Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Polymorphism in Serotonin Receptor 3B Is Associated with Pain Catastrophizing

  • Emilia Horjales-Araujo ,

    emilia.horjales@ki.au.dk

    Affiliations Danish Pain Research Center, Aarhus University Hospital, Aarhus, Denmark, Center for Functionally Integrative Neuroscience (CFIN), MindLab, Aarhus University Hospital, Aarhus, Denmark

  • Ditte Demontis,

    Affiliation Department of Biomedicine and Center for Integrative Sequencing (iSEQ), Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark

  • Ellen Kielland Lund,

    Affiliation Danish Pain Research Center, Aarhus University Hospital, Aarhus, Denmark

  • Nanna Brix Finnerup,

    Affiliation Danish Pain Research Center, Aarhus University Hospital, Aarhus, Denmark

  • Anders D. Børglum,

    Affiliation Department of Biomedicine and Center for Integrative Sequencing (iSEQ), Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark

  • Troels Staehelin Jensen,

    Affiliations Danish Pain Research Center, Aarhus University Hospital, Aarhus, Denmark, Department of Neurology, Aarhus University Hospital, Aarhus, Denmark

  • Peter Svensson,

    Affiliations Center for Functionally Integrative Neuroscience (CFIN), MindLab, Aarhus University Hospital, Aarhus, Denmark, Section of Clinical Oral Physiology, Department of Dentistry, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark

  • Lene Vase

    Affiliations Danish Pain Research Center, Aarhus University Hospital, Aarhus, Denmark, Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark

Abstract

Pain catastrophizing, a coping style characterized by excessively negative thoughts and emotions in relation to pain, is one of the psychological factors that most markedly predicts variability in the perception of pain; however, only little is known about the underlying neurobiology. The aim of this study was to test for associations between psychological variables, such as pain catastrophizing, anxiety and depression, and selected polymorphisms in genes related to monoaminergic neurotransmission, in particular serotonin pathway genes. Three hundred seventy-nine healthy participants completed a set of psychological questionnaires: the Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS), the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory and Beck’s Depression Inventory, and were genotyped for 15 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in nine genes. The SNP rs1176744 located in the serotonin receptor 3B gene (5-HTR3B) was found to be associated with pain catastrophizing scores: both the global score and the subscales of magnification and helplessness. This is the first study to show an association between 5-HTR3B and PCS scores, thus suggesting a role of the serotonin pathway in pain catastrophizing. Since 5-HTR3B has previously been associated with descending pain modulation pathways, future studies will be of great interest to elucidate the molecular pathways involved in the relation between serotonin, its receptors and pain catastrophizing.

Introduction

Psychological variables such as pain catastrophizing, anxiety and depression are known to influence the perception of pain [1][7]. Recently, both psychological traits and states and pain conditions have been correlated with neurotransmitter systems, and it has been suggested that interindividual differences in the expression of psychological factors and pain perception are based on genetic variations [8][10].

Pain catastrophizing along with anxiety and depression is one of the psychological factors that most markedly predict variability in the perception of pain as well as in the development of chronic pain conditions [11][13]. Pain catastrophizing has broadly been defined as “an exaggerated negative mental set or orientation brought to bear during actual or anticipated pain experience”, and it is related to attentional bias and hypervigilance [11], [14]. Pain catastrophizing is characterized by a tendency to magnify the threat value of a stimulus by rumination (i.e. a relative inability to inhibit pain-related thoughts) and by a tendency to feel helpless in the context of the stimulus [11], [12], [15]. Conceptually and empirically, pain catastrophizing is closely related to anxiety and depression, [12]. High levels of pain catastrophizing have been associated with increased pain intensity and increased activity in pain-related areas of the brain [16], [17], and also with increased temporal summation [18], [19] and an altered response to opioid treatment [20]. Despite the well-known clinical relevance of pain catastrophizing, the neurobiological underpinnings of the phenomenon have just begun to be specified [13]. So far, only one study has examined possible associations between genetic variation and pain catastrophizing [13].

Serotonin (5-HT) is a neurotransmitter implicated with a wide range of behaviors such as mood and nociception. Serotonergic genes have been extensively studied in relation to pain, anxiety and depression [21][25]. However, to date there are no published studies of genetic variations of serotonin-related genes in relation to pain catastrophizing.

The serotonin transporter (5-HTT) plays a critical role in determining the duration and intensity of 5-HT communication with receptors and targets, for review see [26]. The 5-HTT is coded by a single gene (SLC6A4) [27]. A 43 base-pair insertion/deletion (referred to as the short “S” and long “L” variant) in the promoter region of the gene in combination with the single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) rs2553 (A/G) has been shown to alter the degree of gene expression [28], [29]. The combination of these two polymorphisms is referred to as “tri-allelic” 5-HTTLPR and permits a functional division of individuals into “high” (LA/LA), “intermediate” (LA/LG; SA/LA) or “low” (SA/SA; LG/SA) expression of the serotonin transporter protein [28].

Expression of low levels of 5-HTT has been associated with neuroticism and anxiety traits [30][32]. It has also been suggested that individuals with low expression of the protein have a predisposition for depression [33], eating disorders [34], affective disorders [35] and obsessive-compulsive disorder [36], among others. In addition, Beevers and colleagues, by using a measure of biased attention, were the first to report that carriers of the S-allele showed a bias toward negative stimuli [37], [38]; similar results are also reported in more recent studies [39], [40]. In addition, Fox and colleagues reported strong evidence for a positive bias in the LL group such that vigilance for positive material was observed in addition to clear avoidance of negative material, a pattern that was completely absent in the S-allele carriers [41]. Altogether, these studies suggest that subjects with low 5-HTT expression are more likely to focus on negative stimuli than high expression subjects. Attentional threat bias has been implicated in the etiology and maintenance of anxiety [42][44], which strengthens the association between S-carriers and anxiety traits. Thus, it is logic to speculate that S-carriers by focusing on a negative stimulus (e.g. pain) would be more prone to rumination and magnification of such a stimulus and hence to pain catastrophizing.

In recent studies, we [45] and others [46] have found that the tri-allelic polymorphism is correlated with the ability to modulate pain. Thus, participants with the genetic variants associated with high 5-HTT expression experienced increased pain intensity during negative pictures and decreased pain intensity during positive pictures when compared with the pain experienced during neutral pictures. These results were not observed in the genotype group corresponding to the low expression of the protein, probably because of the attentional bias to the painful negative stimuli.

Serotonin neurotransmission might also be affected by polymorphisms in other genes, potentially affecting behavior and personality traits. For instance, it has been suggested that polymorphisms in the serotonin receptor 2A (5-HT2A) are associated with low anxiety traits and novelty-seeking behavior [47], [48]. Another example is the serotonin receptor 3 (5-HT3); variations in this gene have been reported to be associated with bipolar disorder [49] and harm-avoidance behavior in women [50]. Moreover, there is now substantial anatomical and functional evidence for the participation of 5-HT3 receptors in spinal nociceptive processing [51]. Since pain catastrophizing is highly associated with processing of noxious stimuli, it is of interest to see if there are associations between genetic variations in these genes and pain catastrophizing [13].

Monoamine oxidase (MAO) is flavin-containing mitochondrial enzymes catalyzing the oxidative deamination of neurotransmitters and biogenic amides in the brain and peripheral tissues [52]. Based on substrate selectivity and inhibitor selectivity, two forms of MAO have been designated: MAO-A and MAO-B [53], [54], which correspond to two distinct genes. Typically, MAO-A catalyzes the oxidation of serotonin (5-HT), whereas MAO-B acts on 2-phenylethylamine and benzylamine [52], [55]. Due to the important role of MAO in monoamine neurotransmission, two SNPs in MAO-A and two in MAO-B were included in this study.

The aim of this study was to investigate if the psychological factors that most markedly predict pain variability (pain catastrophizing, depression and anxiety) were associated with SNPs in genes related to the serotonin pathway. The SNPs included in this study have previously been reported to be associated with pain perception as well as anxiety and depression [30], [46], [56][73].

Methods

Participants

As part of a previous study [45], a DNA bank of 380 healthy individuals of Scandinavian descent between 18 and 39 years of age were recruited at Aarhus University. Individuals were excluded from the study if they had any chronic pain condition (based on the IASP definition of chronic pain), were smokers, pregnant, used medication on a regular basis (except for contraceptives), or if they had any known psychological, cardiovascular or neurological disorder.

All participants gave written informed consent upon having received detailed information on the study and received a bottle of wine in compensation for their participation. The study was conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the local ethical committee (20110165) and the Danish Data Protection Agency (2011-41-6562).

Study Design

Upon entry in the study, participants completed the following psychological questionnaires: the Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS), Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) and the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory I and II (STAI), using SurveyXact (Rambøll Management Consulting, Denmark). Afterwards the participants were asked to give a saliva sample.

DNA Analysis

DNA was extracted from saliva collected using an OC-100 kit (DNA Genotek Inc, Ontario, Canada). To determine the triallelic 5-HTTLPR genotype, PCR reactions were carried out in a total volume of 25 µl using the GoTaq® Hot Start Polymerase (Promega, Wisconsin, USA) and 80 ng of genomic template. The forward primer sequence was 5′-CTCTGAATGCCAGCACCTAACCC-3′ and the reverse 5′-GATTCTGGTGCCACCTAGACGC-3′. Samples were amplified (Gene Amp, PCR System 9700, Applied Biosystems, California, USA) by 2-step PCR consisting of an activation step of 2 min at 94°C, followed by 35 cycles of 30 s denaturation at 93°C, and an annealing and elongation step for 1 min at 62°C, followed by a final elongation step of 10 min at 72°C. The L-allele and the S-allele of the 5-HTTLPR yield a product of 529 bp and 486 bp, respectively.

Fragments were visualized with UV after 45 min of separation at 80 V on a 2.5% agarose gel. In order to ensure that the primers amplified the right DNA region, two random samples were selected, and the PCR product was purified (Jet Quick PCR product purification, Genomed, Löhne, Germany) and Sanger sequenced by Eurofins MWG Operon (Ebersberg, Germany).

To determine the rs25531 genotype, 10 µl of the PCR product was digested for 2 h at 37°C with 1 µl MSP1 (New England Biolabs, Ipswich, MA, USA) and 1 µl buffer per sample. The enzyme cuts at a 5′-C/CGC-3′sequence, resulting in fragments of different lengths, which determined the triallelic genotype (see Table 1). The digested fragments were visualized by UV light after 2 h of separation at 100 V on a 4% agarose gel.

The 15 genotyped SNPs were selected from 86 papers investigating genetic association with pain and psychological traits. The included SNPs have previously been significantly (P<0.05) associated with either pain (rs6313, rs3788862, rs2283729, rs1799836, rs2066713 and rs3813034) or personality traits that have been reported to alter pain perception (rs1364043, rs130058, rs1923886, rs6313, rs7997012, rs518147, rs1176744, rs10917509, rs1042173, rs4325622 and rs3813034), or have been reported to be closely associated with pain catastrophizing (e.g. anxiety and neuroticism) (rs1364043, rs6313, rs7997012, rs1176744, rs10917509, rs4325622 and rs3813034). The populations analyzed in the previous studies were Caucasians with the exception of two studies based on Chinese population. SNP genotyping (see Table 1) was carried out using the Sequenom MassARRAY Genotyping platform (Sequenom, San Diego, CA, USA) following the procedure described in [74]. Primer sequences and assay conditions are available from the authors upon request. Only SNPs that preserved the Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium were included in the analysis.

Pain Catastrophizing

Pain catastrophizing was assessed by means of the Danish version of the PCS, which measures thoughts and feelings when experiencing pain [14]. In agreement with the standard instruction for the PCS, participants were told “we are interested in the thoughts and feelings you have when you are in pain” [14]. The scale has 13 items, and it includes three subscales: “helplessness”, “magnification” and “rumination”. The PCS has previously been used in healthy volunteers with (e.g. [20]) or without pain (e.g. [75]) as well as in chronic pain patients (e.g. [19]). The Danish version of the PCS has previously shown a high reliability (Cronbach’s Alpha:0.956) [19].

Anxiety

Anxiety was measured with the Danish version of the STAI for adults [76]. The STAI assesses emotional, cognitive and behavioral aspects of anxiety. Form Y is the most frequently used version and has 20 items for assessing trait anxiety (STAI I) and 20 items for assessing state anxiety (STAI II). The Danish version of the STAI has previously shown a reasonable reliability (Cronbach’s Alpha: 0.803) [19].

Depression

Depression was assessed with the Danish version of the BDI (second edition) [77], which consists of 21 items assessing psychological and physiological aspects of depression. The Danish version of the BDI has previously shown a high reliability (Cronbach’s Alpha: 0.935) [19], [78].

Statistical Analysis

Tests for deviations from the Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium and allelic association analyses were obtained using the statistical software PLINK (v1.07 http://pngu.mgh.harvard.edu/purcell/plink/). Allelic association analyses were conducted using linear regression applying an additive model (for nonparametric regression method). A significance level of P<0.05 was adopted for all association analyses. Correlations between the three psychological parameters (pain catastrophizing, anxiety and depression) were tested, and they were all significantly correlated (see Table S1). Since the psychological tests are not independent, corrections for multiple comparisons were done only for the 15 SNPs and the tri-allelic polymorphism by Bonferroni test (adjusted α = 0.002).

To detect the power of the sample size to find a true association under the study constraints, the software PGA was used [79]. Power analyses were performed by using 15 independent tests, a false-positive rate of 0.05, a disease prevalence of 0.01, a control to case ratio of two and various settings for the relative risk (1.50, 1.60, 1.70), assuming a codominant model with one degree of freedom, and various settings for the disease allele frequency (0.2, 0.3, 0.4), assuming complete LD between the genotyped marker and the causative SNPs. The power to detect association with a sample size of 379 cases ranged from 29 to 69%.

Results

Three hundred and eighty participants completed the study. The extracted DNA from one male participant had poor quality and was excluded from genotyping. A total of 379 participants were genotyped. Individuals with a genotyping call rate below 0.7 were excluded (31 participants), thus 348 participants (175 males and 173 females, 24.5±4.5 years, with no age difference between genders) were included in the analysis. Quality control of the SNPs with call rate >0.9 was left for further analyses. No SNPs demonstrated significant deviation from the Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium, and the minor allele frequency (MAF) found was 14%, and all MAFs were >0.1%. Nice cluster plots were observed for all SNPs. Genotyping consistency rate of the tri-allelic polymorphism was 98%.

All associations of polymorphisms with pain catastrophizing (PCS), depression (BDI) and anxiety (STAI) scores are presented in Table 2. The significant associations preserved after Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons are described below.

thumbnail
Table 2. Minor allele association with personality trait scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078889.t002

Pain Catastrophizing

The G-allele of rs1176744 was negatively correlated with the global score of the PCS (P = 0.001) and with the subcategories of magnification (P = 0.001) and helplessness (P = 0.001) (Table 2). Also, the minor allele of rs1042173 (G-allele) was negatively related with rumination, whereas rs130058 (T-allele) was positively (P = 0.002) correlated with the rumination subscores of the PCS.

Anxiety.

The minor allele of rs1176744 in the 5-HTR3B showed a positive correlation with both state anxiety (P = 0.0002) and trait anxiety (P = 0.001) scores (Table 2).

Depression.

There were no SNPs correlated with BDI scores (Table 2).

Discussion

Pain catastrophizing, along with anxiety and depression, has been suggested as one of the strongest predictors of pain-related outcomes, showing positive associations with pain report, pain behaviors, analgesic use, length of hospital stay, length of rehabilitation and pain-related disability [1][7], [80].

The present study aimed to investigate possible associations between pain catastrophizing, anxiety and depression (psychological variables of relevance for pain perception) and SNPs, mainly in the serotonin (5-HT) pathway. To our knowledge, this is the first study to show an association of rs1176744 in the serotonin receptor 3B (5-HTR3B) with PCS scores.

The 5-HTR3 is the only serotonin receptor that is a ligand-gated ion channel. The binding of the neurotransmitter serotonin to the 5-HT3 receptor opens the channel, which in turn leads to an excitatory response in neurons. Presynaptic 5-HT3 receptors are considered to mediate or modulate neurotransmitter release (including, e.g., GABA and dopamine) [81], [82], whereas postsynaptic receptors are responsible for the fast excitatory response to 5-HT.

The Rs1176744 polymorphism results in a tyrosine/serine substitution in 5-HTR3B. On a functional level, the tyrosine-allele (Tyr-allele) results in a decreased maximum response to 5-HT due to a sevenfold decrease in single channel mean open time compared to the serine-allele (Ser-allele) [83][85]. It has been suggested that the effect of this polymorphism on the 5-HT3B receptor may have an impact on personality traits by affecting serotonin and dopamine signaling [82], [86], which has been predicted by in silico analyses to be “possibly damaging” [86]. In agreement with this finding, the Tyr-allele of rs11767 has previously been found to be overrepresented in patients with major depression [86].

We found a correlation between the T-allele of the SNP (tyrosine in the protein) and higher PCS scores, both the global score and the subscales concerning magnification and helplessness. This is of interest because there are previous reports suggesting a role of spinal 5-HT3 receptors mediating both nociception and antinociception [87][93]. Although this diversity of results may arise from differential activation of descending serotonergic systems and by the subfamilies of 5-HT3 receptors studied by individual groups, our results increase the evidence for a role of 5-HT3 in pain modulation. Further studies focusing on the relation between spinal 5-HT3 and pain catastrophizing are needed.

Previous studies have reported an association between alterations in the serotonin system and attentional threat bias [39], [40]. Since there are no studies that have analyzed the possible association between the rs1176744 polymorphism and attentional bias, a further study is required in order to analyze if the correlation between the Tyr-allele and the higher PCS scores is due to an attentional bias toward the pain experience.

Interestingly, we also found that the Tyr-allele was negatively associated with anxiety scores measured with STAI I and STAI II. Although this may sound contradictory at first, it is also in accordance with previous publications that have suggested that 5-HT enhances conditioned anxiety by acting in the forebrain [94][96], for review see [97]. Accordingly, it is likely that the Tyr-allele, by diminishing the 5-HT response, decreases the level of anxiety as reflected in lower scores on STAI I and STAI II.

MAO plays an important role in monoamine neurotransmission; however, rs1799836 and rs2283729 in MAO-B did not show any correlation with pain catastrophizing or personality trait. Although Dlugos and colleagues [98] found that intronic MAO-B SNPs (rs10521432 and rs6651806) were associated with the personality trait of negative emotionality in healthy humans, in agreement with our results, they did not find any association between emotionality and rs1799836. Several lines of evidence indicate that MAO, in particular MAO-A, plays an important role in human behavior and physiology [99]. First, low platelet MAO activity has been linked to vulnerability for depression, suicidality and substance abuse disorders [100][103]. Second, MAO inhibitors are used to treat depression [104][106]. The SNP rs2283729 was positively related with agreeableness, which reflects a tendency to be friendly and compassionate [107], [108]. This is in agreement with a previous study showing that other polymorphisms affecting MAO have been positively related with agreeableness [109].

Some methodological problems in this study need to be considered. The study did not test if pain catastrophizing per se, i.e. independently of anxiety and depression, was related to specific polymorphisms. In so far as there was a correlation between pain catastrophizing, anxiety and depression, it cannot be precluded that anxiety and depression also contributed to the findings. Also, the study only included around 350 participants, so the absence of significant correlations between some of the SNPs and psychological outcomes could be the result of the low sample size. In addition, due to the low number of participants, the effect of possible haplotypes had to be excluded from the study in order to avoid underpowered groups. It is also important to remember that participants with a diagnosis of psychological disorder (e.g. depression) were excluded from participation in the study; therefore it is possible that this bias is reflected in the absence of a significant correlation between genetic variations and psychological outcomes such as depression.

Finally, the study was carried out only using participants born in Scandinavia and with Scandinavian descent. Due to the possible differences in allele frequency in other ethnicities, correlations deviating from the ones observed here could be found in samples with different ethnicity. Thus, the results presented in this article should be replicated in larger samples including groups with different ethnic origin.

In this study, we found an association between the SNP rs1176744 in the serotonin receptor 3B (5-HTR3B) and PCS scores, suggesting a role of the serotonin pathway in pain catastrophizing. Further studies are needed to investigate the molecular process behind the interaction between serotonin and pain catastrophizing; in addition it should be examined whether the interaction of polymorphisms in the serotonin pathways enhance or reduce this association.

Supporting Information

Table S1.

Regression coefficient (slopes) of the correlation between psychological traits. *P<0.05: **P<0.005; ***P<0.001. PCS, pain catastrophizing scale; BDI, Beck’s Depression Inventory; STAI, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory I (state) and II (trait).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078889.s001

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Anne Hedemand for her help with genotyping and Helle Obenhausen Andersen for her help with the language revision of the manuscript.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: EHA DD TSJ PS LV. Performed the experiments: EHA DD EKL. Analyzed the data: EHA DD LV. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: NBF ADB TSJ PS LV. Wrote the paper: EHA NBF ADB TSJ PS LV. Discussed the results and implications and commented on the manuscript at all stages: EHA DD EKL ADB TSJ PS LV.

References

  1. 1. Villemure C, Bushnell MC (2009) Mood influences supraspinal pain processing separately from attention. J Neurosci 29: 705–715.
  2. 2. Kenntner-Mabiala R, Pauli P (2005) Affective modulation of brain potentials to painful and nonpainful stimuli. Psychophysiology 42: 559–567.
  3. 3. Rockett MP, Simpson G, Crossley R, Blowey S (2013) Characteristics of pain in hospitalized medical patients, surgical patients, and outpatients attending a pain management centre. Br J Anaesth.
  4. 4. Meagher MW, Arnau RC, Rhudy JL (2001) Pain and emotion: effects of affective picture modulation. Psychosom Med 63: 79–90.
  5. 5. Roy M, Piche M, Chen JI, Peretz I, Rainville P (2009) Cerebral and spinal modulation of pain by emotions. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106: 20900–20905.
  6. 6. van Tulder MW, Ostelo R, Vlaeyen JWS, Linton SJ, Morley SJ, et al. (2001) Behavioral treatment for chronic low back pain - A systematic review within the framework of the Cochrane Back Review Group. Spine (Phila Pa 1976) 26: 270–281.
  7. 7. Fritz JM, George SZ, Delitto A (2001) The role of fear-avoidance beliefs in acute low back pain: relationships with current and future disability and work status. Pain 94: 7–15.
  8. 8. Bouchard TJ Jr (1994) Genes, environment, and personality. Science 264: 1700–1701.
  9. 9. Bouchard TJ Jr, Loehlin JC (2001) Genes, evolution, and personality. Behav Genet 31: 243–273.
  10. 10. Chen C, Moyzis R, Dong Q, He Q, Zhu B, et al. (2011) Sex modulates the associations between the COMT gene and personality traits. Neuropsychopharmacology 36: 1593–1598.
  11. 11. Sullivan MJ, Thorn B, Haythornthwaite JA, Keefe F, Martin M, et al. (2001) Theoretical perspectives on the relation between catastrophizing and pain. Clin J Pain 17: 52–64.
  12. 12. Quartana PJ, Campbell CM, Edwards RR (2009) Pain catastrophizing: a critical review. Expert Rev Neurother 9: 745–758.
  13. 13. Campbell CM, Edwards RR (2009) Mind-body interactions in pain: the neurophysiology of anxious and catastrophic pain-related thoughts. Transl Res 153: 97–101.
  14. 14. Sullivan MJL, Bishop SR, Pivik J (1995) The Pain Catastrophizing Scale: Development and validation. Psychological Assessment 7: 524–532.
  15. 15. Rosenstiel AK, Keefe FJ (1983) The use of coping strategies in chronic low back pain patients: relationship to patient characteristics and current adjustment. Pain 17: 33–44.
  16. 16. Gracely RH, Geisser ME, Giesecke T, Grant MA, Petzke F, et al. (2004) Pain catastrophizing and neural responses to pain among persons with fibromyalgia. Brain 127: 835–843.
  17. 17. Seminowicz DA, Davis KD (2006) Cortical responses to pain in healthy individuals depends on pain catastrophizing. Pain 120: 297–306.
  18. 18. Edwards RR, Smith MT, Stonerock G, Haythornthwaite JA (2006) Pain-related catastrophizing in healthy women is associated with greater temporal summation of and reduced habituation to thermal pain. Clin J Pain 22: 730–737.
  19. 19. Vase L, Nikolajsen L, Christensen B, Egsgaard LL, Arendt-Nielsen L, et al. (2011) Cognitive-emotional sensitization contributes to wind-up-like pain in phantom limb pain patients. Pain 152: 157–162.
  20. 20. Edwards RR, Kronfli T, Haythornthwaite JA, Smith MT, McGuire L, et al. (2008) Association of catastrophizing with interleukin-6 responses to acute pain. Pain 140: 135–144.
  21. 21. Millan MJ (1994) Serotonin and pain: evidence that activation of 5-HT1A receptors does not elicit antinociception against noxious thermal, mechanical and chemical stimuli in mice. Pain 58: 45–61.
  22. 22. MIllan MJ (1995) Serotonin (5-HT) and pain: A reappraisal of its role in the light of receptor multiplicity. Seminars in Neuroscience 7: 409–419.
  23. 23. Carver CS, Miller CJ (2006) Relations of serotonin function to personality: current views and a key methodological issue. Psychiatry Res 144: 1–15.
  24. 24. Ebstein RP (2006) The molecular genetic architecture of human personality: beyond self-report questionnaires. Mol Psychiatry 11: 427–445.
  25. 25. Ebstein RP, Benjamin J, Belmaker RH (2000) Personality and polymorphisms of genes involved in aminergic neurotransmission. Eur J Pharmacol 410: 205–214.
  26. 26. Hariri AR, Holmes A (2006) Genetics of emotional regulation: the role of the serotonin transporter in neural function. Trends Cogn Sci 10: 182–191.
  27. 27. Lesch KP, Balling U, Gross J, Strauss K, Wolozin BL, et al. (1994) Organization of the human serotonin transporter gene. J Neural Transm Gen Sect 95: 157–162.
  28. 28. Wendland JR, Martin BJ, Kruse MR, Lesch KP, Murphy DL (2006) Simultaneous genotyping of four functional loci of human SLC6A4, with a reappraisal of 5-HTTLPR and rs25531. Mol Psychiatry 11: 224–226.
  29. 29. Hu X, Oroszi G, Chun J, Smith TL, Goldman D, et al. (2005) An expanded evaluation of the relationship of four alleles to the level of response to alcohol and the alcoholism risk. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 29: 8–16.
  30. 30. Greenberg BD, Li Q, Lucas FR, Hu S, Sirota LA, et al. (2000) Association between the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism and personality traits in a primarily female population sample. Am J Med Genet 96: 202–216.
  31. 31. Lesch KP, Bengel D, Heils A, Sabol SZ, Greenberg BD, et al. (1996) Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science 274: 1527–1531.
  32. 32. Mazzanti CM, Lappalainen J, Long JC, Bengel D, Naukkarinen H, et al. (1998) Role of the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism in anxiety-related traits. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55: 936–940.
  33. 33. Karg K, Burmeister M, Shedden K, Sen S (2011) The serotonin transporter promoter variant (5-HTTLPR), stress, and depression meta-analysis revisited: evidence of genetic moderation. Arch Gen Psychiatry 68: 444–454.
  34. 34. Calati R, De Ronchi D, Bellini M, Serretti A (2011) The 5-HTTLPR polymorphism and eating disorders: a meta-analysis. Int J Eat Disord 44: 191–199.
  35. 35. Collier DA, Stober G, Li T, Heils A, Catalano M, et al. (1996) A novel functional polymorphism within the promoter of the serotonin transporter gene: possible role in susceptibility to affective disorders. Mol Psychiatry 1: 453–460.
  36. 36. Bloch MH, Landeros-Weisenberger A, Sen S, Dombrowski P, Kelmendi B, et al. (2008) Association of the serotonin transporter polymorphism and obsessive-compulsive disorder: systematic review. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 147B: 850–858.
  37. 37. Beevers CG, Wells TT, Ellis AJ, McGeary JE (2009) Association of the serotonin transporter gene promoter region (5-HTTLPR) polymorphism with biased attention for emotional stimuli. J Abnorm Psychol 118: 670–681.
  38. 38. Beevers CG, Gibb BE, McGeary JE, Miller IW (2007) Serotonin transporter genetic variation and biased attention for emotional word stimuli among psychiatric inpatients. J Abnorm Psychol 116: 208–212.
  39. 39. Perez-Edgar K, Bar-Haim Y, McDermott JM, Gorodetsky E, Hodgkinson CA, et al. (2010) Variations in the serotonin-transporter gene are associated with attention bias patterns to positive and negative emotion faces. Biol Psychol 83: 269–271.
  40. 40. Pergamin-Hight L, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH, Bar-Haim Y (2012) Variations in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter gene and biased attention for emotional information: a meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry 71: 373–379.
  41. 41. Fox E, Ridgewell A, Ashwin C (2009) Looking on the bright side: biased attention and the human serotonin transporter gene. Proc Biol Sci 276: 1747–1751.
  42. 42. Bar-Haim Y, Lamy D, Pergamin L, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IMH (2007) Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Bull 133: 1–24.
  43. 43. Fox E, Cahill S, Zougkou K (2010) Preconscious processing biases predict emotional reactivity to stress. Biol Psychiatry 67: 371–377.
  44. 44. MacLeod C, Rutherford E, Campbell L, Ebsworthy G, Holker L (2002) Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. J Abnorm Psychol 111: 107–123.
  45. 45. Horjales-Araujo E, Demontis D, Lund EK, Vase L, Finnerup NB, et al.. (2013) Emotional modulation of muscle pain is associated with polymorphisms in the serotonin transporter gene. Pain.
  46. 46. Lindstedt F, Berrebi J, Greayer E, Lonsdorf TB, Schalling M, et al. (2011) Conditioned pain modulation is associated with common polymorphisms in the serotonin transporter gene. PLoS One 6: e18252.
  47. 47. Rybakowski F, Slopien A, Dmitrzak-Weglarz M, Czerski P, Rajewski A, et al. (2006) The 5-HT2A -1438 A/G and 5-HTTLPR polymorphisms and personality dimensions in adolescent anorexia nervosa: association study. Neuropsychobiology 53: 33–39.
  48. 48. Serretti A, Calati R, Giegling I, Hartmann AM, Moller HJ, et al. (2007) 5-HT2A SNPs and the Temperament and Character Inventory. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 31: 1275–1281.
  49. 49. Niesler B, Flohr T, Nothen MM, Fischer C, Rietschel M, et al. (2001) Association between the 5′ UTR variant C178T of the serotonin receptor gene HTR3A and bipolar affective disorder. Pharmacogenetics 11: 471–475.
  50. 50. Melke J, Westberg L, Nilsson S, Landen M, Soderstrom H, et al. (2003) A polymorphism in the serotonin receptor 3A (HTR3A) gene and its association with harm avoidance in women. Arch Gen Psychiatry 60: 1017–1023.
  51. 51. Green GM, Scarth J, Dickenson A (2000) An excitatory role for 5-HT in spinal inflammatory nociceptive transmission; state-dependent actions via dorsal horn 5-HT3 receptors in the anaesthetized rat. Pain 89: 81–88.
  52. 52. Shih JC, Chen K, Ridd MJ (1999) Role of MAO A and B in neurotransmitter metabolism and behavior. Polish Journal of Pharmacology 51: 25–29.
  53. 53. Johnston JP (1968) Some observations upon a new inhibitor of monoamine oxidase in brain tissue. Biochem Pharmacol 17: 1285–1297.
  54. 54. Knoll J, Magyar K (1972) Some puzzling pharmacological effects of monoamine oxidase inhibitors. Adv Biochem Psychopharmacol 5: 393–408.
  55. 55. Lenders JW, Eisenhofer G, Abeling NG, Berger W, Murphy DL, et al. (1996) Specific genetic deficiencies of the A and B isoenzymes of monoamine oxidase are characterized by distinct neurochemical and clinical phenotypes. J Clin Invest 97: 1010–1019.
  56. 56. Hammer C, Cichon S, Muhleisen TW, Haenisch B, Degenhardt F, et al. (2012) Replication of functional serotonin receptor type 3A and B variants in bipolar affective disorder: a European multicenter study. Transl Psychiatry 2: e103.
  57. 57. Villafuerte SM, Vallabhaneni K, Sliwerska E, McMahon FJ, Young EA, et al. (2009) SSRI response in depression may be influenced by SNPs in HTR1B and HTR1A. Psychiatr Genet 19: 281–291.
  58. 58. Contini V, Bertuzzi GP, Polina ER, Hunemeier T, Hendler EM, et al. (2012) A haplotype analysis is consistent with the role of functional HTR1B variants in alcohol dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend 122: 100–104.
  59. 59. McAuley EZ, Fullerton JM, Blair IP, Donald JA, Mitchell PB, et al. (2009) Association between the serotonin 2A receptor gene and bipolar affective disorder in an Australian cohort. Psychiatr Genet 19: 244–252.
  60. 60. Serretti A, Drago A, De Ronchi D (2007) HTR2A gene variants and psychiatric disorders: a review of current literature and selection of SNPs for future studies. Curr Med Chem 14: 2053–2069.
  61. 61. Tander B, Gunes S, Boke O, Alayli G, Kara N, et al. (2008) Polymorphisms of the serotonin-2A receptor and catechol-O-methyltransferase genes: a study on fibromyalgia susceptibility. Rheumatol Int 28: 685–691.
  62. 62. McMahon FJ, Buervenich S, Charney D, Lipsky R, Rush AJ, et al. (2006) Variation in the gene encoding the serotonin 2A receptor is associated with outcome of antidepressant treatment. Am J Hum Genet 78: 804–814.
  63. 63. Porcelli S, Fabbri C, Drago A, Gibiino S, de Ronchi D, et al. (2011) Genetics and Antidepressant: where are we. Clinical Neuropsychiatry 8: 99–150.
  64. 64. Porcelli S, Drago A, Fabbri C, Gibiino S, Calati R, et al. (2011) Pharmacogenetics of antidepressant response. J Psychiatry Neurosci 36: 87–113.
  65. 65. Doehring A, Kusener N, Fluhr K, Neddermeyer TJ, Schneider G, et al. (2011) Effect sizes in experimental pain produced by gender, genetic variants and sensitization procedures. PLoS One 6: e17724.
  66. 66. Xiang X, Jiang Y, Ni Y, Fan M, Shen F, et al. (2012) Catechol-O-methyltransferase polymorphisms do not play a significant role in pain perception in male Chinese Han population. Physiol Genomics 44: 318–328.
  67. 67. Kim H, Lee H, Rowan J, Brahim J, Dionne RA (2006) Genetic polymorphisms in monoamine neurotransmitter systems show only weak association with acute post-surgical pain in humans. Mol Pain 2: 24.
  68. 68. Li J, Wang Y, Hu S, Zhou R, Yu X, et al. (2008) The monoamine oxidase B gene exhibits significant association to ADHD. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 147: 370–374.
  69. 69. Kim H, Mittal DP, Iadarola MJ, Dionne RA (2006) Genetic predictors for acute experimental cold and heat pain sensitivity in humans. J Med Genet 43: e40.
  70. 70. Treister R, Pud D, Ebstein RP, Laiba E, Gershon E, et al. (2009) Associations between polymorphisms in dopamine neurotransmitter pathway genes and pain response in healthy humans. Pain 147: 187–193.
  71. 71. Bayerer B, Engelbergs J, Savidou I, Boes T, Kuper M, et al. (2010) Single nucleotide polymorphisms of the serotonin transporter gene in migraine–an association study. Headache 50: 319–322.
  72. 72. Aoki J, Ikeda K, Murayama O, Yoshihara E, Ogai Y, et al. (2010) The association between personality, pain threshold and a single nucleotide polymorphism (rs3813034) in the 3′-untranslated region of the serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4). J Clin Neurosci 17: 574–578.
  73. 73. Aoki J, Iwahashi K, Ishigooka J, Ikeda K (2011) Association study on catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) Val158Met gene polymorphism and NEO-FFI. Psychiatry Res 187: 312–313.
  74. 74. Nyegaard M, Severinsen JE, Als TD, Hedemand A, Straarup S, et al. (2010) Support of association between BRD1 and both schizophrenia and bipolar affective disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 153B: 582–591.
  75. 75. Vervoort T, Eccleston C, Goubert L, Buysse A, Crombez G (2010) Children’s catastrophic thinking about their pain predicts pain and disability 6 months later. Eur J Pain 14: 90–96.
  76. 76. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE (1997) Manual for the stata-trait anxiety inventory. Mind Garden Inc.
  77. 77. Beck A, Steer R, Brown G (1996) Manual: beck depression inventory- second ed. San Antonio, TX: Psychology Corporation.
  78. 78. Zachariae R, Melchiorsen H, Frobert O, Bjerring P, Bagger JP (2001) Experimental pain and psychologic status of patients with chest pain with normal coronary arteries or ischemic heart disease. Am Heart J 142: 63–71.
  79. 79. Menashe I, Rosenberg PS, Chen BE (2008) PGA: power calculator for case-control genetic association analyses. BMC Genet 9: 36.
  80. 80. Rhudy JL, Maynard LJ, Russell JL (2007) Does in vivo catastrophizing engage descending modulation of spinal nociception? J Pain 8: 325–333.
  81. 81. Turner TJ, Mokler DJ, Luebke JI (2004) Calcium influx through presynaptic 5-HT3 receptors facilitates GABA release in the hippocampus: in vitro slice and synaptosome studies. Neuroscience 129: 703–718.
  82. 82. McBride WJ, Lovinger DM, Machu T, Thielen RJ, Rodd ZA, et al. (2004) Serotonin-3 receptors in the actions of alcohol, alcohol reinforcement, and alcoholism. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 28: 257–267.
  83. 83. Walstab J, Hammer C, Bonisch H, Rappold G, Niesler B (2008) Naturally occurring variants in the HTR3B gene significantly alter properties of human heteromeric 5-hydroxytryptamine-3A/B receptors. Pharmacogenet Genomics 18: 793–802.
  84. 84. Krzywkowski K, Davies PA, Feinberg-Zadek PL, Brauner-Osborne H, Jensen AA (2008) High-frequency HTR3B variant associated with major depression dramatically augments the signaling of the human 5-HT3AB receptor. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105: 722–727.
  85. 85. Krzywkowski K, Davies PA, Irving AJ, Brauner-Osborne H, Jensen AA (2008) Characterization of the effects of four HTR3B polymorphisms on human 5-HT3AB receptor expression and signalling. Pharmacogenet Genomics 18: 1027–1040.
  86. 86. Yamada K, Hattori E, Iwayama Y, Ohnishi T, Ohba H, et al. (2006) Distinguishable haplotype blocks in the HTR3A and HTR3B region in the Japanese reveal evidence of association of HTR3B with female major depression. Biol Psychiatry 60: 192–201.
  87. 87. Glaum SR, Proudfit HK, Anderson EG (1988) Reversal of the Antinociceptive Effects of Intrathecally Administered Serotonin in the Rat by a Selective 5-Ht3 Receptor Antagonist. Neurosci Lett 95: 313–317.
  88. 88. Crisp T, Stafinsky JL, Spanos LJ, Uram M, Perni VC, et al. (1991) Analgesic Effects of Serotonin and Receptor-Selective Serotonin Agonists in the Rat Spinal-Cord. General Pharmacology 22: 247–251.
  89. 89. Danzebrink RM, Gebhart GF (1991) Evidence That Spinal 5-Ht1, 5-Ht2 and 5-Ht3 Receptor Subtypes Modulate Responses to Noxious Colorectal Distension in the Rat. Brain Res 538: 64–75.
  90. 90. Bilge SS, Bozkurt A, Ilkaya F, Ciftcioglu E, Kesim Y, et al. (2012) The antinociceptive effects of intravenous tianeptine in colorectal distension-induced visceral pain in rats: The role of 5-HT3 receptors. Eur J Pharmacol 681: 44–49.
  91. 91. Rahman W, Bauer CS, Bannister K, Vonsy JL, Dolphin AC, et al. (2009) Descending serotonergic facilitation and the antinociceptive effects of pregabalin in a rat model of osteoarthritic pain. Mol Pain 5: 45.
  92. 92. Dogrul A, Ossipov MH, Porreca F (2009) Differential mediation of descending pain facilitation and inhibition by spinal 5HT-3 and 5HT-7 receptors. Brain Res 1280: 52–59.
  93. 93. Bee LA, Dickenson AH (2008) Descending facilitation from the brainstem determines behavioural and neuronal hypersensitivity following nerve injury and efficacy of pregabalin. Pain 140: 209–223.
  94. 94. Umriukhin AE, Wigger A, Singewald N, Landgraf R (2002) Hypothalamic and hippocampal release of serotonin in rats bred for hyper- or hypo-anxiety. Stress 5: 299–305.
  95. 95. Wise CD, Berger BD, Stein L (1972) Benzodiazepines: anxiety-reducing activity by reduction of serotonin turnover in the brain. Science 177: 180–183.
  96. 96. Graeff FG, Viana MB, Mora PO (1997) Dual role of 5-HT in defense and anxiety. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 21: 791–799.
  97. 97. Graeff FG (2002) On serotonin and experimental anxiety. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 163: 467–476.
  98. 98. Dlugos AM, Palmer AA, de Wit H (2009) Negative emotionality: monoamine oxidase B gene variants modulate personality traits in healthy humans. J Neural Transm 116: 1323–1334.
  99. 99. Sabol SZ, Hu S, Hamer D (1998) A functional polymorphism in the monoamine oxidase A gene promoter. Hum Genet 103: 273–279.
  100. 100. Buchsbaum MS, Haier RJ, Murphy DL (1977) Suicide attempts, platelet monoamine oxidase and the average evoked response. Acta Psychiatr Scand 56: 69–79.
  101. 101. Fowler JS, Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Franceschi D, Logan J, et al. (2000) Maintenance of brain monoamine oxidase B inhibition in smokers after overnight cigarette abstinence. Am J Psychiatry 157: 1864–1866.
  102. 102. Meltzer HY, Arora RC (1986) Platelet markers of suicidality. Ann N Y Acad Sci 487: 271–280.
  103. 103. von Knorring AL, Hallman J, von Knorring L, Oreland L (1991) Platelet monoamine oxidase activity in type 1 and type 2 alcoholism. Alcohol Alcohol 26: 409–416.
  104. 104. Frazer A, Hensler JG (1994) Serotonin. Siegel GJ, Agranoff BW, Albers RW, Molinoff RB (ed) Basic neurochemistry. New York: Raven. 304–306.
  105. 105. Pletscher A (1991) The Discovery of Antidepressants - a Winding Path. Experientia 47: 4–8.
  106. 106. Zisook S (1985) A clinical overview of monoamine oxidase inhibitors. Psychosomatics 26: 240–251.
  107. 107. Digman JM (1990) Personality Structure - Emergence of the 5-Factor Model. Annual Review of Psychology 41: 417–440.
  108. 108. Costa PT, McCrae RR (1992) Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) Professional Manual. Psychol Assess Res.
  109. 109. Urata T, Takahashi N, Hakamata Y, Iijima Y, Kuwahara N, et al. (2007) Gene-gene interaction analysis of personality traits in a Japanese population using an electrochemical DNA array chip analysis. Neurosci Lett 414: 209–212.
  110. 110. Kato M, Fukuda T, Wakeno M, Okugawa G, Takekita Y, et al. (2009) Effect of 5-HT1A gene polymorphisms on antidepressant response in major depressive disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 150B: 115–123.
  111. 111. Benedetti F, Barbini B, Bernasconi A, Fulgosi MC, Campori E, et al. (2008) Lithium overcomes the influence of 5-HTTLPR gene polymorphism on antidepressant response to sleep deprivation. J Clin Psychopharmacol 28: 249–251.
  112. 112. Lucae S, Ising M, Horstmann S, Baune BT, Arolt V, et al. (2010) HTR2A gene variation is involved in antidepressant treatment response. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 20: 65–68.
  113. 113. Iordanidou M, Tavridou A, Petridis I, Kyroglou S, Kaklamanis L, et al. (2010) Association of polymorphisms of the serotonergic system with smoking initiation in Caucasians. Drug Alcohol Depend 108: 70–76.
  114. 114. Enoch MA, Gorodetsky E, Hodgkinson C, Roy A, Goldman D (2011) Functional genetic variants that increase synaptic serotonin and 5-HT3 receptor sensitivity predict alcohol and drug dependence. Mol Psychiatry 16: 1139–1146.
  115. 115. Hammer C, Kapeller J, Endele M, Fischer C, Hebebrand J, et al. (2009) Functional variants of the serotonin receptor type 3A and B gene are associated with eating disorders. Pharmacogenet Genomics 19: 790–799.
  116. 116. Seneviratne C, Huang W, Ait-Daoud N, Li MD, Johnson BA (2009) Characterization of a functional polymorphism in the 3′ UTR of SLC6A4 and its association with drinking intensity. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 33: 332–339.
  117. 117. Treister R, Pud D, Ebstein RP, Laiba E, Raz Y, et al. (2011) Association between polymorphisms in serotonin and dopamine-related genes and endogenous pain modulation. J Pain 12: 875–883.
  118. 118. Vijayan NN, Iwayama Y, Koshy LV, Natarajan C, Nair C, et al. (2009) Evidence of association of serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms with schizophrenia in a South Indian population. J Hum Genet 54: 538–542.
  119. 119. Lei X, Chen C, He Q, Moyzis RK, Xue G, et al. (2012) Sex determines which section of the SLC6A4 gene is linked to obsessive-compulsive symptoms in normal Chinese college students. J Psychiatr Res 46: 1153–1160.
  120. 120. Gyawali S, Subaran R, Weissman MM, Hershkowitz D, McKenna MC, et al. (2010) Association of a polyadenylation polymorphism in the serotonin transporter and panic disorder. Biol Psychiatry 67: 331–338.