Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Stable self-serving personality traits in recreational and dependent cocaine users

  • Boris B. Quednow,

    Affiliation Experimental and Clinical Pharmacopsychology, Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, Psychiatric Hospital, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland

  • Lea M. Hulka,

    Affiliation Experimental and Clinical Pharmacopsychology, Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, Psychiatric Hospital, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland

  • Katrin H. Preller,

    Affiliation Experimental and Clinical Pharmacopsychology, Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, Psychiatric Hospital, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland

  • Markus R. Baumgartner,

    Affiliation Zurich Institute of Forensic Medicine, Center for Forensic Hair Analysis, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland

  • Christoph Eisenegger,

    Affiliation Neuropsychopharmacology and Biopsychology Unit, Department of Basic Psychological Research and Research Methods, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

  • Matthias Vonmoos

    matthias.vonmoos@bli.uzh.ch

    Affiliation Experimental and Clinical Pharmacopsychology, Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, Psychiatric Hospital, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland

Abstract

Chronic cocaine use has been associated with impairments in social cognition, self-serving and antisocial behavior, and socially relevant personality disorders (PD). Despite the apparent relationship between Machiavellianism and stimulant use, no study has explicitly examined this personality concept in cocaine users so far. In the frame of the longitudinal Zurich Cocaine Cognition Study, the Machiavellianism Questionnaire (MACH-IV) was assessed in 68 recreational and 30 dependent cocaine users as well as in 68 psychostimulant-naïve controls at baseline. Additionally, three closely related personality dimensions from the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI)–cooperativeness, (social) reward dependence, and self-directedness–and the screening questionnaire of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II) were acquired. At the one-year follow-up, 57 cocaine users and 48 controls were reassessed with the MACH-IV. Finally, MACH-IV scores were correlated with measures of social cognition and interaction (cognitive/emotional empathy, Theory-of-Mind, prosocial behavior) and with SCID-II PD scores assessed at baseline. Both recreational and dependent cocaine users showed significantly higher Machiavellianism than controls, while dependent cocaine users additionally displayed significantly lower levels of TCI cooperativeness and self-directedness. During the one-year interval, MACH-IV scores showed high test-retest reliability and also the significant gap between cocaine users and controls remained. Moreover, in cocaine users, higher Machiavellianism correlated significantly with lower levels of cooperativeness and self-directedness, with less prosocial behavior, and with higher cluster B PD scores. However, Machiavellianism was not correlated with measures of cocaine use severity (r<-.15). Both recreational and dependent cocaine users display pronounced and stable Machiavellian personality traits. The lack of correlations with severity of cocaine use and its temporal stability indicates that a Machiavellian personality trait might represent a predisposition for cocaine use that potentially serves as a predictor for stimulant addiction.

Introduction

Disturbed socio-cognitive abilities are important features of all substance use disorders, but they particularly affect stimulant-using populations [1, 2]. Accordingly, it has been proposed that social functioning and, thus, social beliefs and attitudes might play essential roles in the origin and course of stimulant addiction [3]. However, until recently, systematic investigations examining social characteristics and attitudes in stimulant users were scarce, although accumulating evidence suggests that, e.g., chronic cocaine use is associated with broad alterations in socio-cognitive functioning [46]. In particular, recent research showed that recreational cocaine users (RCU) exhibit deficits in emotional empathy and emotion recognition from prosody, whereas dependent cocaine users (DCU) show difficulties with emotional and mental perspective-taking (Theory-of-Mind) [5, 6]. Moreover, RCU and DCU show more self-serving behavior in social interaction paradigms, commit more criminal offenses, and have smaller social networks [4, 6]. In addition, dependent cocaine use is commonly associated with a variety of socially relevant personality disorders (PD), including antisocial and narcissistic PD [6, 7]. Altogether, these specific social characteristics of chronic cocaine users resemble the broad concept of Machiavellianism [8, 9], which describes a social attitude characterized by cynical beliefs, interpersonal manipulation, and pragmatic morality [10]. However, although social functioning was linked to drug addiction [3] and explicitly cocaine addiction has been associated with social isolation and the neglect of social rules [1], only a very limited number of studies have investigated the linkage between Machiavellianism and illegal substance use in general [11, 12], while no study has explicitly examined Machiavellian attitudes in cocaine users so far. Moreover, the currently closest link to Machiavellianism in cocaine users–the elevated self-serving behavior of RCU and DCU [4]–was established using money distribution games showing rather low test-retest reliabilities [13]. Accordingly, more reliable personality questionnaires might be an appropriate way to complement the existing findings on social behavior in cocaine users. Therefore, we aimed to investigate Machiavellianism in a large sample of RCU, DCU, and stimulant-naïve healthy controls (matched for age, sex, verbal IQ, and cigarette smoking) using the self-assessment Machiavellianism-questionnaire MACH-IV [10].

Based on previous studies mentioned above, we expected to find elevated Machiavellianism in both RCU and DCU. As Machiavellianism is generally considered to be a stable personality trait [8, 14], we further investigated the reliability and stability of the MACH-IV across an interval of one year and hypothesized that increased Machiavellianism in cocaine users remains stable over time. In addition, we examined three closely related [15] personality dimensions from Cloninger’s Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI, [16]) and expected less pronounced cooperativeness, reward dependence, and self-directedness in both cocaine users groups. Given that chronic cocaine users exhibit impaired socio-cognitive functioning and increased PD comorbidities [47] and that Machiavellianism has been negatively associated with socio-cognitive functioning [8] but positively linked to most PD [9], we investigated the association of Machiavellianism with socio-cognitive functioning and PD symptoms as well.

Methods

Participants

The present data were collected as part of the Zurich Cocaine Cognition Study (ZuCo2St), for which recruitment information has been previously published in detail [4, 6, 17]. The present study included a sample largely overlapping the studies of Vonmoos et al. [17, 18], consisting of 68 RCU, 30 DCU, and 68 healthy, stimulant-naïve controls at baseline. Specific inclusion criteria for the cocaine groups were cocaine use of at least 0.5g/month, with cocaine being the preferred illegal drug, and a current abstinence duration of <6 months. Cocaine dependence was diagnosed according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual-IV (DSM-IV) criteria [19], with only DCU fulfilling these criteria. Exclusion criteria for cocaine users were previous or present DSM-IV Axis-I adult psychiatric disorders other than cocaine, nicotine, and alcohol abuse/dependence; history of depression; and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [20]. Moreover, the intake of opioids and a polytoxic drug use pattern according to DSM-IV were not permitted. Exclusion criteria for control subjects were any DSM-IV Axis-I psychiatric disorders, with the exception of nicotine dependence, and regular illegal drug use (lifetime use <15 occasions), with the exception of occasional cannabis use (≤1 occasion per day). For both groups, exclusion criteria were severe medical diseases, head injury or neurological disorders, and use of prescription psychotropic drugs. Urine screenings (semi-quantitative enzyme multiplied immunoassay method) and 6-month hair analyses (liquid chromatography tandem mass spectrometry) allowed the objective quantification of drug use (for details see [17, 18]).

For the longitudinal analysis, we re-invited the participants from the cross-sectional sample for a retest-session one year later. In the course of this, 48 psychostimulant-naïve controls and 57 cocaine users could be included in a subsequent one-year follow-up analysis (for recruitment and selection details see [21]. In this analysis, we applied the same group assignment as in two previous ZuCo2St subprojects (for selection details [21, 22]) following a combination of absolute (cocaine concentration of ≥±0.5 ng/mg, [23, 24]) and relative (minimal increase of 20% or a minimal decrease of 10% in cocainetotal, [25]) changes in cocaine concentration in hair samples between baseline (t1) and a one-year follow-up (t2). According to these criteria, cocaine users were divided into three groups of similar size: 19 cocaine increasers (mean increase +30.4 ng/mg [+297%], range +0.5 to +268.5 ng/mg [+20% to +5374%], SD 61.9 ng/mg), 19 cocaine decreasers (mean decrease -10.6 ng/mg [-72%], range -116.9 to -0.6 ng/mg [-100% to -12%], SD 26.7 ng/mg), and 19 stable cocaine users who did not meet either of the former criteria (mean change -0.1ng/mg [-2%], range -1.9 to +0.5 ng/mg [-100% to +720%], SD 0.5 ng/mg).

At baseline and one-year follow-up, participants were asked to abstain from illegal substances for 72h and from alcohol for 24h before the testing session. The study was approved by the Cantonal Ethics Committee of Zurich. All participants provided written informed consent and were compensated for their participation.

Procedure

Machiavellianism was assessed with a German version of the MACH-IV [10], which includes 20 items, with 7-point Likert-scales ranging from -3 to +3 (-3 = totally disagree, 0 = neutral, +3 = totally agree). In order to follow the original MACH-IV-instructions [10], we transformed the ratings into a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 to 7 (1 = totally disagree, 4 = neutral, 7 = totally agree) and added a constant of 20 to the total score (but not to the subscales). Cooperativeness, reward dependence, and self-directedness were measured with a German version of the 240-item TCI [16]. The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis-II Disorder questionnaire (SCID-II, [26]) was used to assess PD symptom severities. Social cognition and interaction were measured with the following tasks: Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET, [27]), Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition (MASC, [28]), Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET, [29]), Distribution Game [30], and Dictator Game [31], which have been described in detail before [4, 6]. The Social Network Questionnaire (SNQ, [32]) was applied to estimate participants’ current social network sizes (see also [6]). Craving for cocaine was assessed by the brief version of the Cocaine Craving Questionnaire (CCQ, [33]). Self-reported cocaine use was assessed with the Interview for Psychotropic Drug Consumption [34].

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed with IBM SPSS Statistics 22.0. Demographic and drug use data were analyzed by analyses of variance, Students t-tests and χ2-tests, where appropriate. MACH-IV and TCI scores were analyzed by analyses of covariance (ANCOVA), followed by Sidak-corrected post-hoc comparisons. Because previous findings suggest a positive link between advancing age and fairness in stimulant users [4] and to control for comprehension differences in the applied language-based questionnaires, age and verbal IQ were introduced as covariates. Verbal IQ was measured with the German vocabulary test Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest B (MWT-B [35]), an estimate of premorbid verbal intelligence. The longitudinal analysis was carried out by a mixed design analyses of variance, again including the covariates age and verbal IQ. Pearson’s product-moment correlations were conducted across a combined cocaine user group (RCU+DCU). To test for potential differences between the correlation coefficients in controls and cocaine users, Fisher’s z-tests were performed [36]. The confirmatory statistical comparisons were carried out on a significance level of p < .05 (two-tailed). P-values in the Pearson’s product-moment correlations and Fisher’s z-tests were adjusted by Bonferroni correction. In the applied adjustment method, the p-values of the single correlations were multiplied by the number of analyzed variables per test to compare the results to the nominal significance level of 0.05 (TCI 16 variables, MET 3, MASC 4, RMET 1, Games 2, SNQ 1, SCID-II 16, cocaine use measures 10) [37].

Results

Demographic characteristics and drug use

As reported in Table 1 and also in previous papers using the same sample [17, 18], the three groups did not differ significantly at baseline regarding age, sex distribution, smoking status, socio-economic status, and verbal IQ. However, DCU showed fewer years of school education compared with RCU and controls. The RCU used cocaine regularly without fulfilling the DSM-IV criteria for dependence, whereas 30 DCU showed a comparatively higher weekly use of cocaine, higher cocaine hair concentration, and higher cumulative lifetime dose.

thumbnail
Table 1. Demographic data and drug use pattern (cross-sectional sample).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172853.t001

In the longitudinal analysis the four investigated groups (controls, increasers, decreasers, stable users) did not significantly differ regarding age, sex distribution, verbal IQ, years of education, smoking status, and length of interval between the two study assessments (Table 2; for a more detailed sample description see [21, 22]).

thumbnail
Table 2. Demographic data and drug use pattern (longitudinal sample).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172853.t002

Machiavellianism

An ANCOVA (corrected for age and verbal IQ) showed significant group differences in the MACH-IV total scores (F(2,156) = 4.41, p < .05), whereby both RCU (p < .05, d = 0.43) and DCU (p < .05, d = 0.55) showed significantly higher MACH-IV total scores than controls but did not significantly differ from each other (p = .94, d = 0.12) (Fig 1).This effect was mainly driven by the subscale ‘cynical views’ (F(2,156) = 4.19, p < .05; pControls vs. RCU = .06, dControls vs. RCU = 0.40; pControls vs. DCU < .05, dControls vs. DCU = 0.55; pRCU vs. DCU = .88, dRCU vs. DCU = 0.15), but also the subscale ‘tactics’ showed a trend towards significant group differences (F(2,156) = 2.75, p < .10) with non-significant but moderate effect sizes for RCU (p = .12, d = 0.35) and DCU (p = .20, d = 0.42) compared to controls. The group differences in the subscale ‘morality’ were not significant (F(2,156) = 0.38, p = .69) and showed only slightly elevated values in both cocaine-using groups (pControls vs. RCU = .80, d = 0.14; pControls vs. DCU = .92, d = 0.13).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Mean scores and standard errors of means for the Machiavellianism-questionnaire MACH-IV at baseline (t1).

[Legend: Values corrected for age and verbal IQ. Sidak post-hoc tests: (*)p < .10; *p < .05. The values of one control, 2 RCU, and 2 DCU are missing.]

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172853.g001

Notably, although the MACH-IV total scores effect size in DCU was larger than in RCU in comparison to the controls, Machiavellianism did not significantly correlate with dimensional cocaine use measures (Table 3). Importantly, if socio-economic status was introduced as an additional factor in a 2*2 ANOVA (median-split low [CHF 0–30'000] vs. high income [CHF >30'000] * CU vs. controls), the significant group differences in Machiavellianism remained, F(1, 157) = 6.29, p < .05), whereas the factor income was not significant, F(2, 159) = 1.11, p = .29).

thumbnail
Table 3. Correlations with Machiavellianism (MACH-IV total score) in cocaine users (CU) and stimulant-naïve controls at baseline (only Bonferroni-corrected p-values ≤.10 are shown).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172853.t003

The longitudinal analysis using repeated measures ANCOVAs (covariates age and verbal IQ) showed a trend towards significance in the group effect (F(3,95) = 3.19, p = .09), but no-significant effect of time (F(1,95) = 1.21, p = .27) and also no significant group*time interaction for the MACH-IV total scores (F(3,95) = 0.55, p = .65) (Fig 2). An independent Student t-test between controls and a combined cocaine user group (increaser + decreaser + stable user) again confirmed the significant group differences at baseline (t(102) = 2.41, p < .05) and follow-up (t(100) = 3.02, p < .01) for the MACH-IV total score. Additionally, the test-retest reliability of the MACH-IV was relatively high in both controls (r = 0.79, p < .001) and cocaine users (r = 0.70, p < .001) as well as in the total sample (r = 0.75, p < .001).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Mean scores and standard errors of means for the Machiavellianism-questionnaire MACH-IV at baseline (t1) and follow-up (t2).

[Legend: 48 controls, 18 cocaine increaser, 18 cocaine decreaser, and 17 stable cocaine users.]

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172853.g002

Machiavellianism-related TCI personality dimensions

Regarding the TCI scales, DCU showed significantly lower scores in the TCI main scales ‘cooperativeness’ and ‘self-directedness’ than the controls (Table 4), whereas RCU were intermediate between controls and DCU, showing a clear linear trend (cooperativeness: plinear trend < .01, self-directedness plinear trend < .001), but did not significantly differ from controls. With regard to the cooperativeness subscales, DCU reported significantly more social intolerance (C1) and more self-advantageous behavior (C5). Regarding self-directedness, only the subscale ‘resourcefulness vs. apathy’ (SD3) showed no significant difference between DCU and controls. Notably, DCU also differed significantly from RCU in self-directedness, which is mainly explained by significant differences in the subscales ‘responsibility’ (SD1), ‘purposefulness’ (SD2), and ‘congruent second nature’ (SD5).

thumbnail
Table 4. Measures of cooperativeness, reward dependence, and self-directedness in recreational and dependent cocaine users as well as stimulant-naïve controls.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172853.t004

Correlation analyses

In the total sample (n = 166), Machiavellianism correlated negatively with the TCI scales ‘cooperativeness’, ‘reward dependence’, and ‘self-directedness’ (including all subscales), and with social decision-making in the Distribution Game, whereas it was positively linked to all cluster A, B, and ‘other’ PD symptoms according to DSM-IV (Table 3).

In cocaine users (n = 98), we found significant negative correlations for Machiavellianism with ‘cooperativeness’, ‘self-directedness’, and social decision-making, as well as a significant positive correlation for Machiavellianism with the cluster B PD.

Interestingly, higher Machiavellianism was associated with significantly lower implicit emotional empathy in the control group but not in cocaine users. Finally, cocaine users and controls significantly differed in their relationship between Machiavellianism and social network size: Among controls, higher Machiavellianism was linked to a slightly smaller social network, whereas a reverse pattern was detected among cocaine users.

Machiavellianism at baseline and subsequent cocaine use

In order to test whether the level of Machiavellianism at baseline might predict cocaine use at the one-year follow-up, we applied three different analyses. Firstly, we classified all cocaine users as low or high according to a median split based on the MACH-IV total scores at baseline (cut-off score 96/97) [10, 38]. However, increaser (11 low/8 high), decreaser (10 low/8 high), and stable cocaine user (7 low/12 high) did not significantly differ regarding this ratio (χ2 (2) = 2.01, p = .37). Secondly, we compared MACH-IV total scores at baseline between cocaine increaser (mean = 99.52, SD = 3.40), cocaine decreaser (mean = 96.57, SD = 3.40), and stable cocaine users (mean = 98.20, SD = 3.47) by ANCOVA (corrected for age and verbal IQ), but could not find any significant group effect (F(2,51) = 0.19, p = .83). Thirdly, we correlated MACH-IV total scores at baseline with cocaine hair concentrations at follow-up (cocainetotal log) and the percental change of cocaine hair concentrations between baseline and follow-up (Δcocainetotal in %) but could neither detect any significant correlation within a consolidated cocaine user group (t2 = -.11, pt2 = .45; rΔ = .09, pΔ = .49), nor in the single groups of increaser (rt2 = -.14, pt2 = .56; rΔ = .11, pΔ = .65), decreaser (rt2 = -.29, pt2 = .31; rΔ = -.09, pΔ = .73), or stable cocaine users (rt2 = -.09, pt2 = .74; rΔ = .26, pΔ = .28).

Discussion

The major finding of the present study is that both RCU and DCU exhibited a higher level of Machiavellianism than controls, reflecting a stronger tendency toward cynical views on human nature and–albeit less pronounced–toward interpersonal manipulation. Across one year, the elevated Machiavellianism was highly stable in cocaine users. Furthermore, DCU reported lower cooperativeness and self-directedness, which were both significantly correlated with higher Machiavellianism in a combined cocaine user sample (RCU+DCU). In general, controls and cocaine users showed similar relationships between Machiavellianism and socio-cognitive functioning and between Machiavellianism and PD. However, Machiavellianism correlated significantly negative with implicit emotional empathy in controls, whereas this correlation was not significant in cocaine users. Moreover, a negative correlation between Machiavellianism and social network size was displayed in controls, whereas the pattern was significantly reversed in cocaine users. Notably, although Machiavellianism was more prominent in DCU than in RCU, the absence of a significant correlation between Machiavellianism and indicators of cocaine use, as well as the high temporal stability of the MACH-IV scores, suggest that increased Machiavellianism might be a stable personality trait in cocaine users rather than a consequence of cocaine use. The latter has been proposed as a possible explanation for social cognition deficits and smaller social networks of cocaine users, as the cocaine dose has been found to correlate with socio-cognitive performance deficits [6]. Finally, MACH-IV scores of cocaine users at baseline did not predict their change of cocaine use within one year.

Remarkably, the MACH-IV score of 97.5 in our pooled cocaine user sample is highly similar to previously reported scores of 97.8 in abusers of ‘narcotics‘ [11] and 97.0 in methylphenidate abusers [39], indicating that substance use in general might be associated with a Machiavellian personality. Specifically, both cocaine users groups reported a pronounced cynical worldview and an increased use of deceit in interpersonal relationships; however, their morality scores did not differ from controls (it is important to note that morality was only rudimentarily measured with two items, wherein one item assessed morality in a very disputable manner by investigating participants’ opinions about euthanasia). Consequently, the pronounced Machiavellian thoughts and behaviors of cocaine users are probably explained by their unwillingness or inability to comply with the prevailing moral imperatives, although they are actually familiar with them.

In line with this, DCU displayed lower cooperativeness and self-directedness on the TCI, confirming previous findings in regular cocaine users and opiate-dependent individuals [40, 41]. In particular, the lower cooperativeness of DCU was mainly driven by two subscales indicating lower social tolerance and increased pursuit of self-advantage [16]. Furthermore, the subscales for self-directedness clearly reflected lower responsibility, purposefulness, resourcefulness, and self-control in DCU compared with RCU and controls [16]. Together with the elevated novelty seeking scores of our cocaine users [18], this temperament configuration can be called ‘antisocial‘, which is an essential part of the Machiavellian concept [8]. In summary, these findings are in accordance with our previous publications from the ZuCo2St sample in which altered social reward processing [42, 43], increased self-serving behavior [4], reduced emotional empathy, and more criminal offenses [6] were reported for chronic cocaine users.

McHoskey [9] stated that Machiavellianism is associated with various traits that may indicate personality dysfunction when they appear in extreme manifestations. Our findings support this assumption because we found significant positive correlations between Machiavellianism and SCID-II PD symptom scores in the total sample and both subsamples. In particular, the strongest link was found for cluster B PD, which is in line with the pronounced cluster B comorbidity reported in cocaine addicted individuals [7, 44].

The negative correlations between Machiavellianism and monetary payoffs for others in the social interaction tasks reflect the ecological validity of the MACH-IV with regard to prosocial behavior and confirm results reported by Bereczkei and Czibor [15]. These authors demonstrated in a healthy population that individuals with high levels of Machiavellianism maximize their profit in the public goods game. Notably, they also found similar negative relationships between Machiavellianism and the TCI scores self-directedness and cooperativeness as reported here [15].

By contrast, Machiavellianism did not correlate with emotion recognition and mental perspective taking, suggesting that Machiavellianism is independent of the ability to understand another person’s mental and emotional state. However, this finding differs from a previous report with participants who had all obtained at least a high school diploma, showing that the MACH-IV was correlated with self-reported empathy, alexithymia, and emotion recognition [14]. Interestingly, although we found a significant correlation between Machiavellianism and implicit emotional empathy in controls, this relationship was absent in cocaine users. The finding in controls is consistent with the literature, suggesting that high Machiavellianism is linked to diminished capabilities for emotional responses in terms of empathic concern (explicit) or arousal (implicit) [8].

Furthermore, higher Machiavellianism in cocaine users was related to having a broader social network, although their networks were in fact smaller than those of controls [6]. By contrast, controls showed the expected, although non-significant, negative link between Machiavellianism and social network size, confirming that Machiavellianism is inversely related to the self-reported importance of friendship [45]. We therefore hypothesize (I) that cocaine users establish rather low quality social relationships that are not affected by Machiavellian tendencies or (II) that high Machiavellianism is even helpful or popular in their peer environment.

Regarding the possibly underlying cortical structures, our findings fit well into the existing literature indicating a close relationship between chronic cocaine use and prefrontal dysfunctions [46] and between prefrontal dysfunctions and social behavior in general [1, 2] as well as Machiavellianism in particular [38, 47]. Given that Machiavellianism seems to be a stable and distinct trait in cocaine users, our data further support the notion of Ersche et al. [48] indicating that dysfunction of prefrontal control might potentially serve as a predictor for stimulant addiction or might even be considered an addiction endophenotype.

In conclusion, cocaine users showed significantly elevated and highly stable levels of Machiavellianism in general, while DCU also displayed lower cooperativeness and self-directedness measured with the TCI. Because cocaine use intensity and changing cocaine use patterns within one year did not affect Machiavellianism, and methylphenidate abuse has also been associated with higher Machiavellianism in a previous study from our lab [39], we propose that Machiavellianism might be a stable predisposition for stimulant use. Thus, in the future, the MACH-IV could be evaluated as a potential and simple tool for predicting the risk for stimulant addiction and abuse.

Supporting information

Acknowledgments

We thank Daniela Jenni, Joëlle Barthassat, Nina Ingold, Franziska Minder, and Michael D. Wunderli for technical assistance. Moreover, we are grateful to David Cole for critical comments and corrections.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: BBQ.
  2. Data curation: BBQ LMH KHP MRB MV.
  3. Formal analysis: BBQ MV.
  4. Funding acquisition: BBQ.
  5. Investigation: BBQ LMH KHP MRB MV.
  6. Methodology: BBQ LMH KHP MRB CE MV.
  7. Project administration: BBQ LMH KHP MV.
  8. Resources: BBQ MRB.
  9. Software: BBQ LMH KHP MRB MV.
  10. Supervision: BBQ.
  11. Validation: BBQ LMH KHP MRB CE MV.
  12. Visualization: BBQ MV.
  13. Writing – original draft: BBQ MV.
  14. Writing – review & editing: BBQ LMH KHP MRB CE MV.

References

  1. 1. Verdejo-Garcia A. Social cognition in cocaine addiction. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111(7):2406–7. Epub 2014/02/07. PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPmc3932928. pmid:24501129
  2. 2. Quednow BB. Social cognition and interaction in stimulant use disorders. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2017;13:55–62.
  3. 3. Volkow ND, Baler RD, Goldstein RZ. Addiction: pulling at the neural threads of social behaviors. Neuron. 2011;69(4):599–602. pmid:21338873
  4. 4. Hulka LM, Eisenegger C, Preller KH, Vonmoos M, Jenni D, Bendrick K, et al. Altered social and non-social decision-making in recreational and dependent cocaine users. Psychol Med. 2014;44(5):1015–128. pmid:23870112
  5. 5. Hulka LM, Preller KH, Vonmoos M, Broicher SD, Quednow BB. Cocaine users manifest impaired prosodic and cross-modal emotion processing. Front Psychiatry. 2013;4:98. pmid:24046750
  6. 6. Preller KH, Hulka LM, Vonmoos M, Jenni D, Baumgartner MR, Seifritz E, et al. Impaired emotional empathy and related social network deficits in cocaine users. Addict Biol. 2013;19(3):452–66. Epub 2013/06/27. pmid:23800218
  7. 7. Kranzler HR, Satel S, Apter A. Personality disorders and associated features in cocaine-dependent inpatients. Compr Psychiatry. 1994;35(5):335–40. pmid:7995024
  8. 8. Jones DN, Paulhus DL. Machiavellianism. In: Leary MR, Hoyle RH, editors. Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2009. p. 93–108.
  9. 9. McHoskey J. Machiavellianism and personality dysfunction. Pers Individ Dif. 2001;31:791–8.
  10. 10. Christie R, Geis F. Studies in Machiavellianism. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1970.
  11. 11. Kaestner E, Rosen L, Appel P, Sofer S. Manipulativeness among drug abusers: reliability and validity of the Mach IV Scale. Br J Addict. 1977;72(3):245–9. Epub 1977/09/01.
  12. 12. Stenason L, Vernon PA. The Dark Triad, reinforcement sensitivity and substance use. Pers Individ Dif. 2016;94:59–63.
  13. 13. Zizzo DJ. Experimenter demand effects in economic experiments. Experimental Economics. 2010;13(1):75–98.
  14. 14. Al Ain S, Carre A, Fantini-Hauwel C, Baudouin JY, Besche-Richard C. What is the emotional core of the multidimensional Machiavellian personality trait? Front Psychol. 2013;4:454. Epub 2013/07/26. PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPmc3717508. pmid:23885245
  15. 15. Bereczkei T, Czibor A. Personality and situational factors differently influence high Mach and low Mach persons’ decisions in a social dilemma game. Pers Individ Dif. 2014;64(0):168–73.
  16. 16. Cloninger C, Pryzbeck T, Svrakic D, Wetzel R. Das Temperament- und Charakter Inventar TCI: Manual. [The Temperament and Character Inventory TCI: Manual]). Frankfurt: Swets; 1999.
  17. 17. Vonmoos M, Hulka LM, Preller KH, Jenni D, Baumgartner MR, Stohler R, et al. Cognitive dysfunctions in recreational and dependent cocaine users: role of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, craving and early age at onset. Br J Psychiatry. 2013;203(1):35–43. pmid:23703315
  18. 18. Vonmoos M, Hulka LM, Preller KH, Jenni D, Schulz C, Baumgartner MR, et al. Differences in self-reported and behavioral measures of impulsivity in recreational and dependent cocaine users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013;133(1):61–70. pmid:23806872
  19. 19. APA. American Psychological Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV. 4th ed. ed. American Psychiatric Association (APA): Washington D, editor. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (APA); 1994.
  20. 20. APA. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (APA); 1994.
  21. 21. Vonmoos M, Hulka LM, Preller KH, Minder F, Baumgartner MR, Quednow BB. Cognitive impairment in cocaine users is drug-induced but partially reversible: evidence from a longitudinal study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39(9):2200–10. pmid:24651468
  22. 22. Hulka LM, Vonmoos M, Preller KH, Baumgartner MR, Seifritz E, Gamma A, et al. Changes in cocaine consumption are associated with fluctuations in self-reported impulsivity and gambling decision-making. Psychol Med. 2015;45(14):3097–110. Epub 2015/06/18. pmid:26081043
  23. 23. Bush DM. The U.S. Mandatory Guidelines for Federal Workplace Drug Testing Programs: current status and future considerations. Forensic Sci Int. 2008;174(2–3):111–9. Epub 2007/04/17. pmid:17434274
  24. 24. Cooper GA, Kronstrand R, Kintz P. Society of Hair Testing guidelines for drug testing in hair. Forensic Sci Int. 2012;218(1–3):20–4. Epub 2011/11/18. pmid:22088946
  25. 25. Hoelzle C, Scheufler F, Uhl M, Sachs H, Thieme D. Application of discriminant analysis to differentiate between incorporation of cocaine and its congeners into hair and contamination. Forensic Sci Int. 2008;176(1):13–8. Epub 2007/12/08. pmid:18063333
  26. 26. Fydrich T, Renneberg B, Schmitz B, Wittchen H-U. SKID-II Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV, Achse II: Persönlichkeitsstörungen. [SCID-II Structured Cinical Interview for DSM-IV, Axis II: Personality Disorders.]. Goettingen: Hogrefe; 1997.
  27. 27. Dziobek I, Rogers K, Fleck S, Bahnemann M, Heekeren HR, Wolf OT, et al. Dissociation of cognitive and emotional empathy in adults with Asperger syndrome using the Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET). J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38(3):464–73. pmid:17990089
  28. 28. Dziobek I, Fleck S, Kalbe E, Rogers K, Hassenstab J, Brand M, et al. Introducing MASC: a movie for the assessment of social cognition. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006;36(5):623–36. Epub 2006/06/07. pmid:16755332
  29. 29. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I. The "Reading the Mind in the Eyes" Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42(2):241–51. pmid:11280420
  30. 30. Engelmann D, Strobel M. Inequality aversion, efficiency, and maximin preferences in simple distribution experiments. Am Econ Rev. 2004;94(4):857–69.
  31. 31. Charness G, Rabin M. Understanding social preferences with simple tests. Q J Econ. 2002:817–69.
  32. 32. Linden M, Lischka A-M, Popien C, Golombek J. Der multidimensionale Sozialkontakt Kreis (MuSK)–ein Interviewverfahren zur Erfassung des sozialen Netzes in der klinischen Praxis. [The Multidimensional Social Contact Circle–an Interview for the Assessment of the Social Network in Clinicle Practical]. Z Med Psychol. 2007;16(3):135–43.
  33. 33. Sussner BD, Smelson DA, Rodrigues S, Kline A, Losonczy M, Ziedonis D. The validity and reliability of a brief measure of cocaine craving. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2006;83(3):233–7. pmid:16384655
  34. 34. Quednow BB, Kuhn KU, Hoenig K, Maier W, Wagner M. Prepulse inhibition and habituation of acoustic startle response in male MDMA ('ecstasy') users, cannabis users, and healthy controls. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29(5):982–90. pmid:14970829
  35. 35. Lehrl S. Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest. MWT-B. 4th ed. Balingen: Spitta; 1999.
  36. 36. Cohen J, Cohen P. Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1983.
  37. 37. Edgington ES, Onghena P. Randomization Tests. 4th ed. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC; 2007.
  38. 38. McNamara P, Durso R, Harris E. "Machiavellianism" and frontal dysfunction: evidence from Parkinson's disease. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2007;12(4):285–300. Epub 2007/06/15. pmid:17558639
  39. 39. Maier LJ, Wunderli MD, Vonmoos M, Rommelt AT, Baumgartner MR, Seifritz E, et al. Pharmacological Cognitive Enhancement in Healthy Individuals: A Compensation for Cognitive Deficits or a Question of Personality? PLoS One. 2015;10(6):e0129805. PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4479570. pmid:26107846
  40. 40. Herrero MJ, Domingo-Salvany A, Torrens M, Brugal MT, Gutierrez F, Itinere I. Personality profile in young current regular users of cocaine. Subst Use Misuse. 2008;43(10):1378–94. pmid:18696374
  41. 41. Milivojevic D, Milovanovic SD, Jovanovic M, Svrakic DM, Svrakic NM, Svrakic SM, et al. Temperament and character modify risk of drug addiction and influence choice of drugs. Am J Addict. 2012;21(5):462–7. Epub 2012/08/14. pmid:22882397
  42. 42. Preller KH, Herdener M, Schilbach L, Stampfli P, Hulka LM, Vonmoos M, et al. Functional changes of the reward system underlie blunted response to social gaze in cocaine users. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111(7):2842–7. Epub 2014/01/23. PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3932870. pmid:24449854
  43. 43. Tobler PN, Preller KH, Campbell-Meiklejohn DK, Kirschner M, Kraehenmann R, Stampfli P, et al. Shared neural basis of social and non-social reward deficits in chronic cocaine users. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2016;11(6):1017–25. Epub 2016/03/13. PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPmc4884323. pmid:26969866
  44. 44. Fernandez-Montalvo J, Lorea I. Comorbilidad de la adicción a la cocaína con los trastornos de la personalidad. An Sist Sanit Navar. 2007;30(2):225–31. pmid:17898818
  45. 45. Lyons M, Aitken S. Machiavellian friends? The role of Machiavellianism in friendship formation and maintenance. J Soc Evol Cult Psychol. 2010;4(3):194–202.
  46. 46. Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND. Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12(11):652–69. Epub 2011/10/21. PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPmc3462342. pmid:22011681
  47. 47. Verbeke W, Rietdijk W, van den Berg W, Dietvorst R, Worm L, Bagozzi R. The making of the Machiavellian brain: A structural MRI analysis. J Neurosci Psychol Econ. 2011;4(4):205–16.
  48. 48. Ersche KD, Turton AJ, Pradhan S, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW. Drug Addiction Endophenotypes: Impulsive Versus Sensation-Seeking Personality Traits. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68(8):770–3. pmid:20678754