Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Grey Matter Correlates of Three Language Tests in Non-demented Older Adults

  • Haobo Zhang,

    Affiliation Brain and Ageing Research Program, School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia

  • Perminder S. Sachdev,

    Affiliations Brain and Ageing Research Program, School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia, Neuropsychiatric Institute, Prince of Wales Hospital, Randwick, New South Wales, Australia

  • Wei Wen,

    Affiliations Brain and Ageing Research Program, School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia, Neuropsychiatric Institute, Prince of Wales Hospital, Randwick, New South Wales, Australia

  • Nicole A. Kochan,

    Affiliations Brain and Ageing Research Program, School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia, Neuropsychiatric Institute, Prince of Wales Hospital, Randwick, New South Wales, Australia

  • John D. Crawford,

    Affiliation Brain and Ageing Research Program, School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia

  • Henry Brodaty,

    Affiliations Academic Department for Old Age Psychiatry, Prince of Wales Hospital, Randwick, New South Wales, Australia, Dementia Collaborative Research Centre, School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia

  • Melissa J. Slavin,

    Affiliation Dementia Collaborative Research Centre, School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia

  • Simone Reppermund,

    Affiliation Brain and Ageing Research Program, School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia

  • Kristan Kang,

    Affiliation Brain and Ageing Research Program, School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia

  • Julian N. Trollor

    J.Trollor@unsw.edu.au

    Affiliations Brain and Ageing Research Program, School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia, Department of Developmental Disability Neuropsychiatry, School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia

Correction

17 Jan 2014: Zhang H, Sachdev PS, Wen W, Kochan NA, Crawford JD, et al. (2014) Correction: Grey Matter Correlates of Three Language Tests in Non-demented Older Adults. PLOS ONE 9(1): 10.1371/annotation/2e4d150f-c396-4867-b170-e43ccff9fcd7. https://doi.org/10.1371/annotation/2e4d150f-c396-4867-b170-e43ccff9fcd7 View correction

Abstract

Language has been extensively investigated by functional neuroimaging studies. However, only a limited number of structural neuroimaging studies have examined the relationship between language performance and brain structure in healthy adults, and the number is even less in older adults. The present study sought to investigate correlations between grey matter volumes and three standardized language tests in late life. The participants were 344 non-demented, community-dwelling adults aged 70-90 years, who were drawn from the population-based Sydney Memory and Ageing Study. The three language tests included the Controlled Oral Word Association Task (COWAT), Category Fluency (CF), and Boston Naming Test (BNT). Correlation analyses between voxel-wise GM volumes and language tests showed distinctive GM correlation patterns for each language test. The GM correlates were located in the right frontal and left temporal lobes for COWAT, in the left frontal and temporal lobes for CF, and in bilateral temporal lobes for BNT. Our findings largely corresponded to the neural substrates of language tasks revealed in fMRI studies, and we also observed a less hemispheric asymmetry in the GM correlates of the language tests. Furthermore, we divided the participants into two age groups (70-79 and 80-90 years old), and then examined the correlations between structural laterality indices and language performance for each group. A trend toward significant difference in the correlations was found between the two age groups, with stronger correlations in the group of 70-79 years old than those in the group of 80-90 years old. This difference might suggest a further decline of language lateralization in different stages of late life.

Introduction

The neural basis of language has been extensively studied, showing that distinct brain regions play different roles in language processing [1-7]. The temporal lobe functions as a core storage site for phonemic and semantic information [2,5,8,9]; the frontal lobe is involved in executive control and articulatory planning [10-13]; and the parietal lobe is associated with the integration of information regarding various attributes of objects [14,15]. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has been widely used in language studies, as it can directly measure neural activity during language tasks. However, the adaptation of standardized tests to fMRI designs has been constrained. Standardized language tests, specifically verbal fluency and naming tests, have been commonly used by clinical psychologists to assess language function of participants [16]. Both verbal fluency and naming tests require overt speech production that can cause head movements and increase image artefacts, making them unsuitable for fMRI studies [17]. Different from fMRI studies, structural MRI studies investigate the relationships between brain structural measures and language performance, which can provide insights regarding the neuroanatomical basis of standardized language tests [18].

In contrast to abundant functional neuroimaging studies on language, only a small number of structural neuroimaging studies have examined the correlations between grey matter (GM) measures and language performance in healthy adults [18-20]. In participants with a wide age range across young and old adulthoods, language tests have been found to be positively correlated with regional GM measurements. Previous studies have shown that positive GM correlates of phonemic fluency tests were located in the left frontal and temporal lobes [19,20]; and the positive GM correlates of semantic fluency tests were located in the left temporal lobe [19]. The results from structural imaging studies are concordant with the findings of functional neuroimaging studies, as the locations where the GM volumes are positively correlated with language performance are consistent with the brain regions that show functional activations during comparable language tasks.

As ageing is accompanied with brain atrophy and cognitive decline, the structure-language relationship in old adulthood might be different from the relationship in young adulthood. However, only a few studies have examined the correlations between GM measures and language tests in older adults, and their results are rather diverse. One study reported that naming tests were positively correlated with GM volumes in the left temporal lobe [21]. In another two studies, however, no positive correlation was found between GM volumes and verbal fluency and naming tests [22,23]. The inconsistent results may be due to different sample compositions across studies. Several studies have shown that language function remains stable until late old age [24-26]. One recent study demonstrated a late change in the structural cortical network associated with language function [27]. In people of early old age, individual variations on language performance and relevant brain structures may be limited, which reduce the likelihood of a relationship between the two variables [22,23]. The small sample size of studies may also contribute to the inconsistency of results [21,23]. Thus, a further study in a large sample of elderly adults aged 70 and beyond is warranted to better understand the structure-language relationship in late life.

Language has an interesting characteristic as its neural basis often presents an asymmetric pattern, showing a left-hemispheric dominance in the frontal and temporal lobes, termed as language lateralization [28-30]. Most studies on language lateralization employ fMRI to directly compare bilateral neural activities related to a language task. However, structural MRI studies also provide evidence to support language lateralization. Structural-functional correlation analysis has shown that the GM volumes that are positively correlated with verbal fluency tests are mainly located in the left frontal and temporal lobes [19,20]. Moreover, the variation in language lateralization is found to be associated with the degree of structural laterality [31-33]. Evidence also suggests that higher degree of leftward asymmetry in language-related regions is correlated with better language function [34-36]. In older adults, however, the neural basis of language displays a more symmetric pattern, located in the bilateral frontal and temporal lobes [37-39]. Based on substantial evidence provided by fMRI studies, a theory was proposed (the HAROLD model) [40], suggesting that the reduction of hemispheric asymmetry in older adults may reflect the plasticity of the ageing brain to engage extra neural circuits to compensate age-related neural inefficiency [40-42]. A recent fMRI study examined language lateralization in different age groups of healthy adults ranging from 5 to 67 years old, and the results showed a trend of decreasing language lateralization from 25 years old onwards [43]. However, few studies have investigated how language lateralization might change in different stages of late life.

The present study employed three standardized language tests to evaluate the performance of older adults in verbal fluency and naming ability. The performance on language tests was correlated with GM volumes at the voxel-level across the whole brain, which precludes a priori hypotheses for particular brain regions. Our participants were epidemiologically recruited from community-dwelling non-demented adults. With a big sample size (n=344) and an old age range for the participants (70-90 years), this study allows greater inter-individual variations on language performance and GM volumes, which may increase the likelihood of a relationship between the two variables. We hypothesized that some regional GM correlates of language tests might be located in the right hemisphere, consistent with the reduction of hemispheric asymmetry in old age. Moreover, we divided the whole sample into two age groups (70-79 and 80-90 years old), calculated the correlations between structural laterality indices and language tests for each group, and then compared the correlations between the two age groups. We aimed to explore whether the relationship between structural laterality and language function differs in the two age groups, which may indicate a change of language lateralization in different stages of late life.

Methods

Subjects

The whole sample (n=344) was drawn from Wave 1 of the Sydney Memory and Ageing Study (MAS). The MAS participants (n=1037) were randomly recruited from community-dwelling adults aged 70-90 years [44], with the following exclusion rules: dementia based on DSM-IV criteria [45]; adjusted Mini-Mental State Examination score (MMSE) <24 [46,47]; developmental disability; history of psychosis; multiple sclerosis; motor neuron disease; progressive malignancy; or inadequate English to complete basic assessment.

For the purposes of this study, additional exclusion criteria were applied to the eligible MAS participants: no MRI scan data (n=495); diagnosed with stroke (n=12), Parkinson's disease (n=8), epilepsy (n=4), severe head injury (unconsciousness > 24 hrs, n=2), brain cancer (n=1), benign meningioma (n=2), brain infection (n=6), transient global amnesia (n=3), or depression (n=60); non-English speaking background (n=78); incomplete data on language tests (n=10); extreme outliers of the language tests scores (> 3 interquartile range below/above 1st/ 3rd quartile) (n=1); or poor MRI scan quality (including MR artifacts, or errors in data saving or converting) (n=11). Of all participants (n=340), 93.3% are right-handers (n=321), 3.2% are left-handers (n=11), and 3.5% are ambidextrous individuals (n=12).

Ethics Statement

The study was approved by the ethics committee of the University of New South Wales and written informed consent was obtained from each participant.

Neuropsychological Tests

In the present study, three standardized language tests were administered to all participants by trained psychology graduates. The three language tests were part of a comprehensive battery of neuropsychological tests applied in the Sydney Memory and Ageing study to assess cognitive function of the participants. The Controlled Oral Word Association Task (COWAT) was conducted by asking participants to verbally generate as many words as possible within 60 seconds, beginning with an assigned letter, in this case the letters F, A, and S [48]. The Category Fluency test (CF) required participants to verbally generate as many words from a particular category as possible within 60 seconds, in this study ‘animals’ [49]. The 30 item Boston Naming Test (BNT) consists of 30 picture plates with drawn objects, and required participants to verbally name them [50].

MRI Acquisition

Structural MRI scans of 184 participants were acquired using a Philips 3T Intera Quasar scanner (Philips Medical Systems, Best, The Netherlands). The remaining 160 participants were scanned on a Philips 3T Achieva Quasar Dual scanner which replaced the original one in 2007 for reasons outside of the investigators’ control. Acquisition parameters for all T1-weighted structural MRI scans were: TR=6.39 ms, TE=2.9 ms, flip angle=8°, matrix size=256x256, FOV=256x256x190, and slice thickness=1 mm with no gap between; yielding 1x1x1 mm3 isotropic voxels. No significant differences on GM, WM, and cerebrospinal fluid volumes were found between the two scanner groups. Moreover, no significant difference in the distribution of two age groups (70-79 and 80-90 years old) was found between the two scanner groups (p=0.24). Nevertheless, a binary variable accounting for each of the scanners was included in the statistical analysis as a covariate to minimize potential scanner effects.

Image Processing

The procedure for processing T1-weighted MRI scans using the approach of voxel-based morphometry (VBM) had been described previously [51]. In brief, after visual inspection by experienced radiologists the brain scans with structural abnormalities such as brain tumour or severe image artifacts were removed from the study. Secondly we used the hidden Markov random field option in the unified segmentation of the Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM5, Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) to segment T1 images into different tissues with the most commonly used ICBM152 atlas as the template. Next, the toolbox of Diffeomorphic Anatomical Registration Through and Exponentiated Lie Algebra (DARTEL) [52] in SPM5 was used to generate a series of customized templates and flow fields of GM and white matter (WM) from all T1 images. Each T1 image was then registered to the customized templates to create the modulated warped tissue class image. Then, spatial normalization of GM to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space was achieved by using an affine transformation to the ICBM152 template. Lastly, the 12-mm full width at half maximum Gaussian kernel smoothing was performed to generate the voxel-based GM volumes for each subject for the subsequent statistical analysis.

Statistical Analysis

Correlation analyses between voxel-wise GM volumes and language performance were performed in the whole sample (n=344). Using the SPM5 package, the GM volume for each voxel was regressed on the raw score of each test after controlling for age, sex, years of education, total intracranial volume (TIV), scanner, cardiovascular risk score (CVR), and handedness. The calculation of CVR was performed by the MAS research group based on a regression model developed by the researchers of The Framingham Stroke Study [53]. A non-stationary correction toolbox was utilized to overcome the non-stationarity problem in VBM [54,55]. Anatomical locations of the peak voxels were labelled using the SPM Anatomy Toolbox version 1.7 (http://www.fz-juelich.de/inm/inm-1/spm_anatomy_toolbox) [56], and xjView 8 (http://www.alivelearn.net/xjview/). For the voxel-wise GM correlation analyses, the significance threshold in the whole sample was set at a voxel-level inference of p<0.001 (uncorrected) combined with a cluster-level inference of p<0.05 (FWE-corrected).

A conjunction analysis was performed to locate the common brain areas where GM volumes were positively correlated with all three tests or any two tests in the whole sample, based on the conjunction null method [57]. The positive GM correlates of the three language tests that survived the significance threshold were overlapped with each other, and the common areas were extracted for illustration.

Furthermore, we divided the whole sample into two age groups (aged 70-79 years, n=205; aged 80-90 years, n=139). Based on the results from prior analysis in the whole sample, brain regions where GM volumes were positively correlated with a language test were determined as region-of-interests (ROIs) for that test, and the boundaries of bilateral ROIs were defined using the Automated Anatomical Labelling atlas (AAL) [58]. The structural laterality index (sLI) for each ROI was computed individually with the formula used by previous studies, sLI = (Vleft - Vright)/(Vleft + Vright) [33]. The values of Vleft and Vright were calculated as the sum of voxel-wise GM volumes within bilateral ROIs. Then the correlation coefficient (r) between sLI and language test was calculated for each age group, after controlling for age, years of education, sex, TIV, scanner, CVR, and handedness (IBM SPSS 20.0, New York). Lastly, to compare the correlation coefficients between two age groups, a Fisher’s z-transformation of the r values was performed and the level of significance was determined [59].

Results

The demographic characteristics of age, sex and years of education, and the raw scores for each of the three language tests in all participants as well as each age group are shown in Table 1. Correlations between voxel-wise GM volumes and three language test were all positive as no negative correlations were found. The cluster of voxels, where GM volumes were significantly positively correlated with language performance in the whole sample, were superimposed on the sagittal slices of the standard brain template (provided by the MRIcro package http://www.mricro.com), as illustrated in Figure 1-3 for each language test separately. Within the suprathreshold clusters, the neuroanatomical locations of peak voxels were summarized in Table 2 for each language test individually.

% or Mean (SD)Total (n=344)   70-79 years (n=205)   80-90 years (n=139)   p-value
Sex (% male)45.647.343.20.45
Age78.3 (4.8)75.0 (2.4)83.2 (2.7)<0.001
Education (year)11.8 (3.6)11.9 (3.6)11.5 (3.7)0.77
COWAT37.8 (12.3)39.0 (12.2)36.1 (12.4)0.06
CF16.0 (4.4)16.7 (4.5)14.8 (3.8)<0.001
BNT24.9 (3.4)25.3 (3.2)24.4 (3.6)0.02

Table 1. Demographic characteristics and neuropsychological performance.

COWAT = Controlled Oral Word Association Task; CF = Category Fluency; BNT= Boston Naming Test.
Ratio of males was compared between the two age groups using the chi-square test. Age, years of education, and raw score on each of the three language tests were compared between two age groups using a univariate general linear model.
CSV
Download CSV
thumbnail
Figure 1. Grey matter correlates of COWAT.

Brain regions where voxel-based GM volumes are positively correlated with COWAT in 344 participants aged 70-90 years, are superimposed on the sagittal slices of the brain template. The slices are at 5 mm intervals between and including -80 mm and 75 mm. The colour bar represents the t score ranging from 0 to 5.5; and yellow indicates a higher t score than red.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080215.g001

thumbnail
Figure 2. Grey matter correlates of CF.

Brain regions where voxel-based GM volumes are positively correlated with CF in 344 participants aged 70-90 years, superimposed on the sagittal slices of the brain template. The slices are at 4 mm intervals between and including -48 mm and -20 mm. The colour bar represents the t score ranging from 0 to 5.5; and yellow indicates a higher t score than red.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080215.g002

thumbnail
Figure 3. Grey matter correlates of BNT.

Brain regions where voxel-based GM volumes are positively correlated with BNT in 344 participants aged 70-90 years, superimposed on the sagittal slices of the brain template. The slices are at 4 mm intervals between and including -48 mm and 44 mm. The colour bar represents the t score ranging from 0 to 5.5; and yellow indicates a higher t score than red.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080215.g003

TestCluster-levelVoxel-level
p (FWE)   size (n)MNI coordinates   T value   Anatomical location (BA)
XYZ
COWAT0.002466-52-38-44.47L posterior middle temporal gyrus (21)
0.008221-20-40-384.46L cerebellum
0.03527560-2204.07R precentral gyrus (6)
5614243.88R inferior frontal gyrus (44)
0.00825730-22-103.28R hippocampus
32-20-123.27R substantia nigra
0.02717720-42-384.27R cerebellum
CF<0.0013357-32-26-105.14L hippocampus
-5018-124.38L temporal pole (38)
-42023.67L insula
-3824-203.60L orbitofrontal gyrus (47)
-42683.83L inferior frontal gyrus (44)
<0.001847-28-62-383.95L cerebellum
BNT0.0011198-244-264.14L parahippocampal gyrus (28)
-346-263.59L temporal pole (38)
-26-10-243.56L hippocampus
0.002998368-284.38R temporal pole (38)
34-20-204.03R parahippocampal gyrus (28)
38-16-263.27R fusiform gyrus (20)

Table 2. Anatomical region and coordinates of peak voxels within the suprathreshold clusters correlated with three language tests in 344 participants aged 70-90 years.

The voxel-wise GM volumes were regressed on the three language test scores after controlling for age, years of education, sex, scanner, total intracranial volume (TIV), cardiovascular risk score, and handedness in the whole sample. The significance level was set at a voxel-level p<0.001 (uncorrected) combined with cluster-level p<0.05 (FWE-corrected) for the whole sample.
CSV
Download CSV

The cluster of voxels where GM volumes were positively correlated with COWAT were located in the left posterior middle temporal gyrus, right precentral and inferior frontal gyri, right hippocampus, right substantia nigra, and bilateral cerebellum (Figure 1). The positive GM correlates of CF were only located in the left hemisphere, including the hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, temporal pole, orbitofrontal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, insula, and cerebellum (Figure 2). The positive GM correlates of BNT were located in largely symmetrical positions of bilateral hemispheres, including the bilateral hippocampi, parahippocampal gyri and temporal poles, as well as the right fusiform gyrus (Figure 3).

The conjunction analysis revealed no common GM correlates to all three language tests. However, there were common GM correlates (893 voxels) to CF and BNT in the left hippocampus and left parahippocampal gyrus (Figure S1A). The common GM correlates to COWAT and CF (34 voxels) were located in the left cerebellum (Figure S1B), and the common GM correlates to COWAT and BNT (8 voxels) were located in the right hippocampus (Figure S1C).

The correlations between structural laterality indices of ROIs and language tests in two age groups were shown in Table 3. We found a significantly positive correlation between CF and sLI of the inferior frontal gyrus (r=0.142, p=0.047) and an almost significant correlation between COWAT and sLI of the precentral gyrus (r=0.126, p=0.079) in the group of 70-79 years old, but both were not significant in the 80-90 year old group. Furthermore, Fisher's z test revealed a trend toward significant difference between the two age groups in the correlation between COWAT and sLI of the precentral gyrus (z=1.83, p=0.067), and the correlation between CF and sLI of the inferior frontal gyrus (z=1.65, p=0.099). After adjusted by Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, however, the differences between two age groups in the correlations between structural laterality indices of ROIs and language tests were not significant. The boundaries of bilateral ROIs were illustrated in Figure S2. Descriptive statistics of sLI for two age groups were presented in Table S1.

YoungOldDifference
rp-valuerp-valuezp-value
COWATPRE0.1260.079-0.0780.3791.830.067
IFG0.1020.1540.0940.2890.070.94
MTG0.0250.7320.0300.738-0.040.968
CFIFG0.1420.047*-0.0420.6391.650.099
TP0.1140.1110.0190.8320.850.395
HIPP0.0270.7070.1640.064-1.230.219
BNTTP0.0800.262-0.0250.7770.940.347
HIPP-0.0150.829-0.0120.896-0.030.976
FG-0.0210.772-0.0130.883-0.070.944

Table 3. Correlations between structural laterality indices of ROIs and language tests in two age groups.

The correlation coefficient (r) between structural laterality index of each ROI and each language test was calculated for each age group, using partial correlation model and controlling for age, years of education, sex, TIV, scanner, CVR, and handedness. Then the correlation coefficients were compared between the two age groups, using a Fisher’s z-transformation of the r values and the level of significance was determined.
Abbreviation for ROIs: PRE = precentral gyrus; IFG = inferior frontal gyrus (including opercular part and triangular part); MTG = middle temporal gyrus; TP = superior temporal pole; HIPP = combined hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus; FG = fusiform gyrus.
*p<0.05
CSV
Download CSV

In addition, we divided the whole sample into two groups according to the two scanners, and then performed correlation analysis between voxel-wise GM volumes and language tests for each scanner group. We found similar patterns in the GM correlates of language tests between the two scanner groups (see Figure S3). Furthermore, we performed correlation analyses between structural laterality indices and language tests for each scanner group. The correlation coefficients were not significantly different between the two groups.

Discussion

Our study demonstrated that three language tests were all positively correlated with GM volumes in the frontal, temporal and parietal lobes in the non-demented elderly adults. Our results also displayed distinct GM correlation patterns for each language test. Although all three tests involve word retrieval and articulation, they differ in the strategies applied in word searching, selection and inhibition processes. Both verbal fluency tests, COWAT and CF, measure the efficiency of word generation, but differ in how the output is induced by either phonemic or semantic cues. The confrontational naming test, BNT, does not require automatic word generation, but evaluates the naming ability induced by visual stimulus. The GM correlation patterns of three language tests were discussed individually, and the common and specific features were identified.

Grey Matter Correlates of COWAT

As a classical phonemic fluency test, GM correlates of COWAT were mainly located in the frontal, temporal and subcortical areas in the non-demented adults aged 70-90 years. The relationship between COWAT and the frontal GM volumes, in particular the precentral and inferior frontal gyri, has been consistently demonstrated in functional neuroimaging studies, which suggest the frontal regions are involved in executive control and search strategies [60-62]. Our results were also consistent with previous structural MRI studies, which showed the patients with frontal lobe lesions performed poorly on phonemic fluency tests [63], Moreover, we found that COWAT was positively correlated with the left posterior middle temporal GM volumes. Prior studies have shown that this brain area, along with the inferior frontal gyrus, is involved in executive control of demanding language tasks [13,64-66]. A positive correlation between the substantia nigra and COWAT was also observed in this study, consistent with the involvement of this region in planning and execution of articulatory movement that is heavily engaged in this phonemic fluency test [67,68]. We noted that the frontal GM correlates of COWAT was only located in the right hemisphere, instead of a left-hemispheric dominance that is often found in healthy younger adults [28]. This observation might reflect the reduction of hemispheric asymmetry in late life, consistent with the findings of prior fMRI studies on the neural substrates of language tasks in older adults [69].

Grey Matter Correlates of CF

Our results showed that this semantic fluency test was correlated with GM volumes in the left hemisphere, including the frontal and temporal lobes, as well as the hippocampus, insula and cerebellum. Similar to COWAT, CF had the GM correlates in the frontal lobe, which may also indicate the involvement of this region in executive control and search strategies that are essential to both verbal fluency tests [60-62]. However, the extent and exact locations of the frontal GM correlates of two verbal fluency tests were different, with a bigger cluster of voxels in the precentral and inferior frontal gyrus for COWAT, and a smaller cluster of voxels in the orbitofrontal and inferior frontal gyrus for CF. This difference is coherent with previous studies, which suggests that phonemic fluency tasks have greater executive demands and require more frontal activation than semantic fluency tasks, and the frontal neural basis is located in more dorsal positions than that of semantic fluency tasks [70-72]. Our study also showed that the positive GM correlates of CF were located in the temporal and hippocampal regions. Although the involvement of the hippocampus and its neighbouring areas in episodic memory has been well-established [73-75], their roles in semantic processes is still a topic of debate [76-79]. In a recent fMRI study Ryan and colleagues found the hippocampal activation during performing both semantic and episodic retrieval tasks in healthy adults [80]. Previous studies also showed that the patients with the temporal atrophy such as Alzheimer’s disease often had semantic impairments [81,82]. Our findings provide further support for the roles of the hippocampus and its adjacent brain regions in the retrieval of semantic knowledge. This study also showed that the insula was correlated with CF. A previous study revealed that the left insula atrophy was correlated with verbal generation difficulty in healthy participants across 19-88 years [83]. Our findings are consistent with the role of the insula in articulation planning [84-86].

Grey Matter Correlates of BNT

The present study showed a symmetrical pattern in the GM correlates of BNT, which were located in the bilateral hippocampi, parahippocampal gyri and temporal poles. In contrast to the two verbal fluency tests, BNT had no significant GM correlates in the frontal lobe. Evidence from lesion studies revealed that patients suffering from frontal lobe damage showed no deficit in naming ability, but severe impairment in verbal fluency [87,88]. The evidence may suggest that the naming test has a relatively small demand on executive function and attention, different from verbal fluency tests [89]. As shown in the conjunction analysis, BNT and CF were both correlated with GM volumes in the left hippocampus and left parahippocampal gyrus, consistent with the involvement of these regions in semantic retrieval [80], an essential cognitive component for both tests [90]. The temporal pole was also found to be correlated with BNT in this study, which is possibly concordant with the role of this region as a hub area to converge different sources of information about an object, in order to assist semantic retrieval for the naming task [91-93]. The correlation of BNT with the fusiform gyrus may indicate the involvement of this region in processing visual information regarding an object, as revealed in previous studies [28,94]. Moreover, we found that three language tests all had GM correlates in the cerebellum. Although only a few studies investigated the role of cerebellum in language, they have shown that the cerebellum has a contribution to speech production and verbal working memory [84,95,96], which are important components for the three language tests.

Language Lateralization in Late Life

Our study showed that positive GM correlates of language tests were located in the right frontal lobe for COWAT and the bilateral temporal lobes for BNT. This finding indicated a less leftward GM correlation pattern for language tests, consistent with the reduction of hemispheric asymmetry in older adults as observed in previous fMRI studies [37-39]. The trend of changing language lateralization with age has been demonstrated by a recent fMRI study, which shows the left-hemispheric dominance increasing with age between 5-20 years, reaching a plateau at 20-25 years, and then slowly declining afterwards [43]. Different from fMRI studies that directly compare bilateral neural activities related to a language task, structural MRI studies have used structural laterality to investigate language lateralization. Evidence has shown that language lateralization is associated with structural laterality; moreover, the degree of leftward structural asymmetry is positively correlated with language performance, consistent with the notion of language lateralization [31,34-36]. Using structural asymmetry indices that were computed with regional GM volumes, we explored the relationship between structural asymmetry indices and language performance in two age groups (70-79 and 80-90 years old). We noted that the difference between two groups was approaching significance (p<0.1), with stronger correlations between leftward asymmetry of the frontal regions and two language tests (CF and COWAT) in the younger group than those in the older group. The results, together with our findings of less leftward patterns in voxel-wise GM correlations of language tests, suggest a further declining of language lateralization in different stages of late life.

Our study is subject to several limitations. Firstly, as the ageing brain often undergoes regional structural changes, such as GM atrophy and WM disruption, the reliability of brain registration and segmentation could be affected in processing brain images of the elderly adults [97]. However, we used an advanced DARTEL method, which has shown a better registration effect compared to other methods [98], to improve the accuracy of image processing. Secondly, the change of scanner in the middle of the study is a potential limitation though the two scanners were made by the same manufacturer and used the same parameter settings. The validation tests showed that the two scanner groups had similar neuroanatomical correlation patterns with language tests, and the correlations between structural laterality indices and language tests did not show any significantly difference between the two scanner groups. Moreover, the inclusion of scanner type as a covariate could minimize the likelihood of its influence on the relationship between voxel-wise GM volumes and language tests.

In conclusion, our study demonstrated distinctively positive correlation patterns between voxel-wise GM volumes and three standardized language tests (COWAT, CF and BNT) in a large sample of non-demented, community-dwelling adults aged 70-90 years. Our results showed that COWAT was mainly correlated with the right frontal and left temporal GM volumes, CF with the left frontal and left temporal GM volumes, and BNT with bilateral temporal GM volumes. The neuroanatomical locations of these GM correlates were largely consistent with the findings of fMRI studies on neural substrates of language tasks, and they also indicated a reduction of hemispheric asymmetry as shown in the pattern of GM correlates of language tests. In addition, we found a trend toward significant difference in the correlations between structural laterality index and language tests between two age groups (70-79 and 80-90 years old), with stronger correlations in the younger age group than those in the older age group. This difference may suggest a further decline of language lateralization with age in late life.

Supporting Information

Figure S1.

Common GM correlates of language tests. The conjunction analysis showed the common voxels where GM volumes are positively correlated with different language tests in 344 participants aged 70-90 years. These common GM correlates in colour red were superimposed on the sagittal slices of the brain template. A) common GM correlates to CF and BNT; B) common GM correlates to COWAT and CF; C) common GM correlates to COWAT and BNT.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080215.s001

(TIF)

Figure S2.

Bilateral ROIs for each language test. Based on the locations where voxel-wise volumes were positively correlated with three language tests in the whole sample, region-of-interests (ROIs) for each language test were determined. The boundary of each ROI was delineated using the Automated Anatomical Labelling atlas (AAL), and demonstrated by superimposing on the sagittal slices of the brain template. The slices were at 4 mm intervals between and including -80 mm and 76 mm. A) bilateral ROIs of COWAT; B) bilateral ROIs of CF; C) bilateral ROIs of BNT.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080215.s002

(TIF)

Figure S3.

Grey matter correlates of three language tests in two scanner groups. Brain regions where voxel-based GM volumes were positively correlated with three language tests in two scanner groups were superimposed on the 3D brain templates. The figures shown in the 1st column were for the group of Scanner 1, while the figures in the 2nd column were for the group of Scanner 2. The figures for each language test were demonstrated in three rows. A) Grey matter correlates of COWAT; B) Grey matter correlates of CF; C) Grey matter correlates of BNT.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080215.s003

(TIF)

Table S1.

Descriptive statistics of structural laterality indices (sLI) of region-of-interests (ROIs) in two age groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080215.s004

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to all participants in MAS and the MAS research team. The authors also thank Ms. Kate Crosbie for proofreading the manuscript and Ms. Janelle Govett for providing language assistance.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: HZ JT PS WW. Performed the experiments: HZ. Analyzed the data: HZ. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: HZ WW NK JC KK. Wrote the manuscript: HZ JT PS WW NK JC HB MS SR KK.

References

  1. 1. Cabeza R, Nyberg L (2000) Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. J Cogn Neurosci 12: 1-47. doi:https://doi.org/10.1162/08989290051137585.
  2. 2. Martin A, Chao LL (2001) Semantic memory and the brain: structure and processes. Curr Opin Neurobiol 11: 194-201. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00196-3. PubMed: 11301239.
  3. 3. Bookheimer S (2002) Functional MRI of language: new approaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing. Annu Rev Neurosci 25: 151-188. doi:https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142946. PubMed: 12052907.
  4. 4. Thompson-Schill SL (2003) Neuroimaging studies of semantic memory: inferring "how" from "where". Neuropsychologia 41: 280-292. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00161-6. PubMed: 12457754.
  5. 5. Damasio H, Tranel D, Grabowski T, Adolphs R, Damasio A (2004) Neural systems behind word and concept retrieval. Cognition 92: 179-229. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2002.07.001. PubMed: 15037130.
  6. 6. Vigneau M, Beaucousin V, Hervé PY, Duffau H, Crivello F et al. (2006) Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: phonology, semantics, and sentence processing. NeuroImage 30: 1414-1432. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.002. PubMed: 16413796.
  7. 7. Gerlach C (2007) A review of functional imaging studies on category specificity. J Cogn Neurosci 19: 296-314. doi:https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.2.296. PubMed: 17280518.
  8. 8. Chao LL, Haxby JV, Martin A (1999) Attribute-based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects. Nat Neurosci 2: 913-919. doi:https://doi.org/10.1038/13217. PubMed: 10491613.
  9. 9. Noppeney U, Price CJ (2004) Retrieval of abstract semantics. NeuroImage 22: 164-170. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.010. PubMed: 15110006.
  10. 10. Chee MW, Hon NH, Caplan D, Lee HL, Goh J (2002) Frequency of concrete words modulates prefrontal activation during semantic judgments. NeuroImage 16: 259-268. doi:https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1061. PubMed: 11969333.
  11. 11. Nyberg L, Marklund P, Persson J, Cabeza R, Forkstam C et al. (2003) Common prefrontal activations during working memory, episodic memory, and semantic memory. Neuropsychologia 41: 371-377. PubMed: 12457761.
  12. 12. Zheng ZZ, Munhall KG, Johnsrude IS (2010) Functional overlap between regions involved in speech perception and in monitoring one's own voice during speech production. J Cogn Neurosci 22: 1770-1781. PubMed: 19642886.
  13. 13. Whitney C, Kirk M, O'Sullivan J, Lambon Ralph MA, Jefferies E (2011) The neural organization of semantic control: TMS evidence for a distributed network in left inferior frontal and posterior middle temporal gyrus. Cereb Cortex 21: 1066-1075. PubMed: 20851853.
  14. 14. Indefrey P (2011) The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components: a critical update. Front Psychol 2: 255. PubMed: 22016740.
  15. 15. Sharp DJ, Awad M, Warren JE, Wise RJ, Vigliocco G et al. (2010) The neural response to changing semantic and perceptual complexity during language processing. Hum Brain Mapp 31: 365-377. PubMed: 19777554.
  16. 16. Lezak MD (2004) Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
  17. 17. Price CJ (2010) The anatomy of language: a review of 100 fMRI studies published in 2009. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1191: 62-88. PubMed: 20392276.
  18. 18. Richardson FM, Price CJ (2009) Structural MRI studies of language function in the undamaged brain. Brain Struct Funct 213: 511-523. PubMed: 19618210.
  19. 19. Grogan A, Green DW, Ali N, Crinion JT, Price CJ (2009) Structural correlates of semantic and phonemic fluency ability in first and second languages. Cereb Cortex 19: 2690-2698. doi:https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp023. PubMed: 19293396.
  20. 20. Newman LM, Trivedi MA, Bendlin BB, Ries ML, Johnson SC (2007) The Relationship Between Gray Matter Morphometry and Neuropsychological Performance in a Large Sample of Cognitively Healthy Adults. Brain Imaging Behavior 1: 3-10. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-007-9000-5. PubMed: 18836573.
  21. 21. Cardenas VA, Chao LL, Studholme C, Yaffe K, Miller BL et al. (2011) Brain atrophy associated with baseline and longitudinal measures of cognition. Neurobiol Aging 32: 572-580. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.04.011. PubMed: 19446370.
  22. 22. Chee MWL, Chen KHM, Zheng H, Chan KPL, Isaac V et al. (2009) Cognitive function and brain structure correlations in healthy elderly East Asians. Neuroimage 46: 257-269. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.036. PubMed: 19457386.
  23. 23. Ziegler DA, Piguet O, Salat DH, Prince K, Connally E et al. (2010) Cognition in healthy aging is related to regional white matter integrity, but not cortical thickness. Neurobiol Aging 31: 1912-1926. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.10.015. PubMed: 19091444.
  24. 24. Nicholas M, Obler L, Albert M, Goodglass H (1985) Lexical retrieval in healthy aging. Cortex 21: 595-606. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(58)80007-6. PubMed: 4092486.
  25. 25. Park DC, Lautenschlager G, Hedden T, Davidson NS, Smith AD et al. (2002) Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychol Aging 17: 299-320. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.17.2.299. PubMed: 12061414.
  26. 26. Tombaugh TN, Kozak J, Rees L (1999) Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 14: 167-177. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0887-6177(97)00095-4. PubMed: 14590600.
  27. 27. Chen ZJ, He Y, Rosa-Neto P, Gong G, Evans AC (2011) Age-related alterations in the modular organization of structural cortical network by using cortical thickness from MRI. NeuroImage 56: 235-245. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.010. PubMed: 21238595.
  28. 28. Binder JR, Frost JA, Hammeke TA, Cox RW, Rao SM et al. (1997) Human brain language areas identified by functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 17: 353-362. PubMed: 8987760.
  29. 29. Pujol J, Deus J, Losilla JM, Capdevila A (1999) Cerebral lateralization of language in normal left-handed people studied by functional MRI. Neurology 52: 1038-1043. doi:https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.52.5.1038. PubMed: 10102425.
  30. 30. Somers M, Neggers SF, Diederen KM, Boks MP, Kahn RS et al. (2011) The Measurement of Language Lateralization with Functional Transcranial Doppler and Functional MRI: A Critical Evaluation. Front Hum Neurosci 5: 31. PubMed: 21483761.
  31. 31. Josse G, Kherif F, Flandin G, Seghier ML, Price CJ (2009) Predicting language lateralization from gray matter. J Neurosci 29: 13516-13523. doi:https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1680-09.2009. PubMed: 19864564.
  32. 32. Knaus TA, Silver AM, Kennedy M, Lindgren KA, Dominick KC et al. (2010) Language laterality in autism spectrum disorder and typical controls: a functional, volumetric, and diffusion tensor MRI study. Brain Lang 112: 113-120. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2009.11.005. PubMed: 20031197.
  33. 33. Schneider P, Sluming V, Roberts N, Scherg M, Goebel R et al. (2005) Structural and functional asymmetry of lateral Heschl's gyrus reflects pitch perception preference. Nat Neurosci 8: 1241-1247. doi:https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1530. PubMed: 16116442.
  34. 34. Foundas AL, Leonard CM, Gilmore RL, Fennell EB, Heilman KM (1996) Pars triangularis asymmetry and language dominance. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 93: 719-722. doi:https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.93.2.719. PubMed: 8570622.
  35. 35. Gauger LM, Lombardino LJ, Leonard CM (1997) Brain morphology in children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res 40: 1272-1284. PubMed: 9430748.
  36. 36. Josse G, Mazoyer B, Crivello F, Tzourio-Mazoyer N (2003) Left planum temporale: an anatomical marker of left hemispheric specialization for language comprehension. Brain Res. Cogn Brain Res 18: 1-14. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.08.007.
  37. 37. Meinzer M, Flaisch T, Seeds L, Harnish S, Antonenko D et al. (2012) Same modulation but different starting points: performance modulates age differences in inferior frontal cortex activity during word-retrieval. PLOS ONE 7: e33631. doi:https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0033631. PubMed: 22438970.
  38. 38. Meinzer M, Flaisch T, Wilser L, Eulitz C, Rockstroh B et al. (2009) Neural signatures of semantic and phonemic fluency in young and old adults. J Cogn Neurosci 21: 2007-2018. doi:https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21219. PubMed: 19296728.
  39. 39. Wierenga CE, Benjamin M, Gopinath K, Perlstein WM, Leonard CM et al. (2008) Age-related changes in word retrieval: role of bilateral frontal and subcortical networks. Neurobiol Aging 29: 436-451. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.10.024. PubMed: 17147975.
  40. 40. Cabeza R (2002) Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. Psychol Aging 17: 85-100. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.85. PubMed: 11931290.
  41. 41. Park DC, Reuter-Lorenz P (2009) The Adaptive Brain: Aging and Neurocognitive Scaffolding. Annu Rev Psychol 60: 173-196. doi:https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656. PubMed: 19035823.
  42. 42. Reuter-Lorenz PA, Jonides J, Smith EE, Hartley A, Miller A et al. (2000) Age differences in the frontal lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory revealed by PET. J Cogn Neurosci 12: 174-187. doi:https://doi.org/10.1162/089892900561814. PubMed: 10769314.
  43. 43. Szaflarski JP, Holland SK, Schmithorst VJ, Byars AW (2006) fMRI study of language lateralization in children and adults. Hum Brain Mapp 27: 202-212. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20177. PubMed: 16035047.
  44. 44. Sachdev PS, Brodaty H, Reppermund S, Kochan NA, Trollor JN et al. (2010) The Sydney Memory and Ageing Study (MAS): methodology and baseline medical and neuropsychiatric characteristics of an elderly epidemiological non-demented cohort of Australians aged 70-90 years. Int Psychogeriatr 22: 1248-1264. doi:https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610210001067. PubMed: 20637138.
  45. 45. American Psychiatric Association (1995) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition, International Version (DSM-IV). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.
  46. 46. Anderson TM, Sachdev PS, Brodaty H, Trollor JN, Andrews G (2007) Effects of sociodemographic and health variables on Mini-Mental State Exam scores in older Australians. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 15: 467-476. doi:https://doi.org/10.1097/JGP.0b013e3180547053. PubMed: 17545447.
  47. 47. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR (1975) "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12: 189-198. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. PubMed: 1202204.
  48. 48. Benton AL (1967) Problems of test construction in the field of aphasia. Cortex 3: 32-58. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(67)80005-4.
  49. 49. Spreen O, Benton AL (1969) Neurosensory Center Comprehensive Examination for Aphasia: Manual of instructions (NCCEA). Victoria, BC: University of Victoria.
  50. 50. Kaplan E (2001) The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams Wilkins.
  51. 51. Zhang H, Sachdev PS, Wen W, Kochan NA, Zhu W et al. (2011) Neuroanatomical Correlates of Cognitive Performance in Late Life. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 32: 216-226. doi:https://doi.org/10.1159/000333372. PubMed: 22104974.
  52. 52. Ashburner J (2007) A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. NeuroImage 38: 95-113. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007. PubMed: 17761438.
  53. 53. D'Agostino RB Sr., Vasan RS, Pencina MJ, Wolf PA, Cobain M et al. (2008) General cardiovascular risk profile for use in primary care: the Framingham Heart Study. Circulation 117: 743-753. doi:https://doi.org/10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.107.699579. PubMed: 18212285.
  54. 54. Hayasaka S, Phan KL, Liberzon I, Worsley KJ, Nichols TE (2004) Nonstationary cluster-size inference with random field and permutation methods. NeuroImage 22: 676-687. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.041. PubMed: 15193596.
  55. 55. Worsley KJ, Andermann M, Koulis T, MacDonald D, Evans AC (1999) Detecting changes in nonisotropic images. Hum Brain Mapp 8: 98-101. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:2/3. PubMed: 10524599.
  56. 56. Eickhoff SB, Stephan KE, Mohlberg H, Grefkes C, Fink GR et al. (2005) A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. Neuroimage 25: 1325-1335. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.034. PubMed: 15850749.
  57. 57. Nichols T, Brett M, Andersson J, Wager T, Poline JB (2005) Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic. Neuroimage 25: 653-660. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.005. PubMed: 15808966.
  58. 58. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O et al. (2002) Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15: 273-289. doi:https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0978. PubMed: 11771995.
  59. 59. Fisher RA (1921) On the `probable error' of a coefficient of correlation deduced from a small sample. Metron 1: 3-32.
  60. 60. Abrahams S, Goldstein LH, Simmons A, Brammer MJ, Williams SC et al. (2003) Functional magnetic resonance imaging of verbal fluency and confrontation naming using compressed image acquisition to permit overt responses. Hum Brain Mapp 20: 29-40. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.10126. PubMed: 12953304.
  61. 61. Hirshorn EA, Thompson-Schill SL (2006) Role of the left inferior frontal gyrus in covert word retrieval: neural correlates of switching during verbal fluency. Neuropsychologia 44: 2547-2557. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.035. PubMed: 16725162.
  62. 62. Phelps EA, Hyder F, Blamire AM, Shulman RG (1997) FMRI of the prefrontal cortex during overt verbal fluency. Neuroreport 8: 561-565. doi:https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199701200-00036. PubMed: 9080448.
  63. 63. Henry JD, Crawford JR (2004) A meta-analytic review of verbal fluency performance following focal cortical lesions. Neuropsychology 18: 284-295. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.18.2.284. PubMed: 15099151.
  64. 64. Corbett F, Jefferies E, Ehsan S, Ralph Lambon (2009) Different impairments of semantic cognition in semantic dementia and semantic aphasia: evidence from the non-verbal domain. Brain 132: 2593-2608. doi:https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awp146. PubMed: 19506072.
  65. 65. Jefferies E, Baker SS, Doran M, Ralph MA (2007) Refractory effects in stroke aphasia: a consequence of poor semantic control. Neuropsychologia 45: 1065-1079. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.09.009. PubMed: 17074373.
  66. 66. Whitney C, Jefferies E, Kircher T (2011) Heterogeneity of the left temporal lobe in semantic representation and control: priming multiple versus single meanings of ambiguous words. Cereb Cortex 21: 831-844. doi:https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq148. PubMed: 20732899.
  67. 67. Frenck-Mestre C, Anton JL, Roth M, Vaid J, Viallet F (2005) Articulation in early and late bilinguals' two languages: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroreport 16: 761-765. doi:https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200505120-00021. PubMed: 15858421.
  68. 68. Hoeppner J, Prudente-Morrissey L, Herpertz SC, Benecke R, Walter U (2009) Substantia nigra hyperechogenicity in depressive subjects relates to motor asymmetry and impaired word fluency. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 259: 92-97. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-008-0840-9. PubMed: 18806917.
  69. 69. Reuter-Lorenz PA, Cappell KA (2008) Neurocognitive aging and the compensation hypothesis. Curr Directions Psychol Sci 17: 177-182. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00570.x.
  70. 70. Gourovitch ML, Kirkby BS, Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR, Gold JM et al. (2000) A comparison of rCBF patterns during letter and semantic fluency. Neuropsychology 14: 353-360. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.14.3.353. PubMed: 10928738.
  71. 71. Birn RM, Kenworthy L, Case L, Caravella R, Jones TB et al. (2010) Neural systems supporting lexical search guided by letter and semantic category cues: a self-paced overt response fMRI study of verbal fluency. Neuroimage 49: 1099-1107. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.036. PubMed: 19632335.
  72. 72. Costafreda SG, Fu CH, Lee L, Everitt B, Brammer MJ et al. (2006) A systematic review and quantitative appraisal of fMRI studies of verbal fluency: role of the left inferior frontal gyrus. Hum Brain Mapp 27: 799-810. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20221. PubMed: 16511886.
  73. 73. Nadel L, Samsonovich A, Ryan L, Moscovitch M (2000) Multiple trace theory of human memory: computational, neuroimaging, and neuropsychological results. Hippocampus 10: 352-368. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/1098-1063(2000)10:4. PubMed: 10985275.
  74. 74. Scoville WB, Milner B (1957) Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 20: 11-21. doi:https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11. PubMed: 13406589.
  75. 75. Squire LR, Zola-Morgan S (1991) The medial temporal lobe memory system. Science 253: 1380-1386. doi:https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1896849. PubMed: 1896849.
  76. 76. Bernard FA, Bullmore ET, Graham KS, Thompson SA, Hodges JR et al. (2004) The hippocampal region is involved in successful recognition of both remote and recent famous faces. NeuroImage 22: 1704-1714. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.036. PubMed: 15275926.
  77. 77. Haist F, Bowden Gore J, Mao H (2001) Consolidation of human memory over decades revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat Neurosci 4: 1139-1145. doi:https://doi.org/10.1038/nn739. PubMed: 11600889.
  78. 78. Pihlajamäki M, Tanila H, Hänninen T, Könönen M, Laakso M et al. (2000) Verbal fluency activates the left medial temporal lobe: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Ann Neurol 47: 470-476. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/1531-8249(200004)47:4. PubMed: 10762158.
  79. 79. Schmolck H, Kensinger EA, Corkin S, Squire LR (2002) Semantic knowledge in patient H.M. and other patients with bilateral medial and lateral temporal lobe lesions. Hippocampus 12: 520-533. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.10039. PubMed: 12201637.
  80. 80. Ryan L, Cox C, Hayes SM, Nadel L (2008) Hippocampal activation during episodic and semantic memory retrieval: comparing category production and category cued recall. Neuropsychologia 46: 2109-2121. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.02.030. PubMed: 18420234.
  81. 81. McGeown WJ, Shanks MF, Forbes-McKay KE, Venneri A (2009) Patterns of brain activity during a semantic task differentiate normal aging from early Alzheimer's disease. Psychiatry Res 173: 218-227. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.10.005. PubMed: 19683419.
  82. 82. Venneri A, McGeown WJ, Hietanen HM, Guerrini C, Ellis AW et al. (2008) The anatomical bases of semantic retrieval deficits in early Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia 46: 497-510. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.08.026. PubMed: 17936858.
  83. 83. Shafto MA, Burke DM, Stamatakis EA, Tam PP, Tyler LK (2007) On the tip-of-the-tongue: neural correlates of increased word-finding failures in normal aging. J Cogn Neurosci 19: 2060-2070. doi:https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.12.2060. PubMed: 17892392.
  84. 84. Brown S, Laird AR, Pfordresher PQ, Thelen SM, Turkeltaub P et al. (2009) The somatotopy of speech: phonation and articulation in the human motor cortex. Brain Cogn 70: 31-41. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2008.12.006. PubMed: 19162389.
  85. 85. Indefrey P, Levelt WJ (2004) The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components. Cognition 92: 101-144. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2002.06.001. PubMed: 15037128.
  86. 86. Moser D, Fridriksson J, Bonilha L, Healy EW, Baylis G et al. (2009) Neural recruitment for the production of native and novel speech sounds. Neuroimage 46: 549-557. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.015. PubMed: 19385020.
  87. 87. Leskelä M, Hietanen M, Kalska H, Ylikoski R, Pohjasvaara T et al. (1999) Executive functions and speed of mental processing in elderly patients with frontal or nonfrontal ischemic stroke. Eur J Neurol 6: 653-661. doi:https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1468-1331.1999.660653.x. PubMed: 10529752.
  88. 88. Miller BL, Cummings JL, Villanueva-Meyer J, Boone K, Mehringer CM et al. (1991) Frontal lobe degeneration: clinical, neuropsychological, and SPECT characteristics. Neurology 41: 1374-1382. doi:https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.41.9.1374. PubMed: 1891084.
  89. 89. Melrose RJ, Campa OM, Harwood DG, Osato S, Mandelkern MA et al. (2009) The neural correlates of naming and fluency deficits in Alzheimer's disease: an FDG-PET study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 24: 885-893. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.2229. PubMed: 19296551.
  90. 90. Hodges JR, Salmon DP, Butters N (1992) Semantic memory impairment in Alzheimer's disease: failure of access or degraded knowledge? Neuropsychologia 30: 301-314. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(92)90104-T. PubMed: 1603295.
  91. 91. Blaizot X, Mansilla F, Insausti AM, Constans JM, Salinas-Alamán A et al. (2010) The human parahippocampal region: I. Temporal pole cytoarchitectonic and MRI correlation. Cereb Cortex 20: 2198-2212. doi:https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp289. PubMed: 20064939.
  92. 92. Patterson K, Nestor PJ, Rogers TT (2007) Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 8: 976-987. doi:https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2277. PubMed: 18026167.
  93. 93. Olson IR, Plotzker A, Ezzyat Y (2007) The Enigmatic temporal pole: a review of findings on social and emotional processing. Brain 130: 1718-1731. doi:https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm052. PubMed: 17392317.
  94. 94. Obler LK, Rykhlevskaia E, Schnyer D, Clark-Cotton MR, Spiro A 3rd et al. (2010) Bilateral brain regions associated with naming in older adults. Brain Lang 113: 113-123. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2010.03.001. PubMed: 20399492.
  95. 95. Koelsch S, Schulze K, Sammler D, Fritz T, Müller K et al. (2009) Functional architecture of verbal and tonal working memory: an FMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 30: 859-873. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20550. PubMed: 18330870.
  96. 96. Wildgruber D, Ackermann H, Grodd W (2001) Differential contributions of motor cortex, basal ganglia, and cerebellum to speech motor control: effects of syllable repetition rate evaluated by fMRI. Neuroimage 13: 101-109. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(01)91444-2. PubMed: 11133313.
  97. 97. Kennedy KM, Erickson KI, Rodrigue KM, Voss MW, Colcombe SJ et al. (2009) Age-related differences in regional brain volumes: a comparison of optimized voxel-based morphometry to manual volumetry. Neurobiol Aging 30: 1657-1676. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.12.020. PubMed: 18276037.
  98. 98. Klein A, Andersson J, Ardekani BA, Ashburner J, Avants B et al. (2009) Evaluation of 14 nonlinear deformation algorithms applied to human brain MRI registration. Neuroimage 46: 786-802. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.037. PubMed: 19195496.