency and quality of dialogic reading[1820, 76]. While widely assumed to provide long-lasting benefits, factors intrinsic to the child, such as interest and engagement in shared reading–later called “print motivation”[24]–are relatively under-studied, with little evidence of direct benefit. It has been suggested that such child-intrinsic factors exert indirect influence by catalyzing increased reading frequency, which in turn confers direct benefits[12, 77]. Higher interest and engagement are also thought to enhance the affective quality of shared reading, fueling gains in vocabulary and comprehension in home settings via greater focus on story content[24, 78], and gains in alphabet knowledge and decoding in school settings via greater focus on skill development[12]. Similarly, aspects of cognitive control such as attention and distraction inhibition are thought to exert indirect effects on emergent literacy skills and response to interventions via reinforcing more frequent, higher-quality shared reading[78]. Our findings suggest, albeit tentatively, a neurobiological mechanism by which greater child engagement during shared reading may directly influence–or “turbocharge”–the development of foundational emergent literacy skills, particularly comprehension, in preschool-age children.

The cerebellum has the largest number of neurons in the central nervous system[79], and undergoes rapid growth and development during childhood mirroring that of the cerebrum[80]. Until recently, its role was thought to be limited to motor processes, given the preponderance of motor deficits in lesion studies and difficulty mapping polysynaptic cerebro-cerebellar connections via traditional anatomical techniques[31]. The advent of functional neuroimaging has provided potent means to affirm a cerebellar role in higher-level cognitive processes, such as language, social-emotional processing and executive function[31, 33, 81, 82]. The majority of cerebellar connectivity is with cerebral association cortices, in accordance with consistent rules such as functional asymmetry, homotopicity (geometric correspondence) and contralaterality[31]. This mapping is localized in posterior-lateral areas, notably Crus I/II and lobule VI[31, 32], each highly active in our analyses. For language, strong correlation has been found between activation in Crus I/II and contralateral language areas in the dominant (usually left) cerebral hemisphere, consistent with increased functional connectivity between our seed (right Crus I/II) and left inferior frontal and angular gyri[68]. Right-sided cerebellar activation has been well described in imaging-based studies of narrative comprehension and reading[36, 37, 83, 84], consistent with the primacy of left-sided language and semantic areas in mature reading networks, and our findings suggest a cerebellar role in emergent literacy prior to kindergarten.

Given its consistent cellular architecture and polysynaptic feedback loops, it has been proposed that the cerebellum plays a similar modulatory role–a “cerebellar transform”[35]—in motor and cognitive processes[31]. Thus, as with motor planning and coordination optimizing movement, thought and emotion may be rehearsed and refined via internal models, enhancing skill development and learning[34]. Associations between cerebellar white matter microstructure and reading component skills in school-age children were recently described, consistent with this view[38]. Cerebellar activation seems to increase with greater cognitive loading, such as via novel words, stories or experiences[33], reasonably expected to be frequent during childhood and our story listening task. Increased cerebellar activation with tasks involving higher working memory demands was recently shown, including areas found in our results[85]. As with movement following lesion or surgery, learning and execution of such skills and tasks are possible without cerebellar involvement, but relatively inefficient and/or impaired, described as “dysmetria of thought”[35, 86, 87]. Our findings suggest that children who are more deeply engaged during shared reading more strongly recruit cerebellar association areas to “turbocharge” neural processing and connectivity during story listening, a potential mechanism for enhanced decoding (mediated by more robust recruitment of working memory), semantic processing, lexical access, social-emotional connection, and comprehension. This inference is consistent with the iconic image of rapt expressions on the faces of highly engaged children during preschool story time, hands shooting up to ask and answer questions. Whether such “turbocharged” activation is a consequence of constructive stimulation and nurturing, genetically determined temperament, or (most likely) multi-factorial, is beyond the scope of this paper, but merits further study.

Our findings reinforce behavioral evidence suggesting a nuanced yet broad influence of child interest and engagement in shared reading on emergent literacy skills. Contrary to our hypotheses, we did not find direct association between child engagement scores and activation in attention or semantic cerebral areas. Instead, the correlation between child engagement scores and exclusively right-sided cerebellar activation and connectivity reflect a potential enhancing (or “smoothing”[31]) effect on cerebral language, attention and association cortices. Thus, we suggest that higher child engagement during shared reading reflects fundamental differences in neural processing capacity, or efficiency, rather than simply a more favorable route to increased reading frequency[88]. It is reasonable to speculate that enhanced cognitive and social-emotional processing via this cerebellar “boost” may help make shared reading more pleasant and stimulating for the child, spurring a virtuous cycle of greater interest, inspiring caregivers to read more often with deeper emphasis on interactivity and story content, in turn fueling cerebellar activation and learning. Behavioral evidence is consistent with such a cycle, as children who view shared reading as a pleasurable experience–an “entertainment orientation”[24]–tend to manifest greater interest in reading increasingly advanced material more often[24], predicting later achievement[25]. Unfortunately, despite near-significance between maternal and child engagement scores in our video observation, many mothers exhibited low affective quality during shared reading or were frankly unresponsive to child entreaties to read, most commonly due to smartphone distraction, consistent with low reading motivation and engagement previously reported in low-SES parents[89]. As the preschool age range (3–5) is a highly sensitive period to nurture interest in reading independent of skill level and SES[90], our findings underscore the importance of interventions explicitly addressing both parent and child reading engagement, including awareness and reduction of distractions. Despite low sample size for behavioral analyses, positive correlation between child engagement scores and proximity during reading, child page turning, and use of child-adjusted voice suggests 3 relatively simple behaviors to be recommended that would likely have a positive impact on child reading engagement. Paradoxically, such approaches focused on making reading interesting, interactive and enjoyable—fun—tend to be more effective at promoting emergent skills than those focused on rote instruction[24, 77], and should be emphasized in anticipatory guidance and interventions.

Our study has several important strengths. Our sample of 4 year-old girls is considerably younger than most neuroimaging-based studies of emergent literacy[36, 91], with ample sample size[92] drawn from well-defined cohort, successfully applying an established fMRI story listening task paradigm[93] including connectivity-based analysis. We tested for and excluded important potential confounders, household income and maternal education. Our shared reading assessment involved observation via an explicit protocol, addressing concerns regarding reliability of parental report[94]. Our scorers were trained to apply standardized dialogic reading and child engagement criteria, with high inter-rater reliability. We applied conservative BOLD thresholding parameters, nonparametric permutation testing strictly controlling for family-wise error rate, and strict motion correction in our analyses, reducing the prospect of spurious findings[63]. The decision to include higher-motion children for increased power in our BOLD activation analysis was carefully considered and an unlikely source of bias, given weak correlation between motion parameters and our stories task, modest magnitude of motion, and non-correlation of motion with our behavioral measure. Confidence in our findings is further bolstered by the topology of the activation cluster, which aligns with current models of cerebellar function in higher-level association[31, 33], notably its role in language and reading[37, 91], which in turn aligns with behavioral evidence[12, 24]. These findings were reinforced via connectivity-based analysis, utilizing stricter motion correction and a hypothesis-driven approach. Our results build on recent evidence of positive correlation between home reading environment and maternal shared reading engagement and brain function and connectivity in preschool-age children[19, 58], suggesting synergistic neurobiological mechanisms. Together, these findings inform clinical practice during a formative stage of development via an innovative approach, reinforcing literacy promotion recommendations[21] and early reading interventions[23].

Our study also has limitations. While we attempted to contact all mothers with daughters in our target age range, we were unable to do so for many, and those who did enroll may be more engaged in their child’s development. However, by design all mother-child dyads in the CHIP cohort are at-risk for poor outcomes, limiting the range of such bias. Our decision to exclusively sample girls limits generalizability, though we view this as a strength, allowing us to efficiently collect high-quality data in young children. Furthermore, while gender dimorphisms in brain structure and function in young children have been described[56, 95], these have been found to be negligible for our story listening task[52], leading us to expect similar findings in boys. Our homogeneous sample of low-SES mothers also limits generalizability, though we see this too as a strength, as low-SES populations stand to benefit most from improved insights and interventions. A larger study involving a diverse sample would help determine to what extent SES moderates child engagement during shared reading and its influence on neural processing. Our child engagement score reflected a single snapshot, and may not be representative of longer-term behavior. However, household reading behaviors tend to be stable during the preschool period[96], and such focal observations have been reliably utilized in in assessment of home environment[97], including reading[98]. Our assessment of child reading engagement was conducted outside of the scanner, given restrictions on motion and interactivity during the scan itself. However, it is reasonable to assume that observed engagement in story reading exhibited by the child during the same study visit, reflective of longer-term experience and interest, is a fair proxy for that manifest during the story listening task. Future studies could assess child engagement during the MRI task itself via an age-appropriate post-scan questionnaire. Finally, whereas our results show compelling correlation between observed child interest in and engagement during shared reading and neural activation and connectivity during a story listening task, our study design cannot establish causation. Longitudinal studies are needed beginning in infancy, to better understand dynamic home, maternal/caregiver and child factors contributing to healthy brain development and emergent literacy.

Conclusions

We utilized functional neuroimaging to demonstrate positive correlation between child interest and engagement in shared reading, assessed via video observation, and increased activation and functional connectivity in cerebellar association areas during a story listening fMRI task. The cerebellum is increasingly recognized to play a role in higher-level cognitive processing, typically right-lateralized for language and reading, purportedly enhancing skill rehearsal and learning. Our findings suggest an intriguing, albeit tentative, mechanism by which these factors intrinsic to the child may enhance emergent literacy development via recruitment of cerebellar association areas–a cerebellar “boost”–during story sharing, and underscore the potential of interventions targeting parent-child engagement and interest in reading.

Supporting information

S1 File. Deidentified subject data (n = 22) for children in this study utilized in analyses, including maternal education, household income, observed child engagement scores, and scores for observed mother-child reading behaviors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177398.s001

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank Akila Rajagopal for invaluable support with imaging-based analyses (preprocessing and activation maps), Rachel Gruber and Jonathan Vidovich for assistance scoring reading observations, and Stacey Woeste for coordination of CHIP children, implementation of our reading observation protocol, and data management.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: JSH TD SH.
  2. Data curation: JSH KP SKH.
  3. Formal analysis: JSH JD.
  4. Funding acquisition: JSH KP.
  5. Investigation: JSH.
  6. Methodology: JSH THK SKH.
  7. Project administration: JSH KP.
  8. Resources: JSH KP SKH.
  9. Software: JD.
  10. Supervision: KP THK MA TD SKH.
  11. Visualization: JSH JD.
  12. Writing – original draft: JSH.
  13. Writing – review & editing: JSH KP THK JD MA TD SKH.

References

  1. 1. Cree A. The Economic and Social Cost of Illiteracy: A Snapshot of Illiteracy in a Global Context. World Literacy Foundation; 2012 April 2012.
  2. 2. Norton ES, Beach SD, Gabrieli JD. Neurobiology of dyslexia. Current Opinions in Neurobiology. 2014;30c:73–8.
  3. 3. Heath SM, Bishop DV, Bloor KE, Boyle GL, Fletcher J, Hogben JH, et al. A spotlight on Preschool: the influence of family factors on children's early literacy skills. PLoS One. 2014;9(4):e95255. pmid:24751691
  4. 4. National Literacy Trust. Literacy: state of the Nation. UK: National Literacy Trust; 2011.
  5. 5. National Center for Education Statistics. National Assessment of Educational Progress at Grades 4 and 8: Mathematics and Reading. In: Sciences. IfE, editor. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics; 2015.
  6. 6. The Annie E. Casey Foundation. Double Jeopardy: How Third Grade Reading Skills and Poverty Influence High School Graduation. Baltimore, MD: The Annie E. Casey Foundation; 2012.
  7. 7. Gabrieli JD. Dyslexia: a new synergy between education and cognitive neuroscience. Science (New York, NY). 2009;325(5938):280–3.
  8. 8. Pool JL, Johnson ES. Screening for Reading Problems in Preschool and Kindergarten: An Overview of Select Measures. Boise, ID: Boise State University; 2016.
  9. 9. Justice LM, Invernizzi MA, Meier JD. Designing and implementing an early literacy screening protocol: Suggestions for the speech-language pathologist. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools. 2002;33:84–101. pmid:27764468
  10. 10. Snow CE, Burns MS, Griffin P. Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 1998.
  11. 11. Dehaene S, Cohen L. Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron. 2007;56(2):384–98. pmid:17964253
  12. 12. Whitehurst GJ, Lonigan CJ. Child development and emergent literacy. Child Dev. 1998;69(3):848–72. pmid:9680688
  13. 13. Ramey CT, Ramey SL. Early intervention and early experience. The American Psychologist. 1998;53(2):109–20. pmid:9491742
  14. 14. Bus AIv M; Pellegrini A. Joint Book Reading Makes for Success in Learning to Read: A Meta-Analysis on Intergenerational Transmission of Literacy. Review of Educational Research. 1995;65(1):1–21.
  15. 15. Swanson E, Wanzek J, Petscher Y, Vaughn S, Heckert J, Cavanaugh C, et al. A synthesis of read-aloud interventions on early reading outcomes among preschool through third graders at risk for reading difficulties. Journal of learning disabilities. 2011;44(3):258–75. pmid:21521868
  16. 16. Anderson RC, et al. Becoming a nation of readers: The report of the Commission on Reading. Washington, DC: National Institute of Education; 1985.
  17. 17. National Early Literacy Panel. Developing early literacy: Report of the National Early Literacy Panel. Washington, DC: National Institute for Literacy; 2008.
  18. 18. Hutton J, Horowitz-Kraus T, Mendelsohn A, DeWitt T, Holland S. Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children Listening to Stories. Pediatrics. 2015;136(3):466–78. pmid:26260716
  19. 19. Hutton JS, Phelan KJ, Horowitz-Kraus T, Altaye M, Dudley J, DeWitt T, et al. Shared Reading Quality and Brain Function Supporting Emergent Literacy in Preschool-Age Children. Under Review. 2016.
  20. 20. U.S. Department of Education. Shared Book Reading. Washington, D.C.: Institute of Education Sciences; 2015.
  21. 21. High PC, Klass P. Literacy promotion: an essential component of primary care pediatric practice. Pediatrics. 2014;134(2):404–9. pmid:24962987
  22. 22. Zuckerman B, Augustyn M. Books and reading: evidence-based standard of care whose time has come. Acad Pediatr. 2011;11(1):11–7. pmid:21272819
  23. 23. Zuckerman B, Khandekar A. Reach Out and Read: evidence based approach to promoting early child development. Current Opinions in Pediatrics. 2010;22(4):539–44.
  24. 24. Sonnenschein S, Munsterman K. The influence of home-based reading interactions on 5-year-olds’ reading motivations and early literacy development. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2002;17:318–37.
  25. 25. Scarborough H, Dobrich W. On the Efficacy of Reading to Preschoolers. Developmental Review. 1994;14(3):245–302.
  26. 26. Payne A, Whitehurst G, Angell A. The role of literacy environment in the language development of children from low-income families. Early Child Res Q. 1994;9:427–40.
  27. 27. McClelland MM, Cameron CE, Connor CM, Farris CL, Jewkes AM, Morrison FJ. Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers' literacy, vocabulary, and math skills. Developmental Psychology. 2007;43(4):947–59. pmid:17605527
  28. 28. Lonigan C, Anthony J, Bloomfield B, Dyer S, Samwel C. Effects of Two Shared-Reading Interventions on Emergent Literacy Skills of At-Risk Preschoolers. Journal of Early Intervention. 1999;22(4):306–22.
  29. 29. Hutton JS, DeWitt T, Horowitz-Kraus T, Mendelsohn AL, Holland SK. Association Between Home Reading Environment Domains and an Expanded Emergent Literacy Model. In Submission. 2016.
  30. 30. Power JD, Fair DA, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. The development of human functional brain networks. Neuron. 2010;67(5):735–48. pmid:20826306
  31. 31. Buckner RL. The cerebellum and cognitive function: 25 years of insight from anatomy and neuroimaging. Neuron. 2013;80(3):807–15. pmid:24183029
  32. 32. Keren-Happuch E, Chen SH, Ho MH, Desmond JE. A meta-analysis of cerebellar contributions to higher cognition from PET and fMRI studies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35(2):593–615. pmid:23125108
  33. 33. Marien P, Ackermann H, Adamaszek M, Barwood CH, Beaton A, Desmond J, et al. Consensus paper: Language and the cerebellum: an ongoing enigma. Cerebellum (London, England). 2014;13(3):386–410.
  34. 34. Ito M. Control of mental activities by internal models in the cerebellum. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(4):304–13. pmid:18319727
  35. 35. Guell X, Hoche F, Schmahmann JD. Metalinguistic deficits in patients with cerebellar dysfunction: empirical support for the dysmetria of thought theory. Cerebellum (London, England). 2015;14(1):50–8.
  36. 36. Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA. Paying attention to reading: the neurobiology of reading and dyslexia. Dev Psychopathol. 2008;20(4):1329–49. pmid:18838044
  37. 37. Berl MM, Duke ES, Mayo J, Rosenberger LR, Moore EN, VanMeter J, et al. Functional anatomy of listening and reading comprehension during development. Brain Lang. 2010;114(2):115–25. pmid:20656105
  38. 38. Travis KE, Leitner Y, Feldman HM, Ben-Shachar M. Cerebellar white matter pathways are associated with reading skills in children and adolescents. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36(4):1536–53. pmid:25504986
  39. 39. Knudsen EI. Sensitive periods in the development of the brain and behavior. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2004;16(8):1412–25. pmid:15509387
  40. 40. Kuhl PK. Early Language Learning and Literacy: Neuroscience Implications for Education. Mind, brain and education: the official journal of the International Mind, Brain, and Education Society. 2011;5(3):128–42.
  41. 41. Leppanen PH, Hamalainen JA, Guttorm TK, Eklund KM, Salminen H, Tanskanen A, et al. Infant brain responses associated with reading-related skills before school and at school age. Neurophysiologie clinique = Clinical neurophysiology. 2012;42(1–2):35–41. pmid:22200340
  42. 42. Horowitz-Kraus T, Vannest JJ, Holland SK. Overlapping neural circuitry for narrative comprehension and proficient reading in children and adolescents. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51(13):2651–62. pmid:24029377
  43. 43. Cardillo-Lebdeva G, Kuhl P. Individual differences in infant speech perception predict language and pre-reading skills through age 5. Annnual Meeting of the Society for Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics; Portland, OR2009.
  44. 44. National Scientific Council on the Developing Child. Working Paper #5: The Timing and Quality of Early Experiences Combine to Shape Brain Architecture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Center on the Developing Child, Child. CotD; 2008.
  45. 45. Horowitz-Kraus T, Eaton K, Farah R, Hajinazarian A, Vannest J, Holland SK. Predicting better performance on a college preparedness test from narrative comprehension at the age of 6 years: An fMRI study. Brain Res. 2015;1629:54–62. pmid:26459989
  46. 46. Frost SJ, Landi N, Mencl WE, Sandak R, Fulbright RK, Tejada ET, et al. Phonological awareness predicts activation patterns for print and speech. Annals of dyslexia. 2009;59(1):78–97. pmid:19306061
  47. 47. Dehaene S. Inside the letterbox: how literacy transforms the human brain. Cerebrum. 2013;2013:7. pmid:23847714
  48. 48. American Academy of Pediatrics Council on Early Brain and Child Development. Eco-Bio-Developmental Model of Human Health and Disease: American Academy of Pediatrics; 2014 [Available from: http://www.aap.org/en-us/advocacy-and-policy/aap-health-initiatives/EBCD/Pages/Eco-Bio-Developmental.aspx.
  49. 49. Karunanayaka PR, Holland SK, Schmithorst VJ, Solodkin A, Chen EE, Szaflarski JP, et al. Age-related connectivity changes in fMRI data from children listening to stories. Neuroimage. 2007;34(1):349–60. pmid:17064940
  50. 50. Rajagopal A, Byars A, Schapiro M, Lee GR, Holland SK. Success rates for functional MR imaging in children. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2014;35(12):2319–25. pmid:25082823
  51. 51. Vannest J, Rajagopal A, Cicchino ND, Franks-Henry J, Simpson SM, Lee G, et al. Factors Determining Success of Awake and Asleep Magnetic Resonance Imaging Scans in Nonsedated Children. Neuropediatrics. 2014.
  52. 52. Plante E, Schmithorst VJ, Holland SK, Byars AW. Sex differences in the activation of language cortex during childhood. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(7):1210–21. pmid:16303148
  53. 53. Schmithorst VJ, Holland SK. Sex differences in the development of neuroanatomical functional connectivity underlying intelligence found using Bayesian connectivity analysis. Neuroimage. 2007;35(1):406–19. pmid:17223578
  54. 54. Buysse V, Winton P, Rous B, Epstein D, & Cavanaugh C. CONNECT Module 6: Dialogic Reading Practices [Web-based professional development curriculum]. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina, FPG Child Development Institute, CONNECT: The Center to Mobilize Early Childhood Knowledge.; 2011.
  55. 55. Harris P, Taylor R, Thielke R, Payne J, Gonzalez N, Conde J. Research electronic data capture (REDCap)—A metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. Journal of Biomedical Informatics. 2009;42(2):377–81. pmid:18929686
  56. 56. Schmithorst V, Holland S, Plante E. Cognitive modules utilized for narrative comprehension in children: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroimage. 2006;29(1):254–66. pmid:16109491
  57. 57. Holland SK, Vannest J, Mecoli M, Jacola LM, Tillema JM, Karunanayaka PR, et al. Functional MRI of language lateralization during development in children. Int J Audiol. 2007;46(9):533–51. pmid:17828669
  58. 58. Hutton JS, Horowitz-Kraus T, Mendelsohn AL, DeWitt T, Holland SK. Home Reading Environment and Brain Activation in Preschool Children Listening to Stories. Pediatrics. 2015.
  59. 59. Smith S, Jenkinson M, Woolrich M, Beckmann C, Behrens T, Johansen-Berg H, et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage. 2004;23(S1):208–19.
  60. 60. Woolrich M. Robust group analysis using outlier inference. Neuroimage. 2008;41(2):286–301. pmid:18407525
  61. 61. Woolrich MW, Behrens TE, Beckmann CF, Jenkinson M, Smith SM. Multilevel linear modelling for FMRI group analysis using Bayesian inference. Neuroimage. 2004;21(4):1732–47. pmid:15050594
  62. 62. Beckmann CF, Jenkinson M, Smith SM. General multilevel linear modeling for group analysis in FMRI. Neuroimage. 2003;20(2):1052–63. pmid:14568475
  63. 63. Eklund A, Nichols TE, Knutsson H. Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016;113(28):7900–5. pmid:27357684
  64. 64. Brett M, Christoff K, Cusack R, Lancaster J. Using the Talairach atlas with the MNI template. Neuroimage. 2001;13(6):85.
  65. 65. Winkler AM, Ridgway GR, Webster MA, Smith SM, Nichols TE. Permutation inference for the general linear model. Neuroimage. 2014;92:381–97. pmid:24530839
  66. 66. Diedrichsen J, Balsters JH, Flavell J, Cussans E, Ramnani N. A probabilistic MR atlas of the human cerebellum. Neuroimage. 2009;46(1):39–46. pmid:19457380
  67. 67. Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Nieto-Castanon A. Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect. 2012;2(3):125–41. pmid:22642651
  68. 68. Gelinas JN, Fitzpatrick KP, Kim HC, Bjornson BH. Cerebellar language mapping and cerebral language dominance in pediatric epilepsy surgery patients. NeuroImage Clinical. 2014;6:296–306. pmid:25379442
  69. 69. Lancaster JL, Woldorff MG, Parsons LM, Liotti M, Freitas CS, Rainey L, et al. Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Hum Brain Mapp. 2000;10(3):120–31. pmid:10912591
  70. 70. Stoodley CJ. The cerebellum and cognition: evidence from functional imaging studies. Cerebellum (London, England). 2012;11(2):352–65.
  71. 71. Burton MW, Locasto PC, Krebs-Noble D, Gullapalli RP. A systematic investigation of the functional neuroanatomy of auditory and visual phonological processing. Neuroimage. 2005;26(3):647–61. pmid:15955475
  72. 72. Lara AH, Wallis JD. The Role of Prefrontal Cortex in Working Memory: A Mini Review. Frontiers in systems neuroscience. 2015;9:173. pmid:26733825
  73. 73. Lanzilotto M, Perciavalle V, Lucchetti C. Auditory and visual systems organization in Brodmann Area 8 for gaze-shift control: where we do not see, we can hear. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience. 2013;7:198. pmid:24339805
  74. 74. Binder JR, Desai RH. The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2011;15(11):527–36. pmid:22001867
  75. 75. Boukrina O, Graves WW. Neural networks underlying contributions from semantics in reading aloud. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2013;7:518. pmid:24032009
  76. 76. U.S. Department of Education. Dialogic Reading. Washington, DC: Institute of Education Sciences; 2007.
  77. 77. Baker L, Mackler K, Sonnenschein S, Serpell R. Parents' interactions with their first grade children during storybook reading and relations with subsequent home reading activity and reading achievment. Journal of School Psychology. 2001;39(5):415–38.
  78. 78. Davidse NJ, de Jong MT, Bus AG, Huijbregts SC, Swaab H. Cognitive and environmental predictors of early literacy skills. Reading and writing. 2011;24(4):395–412. pmid:21475735
  79. 79. Herculano-Houzel S. The human brain in numbers: a linearly scaled-up primate brain. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2009;3:31. pmid:19915731
  80. 80. Tiemeier H, Lenroot RK, Greenstein DK, Tran L, Pierson R, Giedd JN. Cerebellum development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal morphometric MRI study. Neuroimage. 2010;49(1):63–70. pmid:19683586
  81. 81. Baumann O, Borra RJ, Bower JM, Cullen KE, Habas C, Ivry RB, et al. Consensus paper: the role of the cerebellum in perceptual processes. Cerebellum (London, England). 2015;14(2):197–220.
  82. 82. Brissenden J, Levin E, Osher D, Rosen M, Halko M, Somers D. Cerebellar Contributions to Visual Attention and Visual Working Memory Revealed by Functional MRI and Intrinsic Functional Connectivity. Journal of vision. 2015;15(12):232.
  83. 83. Vlachos F, Papathanasiou I, Andreou G. Cerebellum and reading. Folia phoniatrica et logopaedica: official organ of the International Association of Logopedics and Phoniatrics (IALP). 2007;59(4):177–83.
  84. 84. Price CJ. A review and synthesis of the first 20 years of PET and fMRI studies of heard speech, spoken language and reading. Neuroimage. 2012;62(2):816–47. pmid:22584224
  85. 85. Kuper M, Kaschani P, Thurling M, Stefanescu MR, Burciu RG, Goricke S, et al. Cerebellar fMRI Activation Increases with Increasing Working Memory Demands. Cerebellum (London, England). 2016;15(3):322–35.
  86. 86. Hoche F, Guell X, Sherman JC, Vangel MG, Schmahmann JD. Cerebellar Contribution to Social Cognition. Cerebellum (London, England). 2015.
  87. 87. Koustenis E, Hernaiz Driever P, de Sonneville L, Rueckriegel SM. Executive function deficits in pediatric cerebellar tumor survivors. European journal of paediatric neurology: EJPN: official journal of the European Paediatric Neurology Society. 2016;20(1):25–37.
  88. 88. Bracken SS, Fischel JE. Family Reading Behavior and Early Literacy Skills in Preschool Children from Low-Income Backgrounds. Early Education and Development. 2008;19(1):45–67.
  89. 89. Whitehurst G, Arnold D, Epstein J, Angell A, Smith M, Fischel J. A picture book reading intervention in day care and home for children from low-income families. Developmental Psychology. 1994;30(5):679–89.
  90. 90. Carroll C. The effects of parental literacy involvement and child reading interest on the development of emergent literacy skills.: University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee; 2013.
  91. 91. Monzalvo K, Dehaene-Lambertz G. How reading acquisition changes children's spoken language network. Brain Lang. 2013;127(3):356–65. pmid:24216407
  92. 92. Desmond J, Glover G. Estimating sample size in functional MRI (fMRI) neuroimaging studies: statistical power analyses. J Neurosci Methods. 2002;118(2):115–28. pmid:12204303
  93. 93. Szaflarski JP, Altaye M, Rajagopal A, Eaton K, Meng X, Plante E, et al. A 10-year longitudinal fMRI study of narrative comprehension in children and adolescents. Neuroimage. 2012;63(3):1188–95. pmid:22951258
  94. 94. Hofferth SL. Response Bias in a Popular Indicator of Reading to Chilldren. Workshop on the National Children’s Study; Crystal City, VA: University of Maryland; 2004.
  95. 95. Cancelliere A, Mangano FT, Air EL, Jones BV, Altaye M, Rajagopal A, et al. DTI values in key white matter tracts from infancy through adolescence. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2013;34(7):1443–9. pmid:23370472
  96. 96. National Household Education Survey. Children’s School Readiness Skills: 1993 and 2007. Washington, DC: US Census Bureau Statistical Abstract of the United States; 2012.
  97. 97. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF, McAdoo HP, Coll CG. The home environments of children in the United States part I: variations by age, ethnicity, and poverty status. Child Dev. 2001;72(6):1844–67. pmid:11768149
  98. 98. Weitzman CC, Roy L, Walls T, Tomlin R. More evidence for reach out and read: a home-based study. Pediatrics. 2004;113(5):1248–53. pmid:15121937