Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

A Population Based Study of the Genetic Association between Catecholamine Gene Variants and Spontaneous Low-Frequency Fluctuations in Reaction Time

  • Jojanneke A. Bastiaansen ,

    j.bastiaansen@umcg.nl

    Affiliation Interdisciplinary Center Psychopathology and Emotion regulation, Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands

  • Tarrant D. R. Cummins,

    Affiliation School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia

  • Harriëtte Riese,

    Affiliations Interdisciplinary Center Psychopathology and Emotion regulation, Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands, Department of Epidemiology, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands

  • Arie M. van Roon,

    Affiliation Department of Vascular Medicine, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands

  • Ilja M. Nolte,

    Affiliation Department of Epidemiology, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands

  • Albertine J. Oldehinkel,

    Affiliation Interdisciplinary Center Psychopathology and Emotion regulation, Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands

  • Mark A. Bellgrove

    Affiliation School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia

Abstract

The catecholamines dopamine and noradrenaline have been implicated in spontaneous low-frequency fluctuations in reaction time, which are associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and subclinical attentional problems. The molecular genetic substrates of these behavioral phenotypes, which reflect frequency ranges of intrinsic neuronal oscillations (Slow-4: 0.027-0.073 Hz; Slow-5: 0.010-0.027 Hz), have not yet been investigated. In this study, we performed regression analyses with an additive model to examine associations between low-frequency fluctuations in reaction time during a sustained attention task and genetic markers across 23 autosomal catecholamine genes in a large young adult population cohort (n = 964), which yielded greater than 80% power to detect a small effect size (f2 = 0.02) and 100% power to detect a small/medium effect size (f2 = 0.15). At significance levels corrected for multiple comparisons, none of the gene variants were associated with the magnitude of low-frequency fluctuations. Given the study’s strong statistical power and dense coverage of the catecholamine genes, this either indicates that associations between low-frequency fluctuation measures and catecholamine gene variants are absent or that they are of very small effect size. Nominally significant associations were observed between variations in the alpha-2A adrenergic receptor gene (ADRA2A) and the Slow-5 band. This is in line with previous reports of an association between ADRA2A gene variants and general reaction time variability during response selection tasks, but the specific association of these gene variants and low-frequency fluctuations requires further confirmation. Pharmacological challenge studies could in the future provide convergent evidence for the noradrenergic modulation of both general and time sensitive measures of intra-individual variability in reaction time.

Introduction

Intra-subject variability in reaction time (RT) contains specific temporal components of relevance for different forms of psychopathology [1]. A greater magnitude of low-frequency fluctuations in RT (< 0.1 Hz) has been associated with ADHD and subclinical attentional problems [16]. This suggests that an inability to modulate low-frequency RT fluctuations may underlie behavioral symptoms such as difficulty sustaining attention [1,2]. The default mode interference hypothesis [7] proposes that slow periodic fluctuations in behavioral performance are caused by an ineffective shift from a default mode to an active processing mode during cognitive challenges. The inability to modulate low-frequency RT fluctuations could be caused by a catecholamine deficiency [2]. Catecholamines dopamine (DA) and noradrenaline (NA) are critical for the ability to sustain attention [8,9] and recent studies have shown that they modulate deactivation of the default mode network (DMN) and its functional coupling with task-related networks (e.g. [1014]). The question then arises: do alterations in the regulation of catecholamine signaling modulate the strength of low-frequency RT fluctuations?

Gene variants that influence the functioning of catecholamines could throw light on this relationship. A plausible candidate is the functional val158met single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) on the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene [15,16], which affects baseline DA levels [1719]. The COMT/val158met SNP may be involved in low-frequency RT fluctuations, because its effect on cognition seems specific for tasks that require the maintenance of task-relevant representations [2022], and it has been associated with altered functional connectivity within and between DMN and task-relevant regions [13,23,24]. The relationship between low-frequency RT fluctuations and catecholamine gene variants has not yet been addressed. Cummins et al [25] did recently show that variations in the alpha-2A adrenergic receptor gene (ADRA2A) are associated with general intra-individual RT variability using an association approach focused on the catecholamine system. Moreover, RT variability mediated the relationship between the ADRA2A SNP rs1800544 and self-reported ADHD symptoms.

Here, we used a similar association approach to study the molecular genetics of low-frequency (i.e. time sensitive) RT fluctuations. We administered a sustained attention RT task in a large young adult population cohort to investigate associations between the power in two low-frequency bands reminiscent of the DMN’s rhythm (Slow-4: 0.027–0.073 Hz, Slow-5: 0.010–0.027 Hz [3,26]) and catecholamine gene variants. We explored SNPs and a variable number tandem repeat (VNTR) polymorphism from 23 autosomal catecholamine genes, with COMT/val158met and ADRA2A gene variants as a priori candidates.

Materials and Methods

Study sample

We used neuropsychological data from the fourth wave (T4) and genetic data of the TRacking Adolescents’ Individual Lives Survey (TRAILS), a large representative prospective population cohort study from the North of the Netherlands. The baseline sample comprised a diverse sample of 2230 10–12 year-olds of predominantly Dutch ethnicity (89.7%), of whom 1881 (83.4%) participated at T4 and 1515 (77.4% of the total T4 TRAILS sample) successfully completed a sustained attention task (criteria are described in [1]). Genotypic data were available for 1095 of these participants, of whom one of each sibling pair and participants from non-Dutch ancestry were excluded (n = 87). An additional 44 participants were excluded because they used medication targeting catecholaminergic systems, which could affect our behavioral measures (see below). The final sample for this study therefore included 964 individuals (mean age: 19.0 ± 0.6 years, 53.3% females). The selected participants were slightly younger (mean age = 19.0 ± 0.6 years) than the unselected T4 participants (n = 790, mean age = 19.2 ± 0.6 years, t(1752) = 7.4, p < .001), but similar in male-female ratio (t(1752) = 1.0, p = .31). The TRAILS study was approved by the Dutch Central Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects (CCMO, no. NL22114.042.08). Written informed consent was obtained from both the parents and the participants. For more details on the sampling procedure and methods see [2729].

Medication exclusion

Medication use was coded according to the Anatomical Therapeutic Chemical (ATC) classification system of the WHO Collaborating Centre for Drug Statistics Methodology (http://www.whocc.no). Participants were excluded if they used medication targeting catecholaminergic systems, that is, cardiovascular medication based on beta blocking agents (C07A) such as propranolol (adrenergic receptor blocker), drugs for functional gastrointestinal disorders (A03) such as domperidone (dopamine receptor antagonist), adrenergic respiratory medication (R03A) such as salbutamol (adrenergic receptor agonist), psychostimulants (N06B) such as methylphenidate (dopamine reuptake inhibitor), antipsychotics (N05A) such as haloperidol (mainly dopamine receptor antagonists), and the sex hormone cabergoline (a dopamine receptor agonist).

Neurocognitive task

The Sustained Attention Dot (SAD) patterns task was part of a test battery of five tasks assessing neurocognitive functioning taken from the Amsterdam Neuropsychological Tasks (ANT) battery [30]. During the SAD task, participants had to respond as quickly as possible to patterns with 3, 4 or 5 dots by pressing the mouse button with their dominant hand (‘yes’ response) when there were 4 dots, and pressing the mouse button with their non-dominant hand (‘no’ response) when there were 3 or 5 dots. The three types of dot patterns occurred with equal frequency and were randomly presented across 50 series of 12 trials without breaks. A dot pattern disappeared immediately after a response had been given (valid response window: 200–6000 ms), entailing that the task pace was largely determined by the participant’s behavior. Each stimulus was followed by a fixed post response interval of 250 ms.

Frequency domain analysis

Detailed procedures concerning data preparation, frequency domain definitions, and frequency domain analysis have been described previously [1]. In brief, the RT data were: 1) normalized by a natural logarithmic transformation (ln) with a displacement parameter of 150 ms; and 2) individually adjusted for the average reaction time for each stimulus type (i.e., 3, 4, or 5 dots). Power spectral analysis was applied to these adjusted logarithmic RT series (alnRT) based on fast Fourier Transform using CARSPAN [31]. Power was calculated for neurophysiologically defined oscillation bands Slow-4 (0.027–0.073 Hz) and Slow-5 (0.010–0.027 Hz) [3,26] as DMN-oscillations have generally been related to frequencies below 0.1 Hz (e.g. [32,33]). Note that power is a relative measure; it indicates the percentage of the total variance the Slow-4 and Slow-5 bands account for in a particular subject. Higher values are indicative of an amplification at these power spectra (i.e., a greater magnitude of these low-frequency RT fluctuations).

DNA extraction and Genotyping

Blood samples or buccal swabs (Cytobrush) were collected at T3. DNA extraction, using the manual salting out procedure [34], and genotyping of all SNPs were performed at the Department of Genetics, University Medical Center Groningen, The Netherlands. Genotyping was executed on the Golden Gate Illumina BeadStation 500 platform (Illumina Inc., San Diego, CA, USA) according to the manufacturer’s protocol. Genotype data clustering was performed in BeadStudio 3.0 (Illumina Inc., San Diego, CA, USA). Concordance between DNA replicates showed a 100% genotyping accuracy. Data cleaning was in line with standard procedures [35]. In addition to this candidate gene genotyping, genome-wide genotyping was performed using the Illumina HumanCytoSNP12 v2 beadchip assay (Illumina, Inc; San Diego, CA, USA). Genotypes were called with the Illumina GenomeStudio software package (Illumina, Inc). Details on genotyping and quality control have been given elsewhere [36]. Next, imputation was carried out with Impute v2 [37] using the 1000 Genomes (release March 2012) as reference panel.

For the DRD4 VNTR polymorphism, the 48 base pair repeat sequence located in exon 3 was determined by direct analysis on an automated capillary sequencer (ABI3730, Applied Biosystems, Nieuwerkerk a/d IJssel, The Netherlands) using standard conditions at the CCKL quality-certified Research lab for Multifactorial Diseases within the Human Genetics department of the Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Centre (Nijmegen, The Netherlands). Three percent blanks as well as duplicates between plates were included for quality controls. Call rate and DNA replicate consistency were 98% and 100%, respectively. Genotypic data for the DRD4 VNTR were coded with 0, 1, and 2 to represent the number of copies of the 7 repeat allele.

Genetic variant selection

Genetic variants were selected according to the catecholamine system-wide association approach developed by Cummins and colleagues [25,38]. In brief, SNPs were selected from 23 autosomal catecholamine genes, namely those that are known to be involved in synthesis, degradation, transport and receptor signaling of DA and/or NA (as identified in the KEGG, [http://www.genome.jp/kegg/pathway.html] and Gene Ontology [http://www.geneontology.org]) metabolic pathway databases). Note that the ANKK1 gene was included in the set of autosomal catecholamine genes, because SNPs within this gene are in strong linkage disequilibrium (LD) with SNPs of DRD2 [39]. Haplotype tagging SNPs were identified to provide 5′–3′ coverage of each gene using the HapMap project database [40]. We favored the inclusion of SNPs as tags if they had been shown to modify the risk for ADHD, were at exon—intron boundaries (300bp) or mapped to within 5kb of the genes of interest including the 5′ and 3′ untranslated (UTR) regions, which may have regulatory roles. Only tagging SNPs (r2 ≥ 0.8) with a minor allele frequency of > 0.1 were selected (see [25] for SNP selection criteria). This process yielded dense coverage of the 23 autosomal catecholamine genes with 152 tagging SNPs. In addition, the 48 base pair VNTR mapped to exon 3 of the DRD4 gene was included.

This selection of SNPs/VNTRs was extracted from the TRAILS database. We included rs4680 of the COMT gene and SNPs for the ADRA2C and PNMT genes that failed during genotyping in the original papers [25,38]. However, two previously studied VNTRs in the SLC6A3 gene [25,38] were not genotyped in the TRAILS dataset and hence could not be included in this study.

Genetic association analyses

Initial correlation analyses indicated that while age was not significantly associated with the Slow-4 and Slow-5 measures, gender approached significant association at uncorrected levels with Slow-4 (r = .06, p = .056) and was significantly associated at corrected significance levels with Slow-5 (r = .16, p<.001). Thus, gender was included as a covariate in a single step Monte-Carlo permutation method that tested for genetic associations with Slow-4 and Slow-5 via an additive regression model. Permutation methods are considered the gold standard when conducting association analyses, because they provide unbiased control for type 1 error and maintain statistical power while correcting for multiple comparisons. As densely typed SNP sets show complex patterns of correlation (LD) arising from DNA recombination, our permutation method corrected for correlated samples (SNP data) to give a critical value of α = .00042 for examining the association between a cognitive measure and any genetic marker (see [25] for a full description of this method). We then corrected for the number of cognitive variables examined (2; Slow-4, Slow-5) to give a final critical value, of α = .00021 (.00042/2), αFWE = .05. With this permutation method that stringently corrects for both genotypic and phenotypic measures, our sample size of 964 participants yielded greater than 80% power to detect a small effect size (f2 = 0.02) and 100% power to detect a small/medium effect size (f2 = 0.15). We also present results that are significant at the uncorrected significance level for our a priori candidates (COMT/val158met and ADRA2A gene variants), and note that we have 100% power to detect associations of small effect size at this level. Power calculations were performed using G*Power 3.1 [41].

Results

Genotyping

149 SNPs remained after a quality control process (S1 Table). SNPs that were excluded from analyses were: rs11568324, rs3730287 and rs62388321 (SLC6A2, ADRA1A and DRD1 genes, respectively), as the former SNP had a MAF <.05 in our sample while the latter two SNPs were homozygous for the major allele. All of the remaining SNPs were in Hardy Weinberg Equilibrium (at pcritical = .001).

Genetic association analyses

The COMT/val158met SNP was not significantly associated with either RT variability measure (Slow-4, Slow-5) at corrected or uncorrected levels (Table 1). The ADRA2A SNP rs1800544 showed a significant association at the uncorrected level with the low-frequency RT fluctuation measure Slow-5 (puncorrected = .007, rsp = .089). There was an additive increase in relative Slow-5 power with each copy of the G allele of rs1800544 (Fig 1). Additionally, each of the three other ADRA2A SNPs (rs521674, rs11195419, rs602618) showed association at nominal (uncorrected) significance (Table 1). Although a number of other SNPs distributed across the 23 autosomal catecholamine genes showed nominally significant associations, none (including the ADRA2A SNPs) survived correction for multiple comparisons (S1 Table).

thumbnail
Table 1. The influence of COMT/val158met and ADRA2A gene variants on low-frequency fluctuation measures Slow-4 and Slow-5.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0126461.t001

thumbnail
Fig 1. The relationship between genotypic variation at ADRA2A rs1800544 and the low-frequency RT fluctuation measure Slow-5.

Relative power in the Slow-5 band (%) increased with each copy of the G allele.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0126461.g001

Discussion

Catecholamine systems have been implicated in low-frequency RT fluctuations, which are associated with attentional problems. In this study, we examined associations between low-frequency RT fluctuation measures (Slow-4, Slow-5) and genetic markers. The results were clear-cut: at corrected significance levels, none of the gene variants were associated with the magnitude of low-frequency RT fluctuations. Despite our a priori hypothesis, there were no associations between the COMT/val158met SNP and the Slow-4 or Slow-5 measures at either the corrected or uncorrected significance levels. Associations were observed between all ADRA2A gene variants and Slow-5, but only at the uncorrected level. Critically, our tagging SNP design provided dense coverage of 23 autosomal catecholamine genes, which entails that any association signal generated from an unselected catecholamine SNP would be well represented by its proxy (tag) SNP. In addition, our large sample size provided sufficient statistical power to detect associations of small effect size after correction for multiple comparisons. Thus, the fact that none of the SNP associations survived correction for multiple comparisons either indicates that associations between low-frequency RT fluctuation measures and catecholamine SNPs are not to be found or that they are of very small effect size in this general population sample.

The fact that catecholamine gene variants were not associated with low-frequency RT fluctuations during a sustained attention task does not exclude the possibility that different results might have been obtained with a different task. However, the long SAD task with its limited cognitive demand seems very suited to induce attentional lapses. Both children and adults with ADHD have shown impairments on the task, which can be improved by methylphenidate (e.g. [4244]). Moreover, low-frequency RT fluctuations documented during this task have been linked to attentional problems in a general population sample [1]. Further, the task is qualitatively very similar to other cognitive paradigms such as the Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART), which have been used to document differences between ADHD cases and controls in low-frequency RT fluctuations [4,5]. While some studies have found a stronger link with attentional problems for the Slow-4 band rather than the Slow-5 band or higher-frequency bands [1, 3], the specificity is still a matter of debate [45].

It is also possible that measures of intra-subject variability may not be invariant across cognitive tasks. Through principal component analysis on general variability measures (i.e. intra-individual coefficients of variation, ICVs) across different cognitive tasks, Cummins and colleagues [25] recently found two variability components. Tasks that, similar to our SAD task, required subjects to select one response from among competing response alternatives loaded strongly on the component termed the response selection variability factor [25]. Individual differences in this factor were predicted by variation in the ADRA2A gene, and the factor mediated the relationship between the ADRA2A SNP rs1800544 and self-reported ADHD symptoms. In this context, it is noteworthy that we observed a small but significant association at the uncorrected significance level for all of the ADRA2A SNPs under study here and individual differences in the Slow-5 RT fluctuation measure. Although ADRA2A seems implicated in RT fluctuations, the directionality and specificity of effects requires further investigation. In contrast to additive decreases in (absolute) ICV scores in the previous paper, we found that relative power in Slow-5 increased with each copy of the G-allele (rs1800544) and C-allele (rs602618). However, higher relative power in a frequency band does not preclude lower overall variance. The strong LD between rs1800544 lying in the promoter region and rs602618 lying in the 3′ untranslated region (r2 = .93, D' = .97) of the ADRA2A gene suggests that these results may reflect a functional variant that lies between these SNPs or within a greater haplotype block.

One may speculate about the significance of the association between ADRA2A gene variants and intra-subject variability as assessed here using low-frequency fluctuations and in [25] using ICV. It is well-established that pharmacological modulation of the pre-synaptic α2A autoreceptor using low dose clonidine reduces noradrenergic release and induces attentional lapses [46]. These effects can be reversed using the α2A antagonist idazoxan [46]. That methylphenidate is able to reduce attentional lapses and stabilize intra-subject variability in both ADHD and non-ADHD populations [2, 4749] may be understood in terms of its well-documented effect on alpha2a receptors in prefrontal cortex [50]. Thus, methylphenidate may selectively engage the α2A receptor and so promote an optimized prefrontal cortex signal-to-noise ratio, which stabilizes fluctuations in top-down control of attention that may contribute to intra-subject variability. The associations observed between ADRA2A gene variants and Slow-5 further suggest that stimulation of the α2A receptor may play a role in slow periodic fluctuations in behavioral performance that are reminiscent of the DMN’s intrinsic rhythm (e.g. [51]). Whereas some studies suggest that low-frequency oscillations in the Slow-5 band might be most robust in default mode structures and Slow-4 oscillations might be strongest in the basal ganglia [5153], the exact brain-behavior relationship of the frequency bands still requires clarification. The specific relationship between the α2A receptor, RT fluctuations, and the DMN, could be tested using selective α2A agents to observe both modulations of intra-subject variability (ICV or Slow-4 and Slow-5) and the intrinsic rhythm of the DMN.

Conclusions

In sum, we did not find strong evidence for associations between catecholamine gene variants and low-frequency RT fluctuations. Our findings are in line with previous reports of an association between ADRA2A gene variants and RT variability during response selection tasks, but the specific association of these gene variants and low-frequency fluctuations requires further confirmation. Studies are now needed that integrate behavioral and neurophysiological measures across a range of cognitive domains to elucidate the functional meaning of the different frequency bands. Moreover, pharmacological challenge studies using receptor specific α2A agents could provide important convergent evidence for the noradrenergic modulation of both general (ICV) and time sensitive (e.g., Slow-5) measures of intra-individual variability.

Supporting Information

S1 Table. The influence of common catecholamine gene variants on the low-frequency RT fluctuation measures of Slow-4 and Slow-5.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0126461.s002

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to all adolescents, their parents, and teachers who participated in the TRacking Adolescents' Individual Lives Survey (TRAILS), and to everyone who worked on this project and made it possible.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: JAB MAB TDRC AJO. Analyzed the data: TDRC JAB AMVR. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: HR IMN AJO. Wrote the paper: JAB MAB TDRC HR AMVR IMN AJO.

References

  1. 1. Bastiaansen JA, van Roon AM, Buitelaar JK, Oldehinkel AJ (2015) Mental health problems are associated with low-frequency fluctuations in reaction time in a large general population sample. The TRAILS study. Eur Psychiatry 30: 347–353. pmid:24909359
  2. 2. Castellanos FX, Sonuga-Barke EJS, Scheres A, Di Martino A, Hyde C, Walters JR (2005) Varieties of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-related intra-individual variability. Biol Psychiatry 57: 1416–1423. pmid:15950016
  3. 3. Di Martino A, Ghaffari M, Curchack J, Reiss P, Hyde C, Vannucci M, et al. (2008) Decomposing intra-subject variability in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 64: 607–614. pmid:18423424
  4. 4. Johnson KA, Kelly SP, Bellgrove MA, Barry E, Cox M, Gill M, et al. (2007) Response variability in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: evidence for neuropsychological heterogeneity. Neuropsychologia 45: 630–638. pmid:17157885
  5. 5. Johnson KA, Robertson IH, Kelly SP, Silk TJ, Barry E, Dáibhis A, et al. (2007) Dissociation in performance of children with ADHD and high-functioning autism on a task of sustained attention. Neuropsychologia 45: 2234–2245. pmid:17433378
  6. 6. Yordanova J, Albrecht B, Uebel H, Kirov R, Banaschewski T, Rothenberger A, et al. (2011) Independent oscillatory patterns determine performance fluctuations in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Brain 134: 1740–1750. pmid:21616970
  7. 7. Sonuga-Barke EJS, Castellanos FX (2007) Spontaneous attentional fluctuations in impaired states and pathological conditions: a neurobiological hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 31: 977–986. pmid:17445893
  8. 8. Bellgrove MA, Hawi Z, Gill M, Robertson IH (2006) The cognitive genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): sustained attention as a candidate phenotype. Cortex 42: 838–845. pmid:17131588
  9. 9. Arnsten AFT (1997) Catecholamine regulation of the prefrontal cortex. J Psychopharmacol 11: 151–162. pmid:9208378
  10. 10. Dang LC, O’Neil JP, Jagust WJ (2012) Dopamine supports coupling of attention-related networks. J Neurosci 32: 9582–9587. pmid:22787044
  11. 11. Kelly C, de Zubicaray G, Di Martino A, Copland DA, Reiss PT, Klein DF, et al. (2009) L-dopa modulates functional connectivity in striatal cognitive and motor networks: a double-blind placebo-controlled study. J Neurosci 29: 7364–7378. pmid:19494158
  12. 12. Liddle EB, Hollis C, Batty MJ, Groom MJ, Totman JJ, Liotti M, et al. (2011) Task-related default mode network modulation and inhibitory control in ADHD: effects of motivation and methylphenidate. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 52: 761–771. pmid:21073458
  13. 13. Liu B, Song M, Li J, Liu Y, Li K, Yu C, et al. (2010) Prefrontal-related functional connectivities within the default network are modulated by COMT val158met in healthy young adults. J Neurosci 30: 64–69. pmid:20053888
  14. 14. Tomasi D, Volkow ND, Wang R, Telang F, Wang G-J, Chang L, et al. (2009) Dopamine transporters in striatum correlate with deactivation in the default mode network during visuospatial attention. PLoS ONE 4: e6102. pmid:19564918
  15. 15. Lotta T, Vidgren J, Tilgmann C, Ulmanen I, Melén K, Julkunen I, et al. (1995) Kinetics of human soluble and membrane-bound catechol O-methyltransferase: a revised mechanism and description of the thermolabile variant of the enzyme. Biochemistry 34: 4202–4210. pmid:7703232
  16. 16. Männistö PT, Kaakkola S (1999) Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): Biochemistry, Molecular Biology, Pharmacology, and Clinical Efficacy of the New Selective COMT Inhibitors. Pharmacol Rev 51: 593–628. pmid:10581325
  17. 17. Chen J, Lipska BK, Halim N, Ma QD, Matsumoto M, Melhem S, et al. (2004) Functional analysis of genetic variation in catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): effects on mRNA, protein, and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. Am J Hum Genet 75: 807–821. pmid:15457404
  18. 18. Lachman HM, Papolos DF, Saito T, Yu YM, Szumlanski CL, Weinshilboum RM (1996) Human catechol-O-methyltransferase pharmacogenetics: description of a functional polymorphism and its potential application to neuropsychiatric disorders. Pharmacogenetics 6: 243–250. pmid:8807664
  19. 19. Weinshilboum RM, Otterness DM, Szumlanski CL (1999) Methylation pharmacogenetics: catechol O-methyltransferase, thiopurine methyltransferase, and histamine N-methyltransferase. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol 39: 19–52. pmid:10331075
  20. 20. Bilder RM, Volavka J, Lachman HM, Grace AA (2004) The catechol-O-methyltransferase polymorphism: relations to the tonic-phasic dopamine hypothesis and neuropsychiatric phenotypes. Neuropsychopharmacology 29: 1943–1961. pmid:15305167
  21. 21. Cools R, D’Esposito M (2011) Inverted-U—Shaped Dopamine Actions on Human Working Memory and Cognitive Control. Biological Psychiatry 69: e113–e125. pmid:21531388
  22. 22. Goldman D, Weinberger DR, Malhotra AK, Goldberg TE (2009) The Role of COMT Val158Met in Cognition. Biol Psychiatry 65: e1–e4. pmid:18838132
  23. 23. Sambataro F, Reed JD, Murty VP, Das S, Tan HY, Callicott J, et al. (2009) Catechol-O-methyltransferase valine(158)methionine polymorphism modulates brain networks underlying working memory across adulthood. Biol Psychiatry 66: 540–548. pmid:19539269
  24. 24. Tunbridge EM, Farrell SM, Harrison PJ, Mackay CE (2013) Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) influences the connectivity of the prefrontal cortex at rest. Neuroimage 68: 49–54. pmid:23228511
  25. 25. Cummins TDR, Jacoby O, Hawi Z, Nandam LS, Byrne MAV, Kim B-N, et al. (2013) Alpha-2A adrenergic receptor gene variants are associated with increased intra-individual variability in response time. Mol Psychiatry.
  26. 26. Penttonen M, Buzsáki G (2003) Natural logarithmic relationship between brain oscillators. Thalamus Relat Syst 2: 145–152.
  27. 27. De Winter AF, Oldehinkel AJ, Veenstra R, Brunnekreef JA, Verhulst FC, Ormel J (2005) Evaluation of non-response bias in mental health determinants and outcomes in a large sample of pre-adolescents. Eur J Epidemiol 20: 173–181. pmid:15792285
  28. 28. Huisman M, Oldehinkel AJ, de Winter A, Minderaa RB, de Bildt A, Huizink AC, et al. (2008) Cohort profile: the Dutch “TRacking Adolescents” Individual Lives’ Survey’; TRAILS. Int J Epidemiol 37: 1227–1235. pmid:18263649
  29. 29. Ormel J, Oldehinkel AJ, Sijtsema J, van Oort F, Raven D, Veenstra R, et al. (2012) The TRacking Adolescents’ Individual Lives Survey (TRAILS): Design, Current Status, and Selected Findings. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 51: 1020–1036. pmid:23021478
  30. 30. De Sonneville LMJ. Amsterdam Neuropsychological Tasks: a computer-aided assessment program. In: den Brinker BPLM, Beek PJ, Brand AN, Maarse SJ, Mulder LJM, editors. Cognitive ergonomics, clinical assessment and computer-assisted learning: Computers in Psychology. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger; 1999. p. 187–203.
  31. 31. Dietrich A, Riese H, van Roon AM, van Engelen K, Ormel J, Neeleman J, et al. (2006) Spontaneous baroreflex sensitivity in (pre)adolescents. J Hypertens 24: 345–352. pmid:16508583
  32. 32. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME (2005) The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102: 9673–9678. pmid:15976020
  33. 33. Cordes D, Haughton VM, Arfanakis K, Carew JD, Turski PA, Moritz CH, et al. (2001) Frequencies contributing to functional connectivity in the cerebral cortex in “resting-state” data. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 22: 1326–1333. pmid:11498421
  34. 34. Miller SA, Dykes DD, Polesky HF (1988) A simple salting out procedure for extracting DNA from human nucleated cells. Nucleic Acids Res 16: 1215. pmid:3344216
  35. 35. Nolte IM, McCaffery JM, Snieder H (2010) Candidate Gene and Genome-Wide Association Studies in Behavioral Medicine. In: Steptoe A, editor. Handbook of Behavioral Medicine. Springer New York. pp. 423–441. Available: http://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-0-387-09488-5_29. Accessed 5 May 2014.
  36. 36. Ganesh SK, Tragante V, Guo W, Guo Y, Lanktree MB, Smith EN, et al. (2013) Loci influencing blood pressure identified using a cardiovascular gene-centric array. Hum Mol Genet 22: 1663–1678. pmid:23303523
  37. 37. Howie BN, Donnelly P, Marchini J (2009) A flexible and accurate genotype imputation method for the next generation of genome-wide association studies. PLoS Genet 5: e1000529. pmid:19543373
  38. 38. Cummins TDR, Hawi Z, Hocking J, Strudwick M, Hester R, Garavan H, et al. (2012) Dopamine transporter genotype predicts behavioural and neural measures of response inhibition. Mol Psychiatry 17: 1086–1092. pmid:21876545
  39. 39. Neville MJ, Johnstone EC, Walton RT (2004) Identification and characterization of ANKK1: a novel kinase gene closely linked to DRD2 on chromosome band 11q23.1. Hum Mutat 23: 540–545. pmid:15146457
  40. 40. The International HapMap Consortium (2005) A haplotype map of the human genome. Nature 437: 1299–1320. pmid:16255080
  41. 41. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang A-G (2009) Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods 41: 1149–1160. pmid:19897823
  42. 42. De Sonneville LMJ, Njiokiktjien C, Bos H (1994) Methylphenidate and information processing. part 1: Differentiation between responders and nonresponders; part 2: Efficacy in responders. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 16: 877–897. pmid:7890822
  43. 43. Konrad K, Günther T, Hanisch C, Herpertz-Dahlmann B (2004) Differential Effects of Methylphenidate on Attentional Functions in Children With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 43: 191–198. pmid:14726726
  44. 44. Marchetta NDJ, Hurks PPM, De Sonneville LMJ, Krabbendam L, Jolles J (2008) Sustained and focused attention deficits in adult ADHD. J Atten Disord 11: 664–676. pmid:17712171
  45. 45. Karalunas SL, Huang-Pollock CL, Nigg JT (2012) Is reaction time variability in ADHD mainly at low frequencies? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 54: 536–44. pmid:23278286
  46. 46. Smith A, Nutt D (1996) Noradrenaline and attention lapses. Nature 380: 291. pmid:8598925
  47. 47. Epstein JN, Brinkman WB, Froehlich T, Langberg JM, Narad ME, Antonini TN, et al. (2011) Effects of Stimulant Medication, Incentives, and Event Rate on Reaction Time Variability in Children With ADHD. Neuropsychopharmacology 36: 1060–1072. pmid:21248722
  48. 48. Johnson KA, Barry E, Bellgrove MA, Cox M, Kelly SP, Dáibhis A, et al. (2008) Dissociation in response to methylphenidate on response variability in a group of medication naïve children with ADHD. Neuropsychologia 46: 1532–1541. pmid:18289615
  49. 49. Nandam LS, Hester R, Wagner J, Cummins TDR, Garner K, Dean AJ, et al. (2011) Methylphenidate but not atomoxetine or citalopram modulates inhibitory control and response time variability. Biol Psychiatry 69: 902–904. pmid:21193172
  50. 50. Arnsten AFT (2011) Catecholamine influences on dorsolateral prefrontal cortical networks. Biol Psychiatry 69: e89–e99. pmid:21489408
  51. 51. Zuo X-N, Di Martino A, Kelly C, Shehzad ZE, Gee DG, Klein DF, et al. (2010) The oscillating brain: complex and reliable. Neuroimage 49: 1432–1445. pmid:19782143
  52. 52. Hutchison WD, Dostrovsky JO, Walters JR, Courtemanche R, Boraud T, Goldberg J, et al. (2004) Neuronal oscillations in the basal ganglia and movement disorders: evidence from whole animal and human recordings. J Neurosci 24: 9240–9243. pmid:15496658
  53. 53. Han Y, Wang J, Zhao Z, Min B, Lu J, Li K, et al. (2011) Frequency-dependent changes in the amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: A resting-state fMRI study. NeuroImage 55: 287–295. pmid:21118724