Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

Humans use multi-objective control to regulate lateral foot placement when walking

  • Jonathan B. Dingwell ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Jonathan B. Dingwell, Joseph P. Cusumano

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    dingwell@psu.edu

    Affiliation Department of Kinesiology, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, State College, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • Joseph P. Cusumano

    Contributed equally to this work with: Jonathan B. Dingwell, Joseph P. Cusumano

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Engineering Science & Mechanics, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, State College, Pennsylvania, United States of America

Abstract

A fundamental question in human motor neuroscience is to determine how the nervous system generates goal-directed movements despite inherent physiological noise and redundancy. Walking exhibits considerable variability and equifinality of task solutions. Existing models of bipedal walking do not yet achieve both continuous dynamic balance control and the equifinality of foot placement humans exhibit. Appropriate computational models are critical to disambiguate the numerous possibilities of how to regulate stepping movements to achieve different walking goals. Here, we extend a theoretical and computational Goal Equivalent Manifold (GEM) framework to generate predictive models, each posing a different experimentally testable hypothesis. These models regulate stepping movements to achieve any of three hypothesized goals, either alone or in combination: maintain lateral position, maintain lateral speed or “heading”, and/or maintain step width. We compared model predictions against human experimental data. Uni-objective control models demonstrated clear redundancy between stepping variables, but could not replicate human stepping dynamics. Most multi-objective control models that balanced maintaining two of the three hypothesized goals also failed to replicate human stepping dynamics. However, multi-objective models that strongly prioritized regulating step width over lateral position did successfully replicate all of the relevant step-to-step dynamics observed in humans. Independent analyses confirmed this control was consistent with linear error correction and replicated step-to-step dynamics of individual foot placements. Thus, the regulation of lateral stepping movements is inherently multi-objective and balances task-specific trade-offs between competing task goals. To determine how people walk in their environment requires understanding both walking biomechanics and how the nervous system regulates movements from step-to-step. Analogous to mechanical “templates” of locomotor biomechanics, our models serve as “control templates” for how humans regulate stepping movements from each step to the next. These control templates are symbiotic with well-established mechanical templates, providing complimentary insights into walking regulation.

Author summary

When we walk, we walk in real-world contexts and with specific goal to achieve. Side-to-side movements are paramount because walking bipeds (humans, animals, robots, etc.) are inherently more unstable laterally. This is particularly important in older adults as sideways falls greatly increase hip fracture risk. Additionally, we normally walk on paths that limit (more or less) our lateral movements. Appropriately regulating lateral stepping movements is thus critical to achieving successful locomotion in any such context. Here, we use appropriate models to test competing hypotheses about how humans regulate lateral stepping movements from each step to the next to identify what task goals they try to achieve. Our work both bridges and unifies perspectives from dynamic walking and computational motor control to provide a coherent theoretical and computational framework from which to study motor regulation in the context of goal-directedness across a wide range of walking tasks and/or conditions.

Introduction

Human movements are subject to both inherent physiological noise [1, 2] and multiple levels of redundancy [35]: i.e., the body has more mechanical degrees-of-freedom than needed to execute most movements, more muscles than needed to move a given joint, etc. Likewise, most tasks we perform exhibit equifinality [3, 68]: i.e., we can achieve them with equal success by an infinite number of movements [912]. It remains a fundamental question in human motor neuroscience to determine how the human nervous system generates accurate and repeatable goal-directed movements in the face of these challenges.

Walking is a highly relevant task that exhibits both considerable variability [13, 14] and equifinality [9, 10, 15]. Lateral (side-to-side) movements in walking are paramount because humans are inherently more unstable laterally [1618]. This contributes to sideways falls and hip fractures in older adults [19, 20]. External lateral stabilization improves lateral walking stability in young [21] and older [22] adults. Similarly, laterally-directed external perturbations destabilize walking humans far more than comparable anterior-posterior perturbations [23]. To prevent falling, lateral CoM accelerations must be redirected in the opposite direction. The simplest way to achieve this is by appropriate lateral placement of the foot on the subsequent step [17, 24, 25]. However, placing the next foot “lateral to the CoM” still leaves infinite options for where to place that foot [18, 25]. Humans readily adapt foot placements to avoid obstacles and/or to step on specified targets [2628]. They also actively modulate foot placement to affect lateral maneuvers [29, 30] or to negotiate stabilizing or destabilizing external forces [31]. That humans can readily modulate foot placement across so many contexts strongly suggests a high degree of equifinality in peoples’ choice of where to step.

Experimentally, it has been shown that center of mass state at mid stance strongly predicts subsequent lateral foot placement of that same step [32, 33]. These predictive correlations vary systematically with changes in step width [34] or walking speed [35], decrease in healthy older adults [36], and are disrupted in persons with stroke who have high fall risk [37]. Lateral foot placement is associated with swing phase activity of the primary hip abductor and adductor muscles [32], although the precise contribution of these activations to the observed predictability is not yet clear. However, such correlations by themselves do not necessarily indicate within-step control processes, but could instead arise largely from passive dynamics [25, 33]. Thus, the extent to which these correlations reflect actual “control” remains unresolved.

More importantly for the present study, such empirical correlations demonstrate only how mid-stance body state predicts subsequent foot placement within a given step [33] and so cannot address how humans regulate stepping movements from each step to the next [9, 10]. However, people walking perform a motor task within a context (i.e., an environment; e.g., Fig 1A) and with specific task goals to achieve (e.g., maintain speed, move in a particular direction, stay on a given path, avoid obstacles, etc.). Numerous studies show people use visual information acquired before each step to plan where to place the foot and then execute the step itself as a predominantly ballistic movement [2628, 38, 39]: i.e., people walk by regulating foot placements across multiple steps. Thus, a complete understanding of walking control requires consistent and complimentary descriptions of both how stepping movements are executed within each step [16, 18, 25, 32, 33] and of how those movements are then regulated between steps [9, 10, 40]. Appropriate models and analytical methods are therefore needed to generate empirically testable and suitably falsifiable hypotheses about how stepping movements are regulated from each step to the next [9, 10].

thumbnail
Fig 1. Defining relevant lateral stepping variables.

A) Common examples of a person walking on paths with lateral boundaries. Such paths occur both indoors and outdoors, can be wide or narrow, may have borders that are more or less well-defined, etc. B-C) Configuration of bipedal walking during a step as viewed in the (B) frontal and (C) horizontal planes. Coordinates are defined in a Cartesian system with {x,y,z} axes as shown in (B) and (C). For convenience, we set the origin at the geometrical center of the path in the lateral direction. Each diagram shows lateral positions of the feet (zL and zR) and body (zB; Eq 2—taken here as a proxy for the center-of-mass, CoM). These yield definitions of primary lateral stepping variables (C) that could be regulated from step to step: positions of the individual feet (zL and zR), lateral body position (zB; Eq 2), change in lateral position (ΔzB; Eq 3), taken here as a proxy for the lateral component of speed or ‘heading’, and step width (w; Eq 4).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006850.g001

Many studies have modeled walking dynamics. Early efforts used inverted pendulum models to show how appropriate foot placement can redirect the center of mass to stabilize walking [41, 42]. Hof derived a ‘Margin of Stability’ [18, 43] that defines the minimum lateral foot placement required to achieve static standing at the end of one step. Koolen et al. [44] derived ‘N-step capture regions’ that predict the range of available foot placements a biped can take to achieve static stability after N steps. The methods of [18, 44] yield inequality conditions on appropriate foot placement that reflect the inherent equifinality [3, 4, 6] of stepping [45]. However, because the above methods are based on achieving some hypothetical stable standing state, and not the continuous dynamic stability expected in walking, they misestimate actual lateral foot placements in humans [18, 25]. Conversely, Kuo [16] and later Hobbelen et al. [46] proposed lateral foot placement control policies that can maintain continuous dynamic limit cycle motion. However, neither of those proposed controllers considered equifinality in choice of foot placement. Thus, prior studies have developed control strategies that either achieve continuous dynamic balance control [16, 46], or allow for equifinality of foot placement [18, 44] as widely observed in humans [9, 15, 40, 4750], but not both.

Moreover, when humans walk in the real world, they regularly negotiate a wide variety of environmental contexts [51], both indoors and outdoors [27, 28], perhaps containing fixed [27, 28, 51] and/or moving [52, 53] obstacles to negotiate. In nearly all of these contexts, humans walk on paths that in some way restrict their lateral movements (e.g., Fig 1A). The lateral boundaries of such paths are sometimes well-defined (e.g., building hallways, store aisles, etc.), or sometimes less so (e.g., outdoor walking paths [28]), etc.). Appropriately regulating lateral stepping movements is critical to achieving successful locomotion in these contexts. However, most models of how people navigate environments [5254] consider the person as a point mass and do not address the question of how people regulate their stepping movements to achieve this navigation [55]. Thus, it is possible purposeful (goal-directed) walking requires precise step-to-step control of position and/or heading to stay on one’s desired path (or trajectory) [52, 53]. Conversely, people may adopt less stringent control that corrects errors in position and/or heading only when they become “sufficiently large” as to need correcting [3, 8, 12]. To our knowledge, the question of how humans regulate their lateral stepping movements from each step to the next within their respective environment has not been addressed, either experimentally or computationally.

Here, we adopt a computational framework to generate concrete, experimentally testable a priori hypotheses [7, 8] to address these questions. This framework is founded on the idea of a goal function that theoretically defines a task and, in the presence of equifinality [3, 4], determines the sets of all possible task solution strategies: i.e., a goal equivalent manifold or GEM [68, 12]. We previously successfully implemented this approach to identify how humans regulate stride-to-stride stepping movements in the sagittal plane [9, 10]. Several independent studies replicated those primary findings [4850] and/or subsequently confirmed other main theoretical predictions experimentally [40, 47, 56]. Analogous to mechanical “templates” of locomotor biomechanics [5759], our prior models [9, 10] serve as “control templates” for how humans regulate sagittal plane stepping movements: i.e., they contain the minimal number of variables and parameters needed to fully capture the relevant walking behavior [57, 59]. Here, we extend this approach to develop goal functions and computational models that pose testable hypotheses for how humans (or other bipeds) might regulate lateral stepping movements as they walk on some specified path.

Results

Defining the task and relevant variables

We consider the common task of some biped (ostensibly a human, but could more generally be any robot, walking model, etc.) walking on any clearly defined path with identifiable lateral boundaries (e.g., Fig 1A). We treat walking control as hierarchical. We assume some within-step processes (be they active, passive, or both) generate each individual step [16, 18, 25, 32, 33]. We presume those processes will be different for different bipeds and our models are purposefully agnostic to such details. Indeed, a strength of our approach is precisely that it can apply to any reasonable biped (human, robot, etc.), regardless of how that biped may be actuated and/or controlled at a mechanical (and/or possibly neuromuscular) level to generate each step [9].

What we study here is the between-step regulation of the output of that process. That is, conceptually, we consider the result of each stepping motion as the input to our models [9, 10, 40] of the between-step regulation needed to achieve goal-directed walking [2628, 38, 39]. We focus on regulating lateral stepping movements in the frontal plane because lateral balance is a significant challenge in bipedal walking [16, 21, 23, 60] and because sideways falls are especially dangerous for elderly adults [19, 20, 61]. We further consider lateral stepping as decoupled from sagittal plane movement (i.e., forward progression). While there is some coupling between frontal and sagittal planes, it is minimal [17, 25, 46, 60] and our prior work has already addressed how humans regulate sagittal plane stepping [9, 10].

The simplest, mechanically sufficient description of the frontal plane configuration of the lateral components of a stepping biped (Fig 1B) includes locations of the body (ostensibly the center-of-mass) and of each of the two feet [33, 46, 62]. Extending our prior sagittal plane work [9, 10], the primary task requirement for any such biped is simply to not step off the path, in this case to either side, i.e.: (1) where we take the z-coordinate to define the lateral direction (Fig 1B and 1C), WP(x) defines the net width of the walking path, and {zLn, zRn} define left and right lateral foot placements for the nth step in a sequence of N consecutive steps. Walking on a treadmill, as studied here, merely implies WP(x) = WTM ≡ the constant width of the treadmill belt, but Eq (1) applies generally to any walking path with finite width (e.g., Fig 1A, etc.).

Our objective is to identify what strategies people choose to accomplish this very general walking task. A key observation is that any sequence of {zLn, zRn} that satisfies the inequality of Eq (1) will achieve this. Thus, an infinite number of choices for each zLn and zRn exists and many strategies could generate feasible sequences of {zLn, zRn} that satisfy Eq (1). To identify these strategies, we need to quantify the lateral motion of the walker relative to the path, and the lateral motions of the walker’s feet relative to its body.

While {zLn, zRn} define the task goal via Eq (1) and ultimately enact any stepping control strategy, we presume {zLn, zRn} are coordinated to achieve some more general goal related to whole body movement. In particular, {zLn, zRn} establish where the walker’s body is relative to the path [25, 32, 33, 35]. Here, we take this body position on any given step, zBn, as the midpoint between the two feet (Fig 1B and 1C): (2)

This choice of zBn approximates the center-of-mass location for steady-state upright walking on level ground [21, 62]. Eq (2) assumes only that stepping movements are symmetrical relative to the center-of-mass, a finding broadly validated in multiple studies of human walking [42, 6365]. Eq (2) is also consistent with multiple well-established models of pedestrian navigation [52, 54, 55].

In addition to the walker’s location on the path, we must also consider how it moves relative to its path. Goal-directed walking and pedestrian navigation require maintaining direction [52, 53] or “heading” [66, 67]. Likewise, the lateral (z) component of CoM velocity contributes to subsequent foot placement within each step [18, 33, 46] and in the sagittal plane, humans regulate speed and not position [9, 10]. Here, we thus take ΔzBn as a proxy for lateral speed, or equivalently the lateral component of “heading” (Fig 1C): (3)

Lastly, regulating step width to maintain lateral balance is also important to walking [16, 21, 23, 68]. Here, we define step width as the difference between the lateral locations of the right and left foot (Fig 1C): (4)

Thus, from the most basic definition of walking, any biped walker (human, robot, model, etc.) must therefore exhibit some sequence of {zLn, zRn, zBn, ΔzBn, wn} over consecutive steps. However, by definition these variables are not all independent, as can be readily seen by writing Eqs (2) and (4) in matrix form as: (5)

Thus, specifying either [zLn, zRn] or [zBn, wn] fully specifies the other. We therefore take {zBn, ΔzBn, wn} to be the biomechanically relevant variables to be regulated and treat {zLn, zRn} as effectors that enact this regulation. Like our prior sagittal-plane work [10], strategies that regulate some combination of {zBn, ΔzBn, wn} thus define “control templates” to describe step-to-step regulation of lateral stepping movements that can then compliment mechanical templates of within-step locomotor biomechanics [57, 58]. We then seek the most parsimonious such strategy that captures the variability and step-to-step dynamical structure we observe in experiments.

Lateral stepping dynamics in humans

To test different hypothesized control strategies, we re-analyzed data from a prior study in which young healthy human participants walked on a 1.77 m wide motorized treadmill at a pre-determined comfortable walking speed [69, 70]. We measured time series of left (zL) and right (zR) lateral foot placements. We used Eqs (2)(4) to calculate corresponding time series of body positions (zB), heading (ΔzB), and step widths (w), which we then analyzed.

To quantify variability, we computed standard deviations for each time series. To quantify temporal correlations across consecutive steps, we computed scaling exponents, α, using Detrended Fluctuation Analysis (DFA) [9, 71] (see Methods). An α > ½ indicates statistical persistence (deviations in either direction are more likely to be followed by deviations in the same direction). An α < ½ implies anti-persistence (subsequent deviations are more likely to be in the opposite direction). An α = ½ indicates uncorrelated fluctuations (subsequent deviations are equally likely to be in either direction). A value of α = 1½ indicates brown noise (i.e., integrated white noise), equivalent to Brownian motion. In the context of control, variables not tightly regulated typically exhibit strong persistence (α >> ½). Conversely, variables that are more tightly regulated typically exhibit approximately uncorrelated fluctuations (α ≈ ½) [9, 10, 12].

All of the candidate stepping variables, {zBn, ΔzBn, wn}, and also foot placements, {zLn, zRn}, fluctuated (e.g., Fig 2A), with magnitudes of variability well above the level of measurement noise (Fig 2B). Fluctuations in lateral body position (zB) exhibited strong statistical persistence (α >> ½). Fluctuations in heading (ΔzB) exhibited mostly statistical anti-persistence (α < ½), consistent with heading being the first-difference of position (Fig 2C). Fluctuations in step width (w) exhibited weak statistical persistence (½ < α << 1½).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Experimental values of primary stepping variables.

A) Example time series data for a representative trial from a typical participant. Each plot shows 290 consecutive steps of left (zL; red) and right (zR; blue) foot placements, body position (zB), heading (ΔzB), and step width (w). For the stepping plot (zL; zR), black vertical dashed lines indicate the lateral edges of the treadmill (±0.885 m). For the other time series plots (zB, ΔzB, and w), red vertical dashed lines indicate ±5 standard deviations, as determined from the average of the standard deviations of all participants. B) Standard deviation (σ) values for all trials for all participants for each variable (zB, ΔzB, and w). C) DFA scaling exponent (α) values for all trials for all participants for each variable (zB, ΔzB, and w). For (B) and (C), each subplot shows a summary boxplot (blue–left) indicating the median, 1st and 3rd quartiles, and whiskers extending to 1.5× the inter-quartile range, with values beyond that range shown as individual data points. Each subplot also shows individual data points (red dots–right) indicating all individual trials for all participants. These experimental data were aggregated across 65 total trials of 290 steps each, as obtained from 13 participants (5 trials each).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006850.g002

Given that all variables exhibited both substantial variability and varying degrees of statistical persistence, one cannot conclude from these data alone which variable (or combination of variables) were or were not regulated. Indeed, we might be tempted to conclude that ΔzB and w were more tightly regulated than zB because they both exhibited α ≈ ½ [9, 10, 12]. However, any such inference would be incorrect. As we demonstrate, such a conclusion would succumb to the formal logical fallacy of “affirming the consequent” [72]: i.e., while processes that are tightly regulated typically yield α ≈ ½, not all processes that exhibit α ≈ ½ are tightly regulated. What is needed are appropriate computational models that can accurately predict these variability and statistical persistence relationships and indicate what gives rise to them.

Uni-objective stepping regulation

We consider first the simplest such stepping control strategies, namely those that regulate only one of the variables (Eqs (2)(4)) to maintain some constant value of that variable. The strategy “stay in the middle of the path” implies one would adjust consecutive foot placements to maintain zBn = zB* ≡ Constant. We note that other reasonable choices for zBn different from Eq (2) exist. Each would yield a different corresponding zB*, but would not change the step-to-step dynamics quantified here. Likewise, because we can define the origin of our coordinate system (Fig 1) at any point we choose, we could always take zB* = 0, but we retain the more general form for now. The strategy “maintain heading” (regardless of lateral position) implies that one would try to maintain ΔzBn = ΔzB* ≡ Constant. Here, ΔzB* = 0 would represent the goal to walk “straight ahead” (i.e., with no lateral deviation), but again we retain the more general form for now. Lastly, the strategy “maintain step width” (e.g., as in [16, 22, 23]) implies that one would try to maintain wn = w* ≡ Constant (regardless of absolute position or heading). Here, we would take w* = 〈w〉 ≡ some average (or typical) step width (e.g., for an individual or group, etc.) as the desired step width to maintain.

Each of these strategies can be defined in terms of a scalar “goal function” [6, 7, 12] in each variable separately, each having the same general form: (6) where q ∈ {zB, ΔzB, w} and the goal is to drive Fq → 0 (i.e., minimize errors with respect to the goal). For each candidate control variable, we write a corresponding state-update equation governing the regulated step-to-step dynamics as a 1D map in q: (7) where q is the controller “state” being regulated, uq(qn) is the relevant control input, σmνm and σaνa are multiplicative and additive noise terms, respectively, and g is an additional gain (introduced in our previous work: [9, 10]) that could be used to tune the regulator away from the derived optimal control. Here, no such tuning was necessary, so we set g = 1 for all subsequent analyses. Single variable regulators such as in Eq (7) thus allow the remaining unregulated variables to “drift” under the action of motor noise.

Following prior work [9, 10, 12], these controllers were derived analytically (see Methods) as stochastic optimal single-step controllers with direct error feedback, following the Minimum Intervention Principle [3, 4]. This yielded the following stochastically optimal control input, uq, as a function of the current state, qn: (8)

We used these models to simulate walking trials that we then compared directly to experimental data from humans. Each such model depends on three parameters (σa, σm, and γ/α). We conducted parameter sensitivity analyses (see S1 Appendix) across wide ranges of each of these parameters to determine if any of these candidate uni-objective control models could adequately replicate the experimental findings of Fig 2.

None of the uni-objective control models came close to capturing the behavior observed experimentally, regardless of parameter choices (see S1 Appendix for details). Nearly all simulations either regularly stepped off the prescribed path, or took biomechanically unrealistic steps, or both. We therefore also imposed additional biomechanically realistic constraints to each control model (see S1 Appendix for details) to ensure they did not either walk off the path (here, the treadmill: zMax = ±0.885 m), or take steps that were unrealistically too wide or too narrow (defined here as no more than ±5 standard deviations, based on experimental means; Fig 2). All of these control models fully satisfied (by construction) the requirements of the walking task (Eq 1) and did so by taking steps that were biomechanically feasible [34, 62, 73]. Nevertheless, all of the model configurations tested (e.g., Fig 3) exhibited step-to-step dynamics that were both qualitatively and quantitatively substantially different from humans (see S1 Appendix for details) and thus failed to replicate the lateral stepping behavior observed in experiment (Fig 2).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Typical model simulation results for biomechanically constrained position control.

A) Example time series data for a single representative trial. The time series plotted, axes, and axis limits are all the same as in Fig 2. B) Standard deviation (σ) values and (C) DFA scaling exponent (α) values for all trials for each variable (zB, ΔzB, and w). Each subplot in (B) and (C) shows boxplots (blue–left) and for 30 representative simulated trials from the model (red–right). All boxplots were constructed in the same manner as described in Fig 2. This model failed to replicate experimental findings from humans, as did all other uni-objective control models across all parameter ranges tested (see S1 Appendix for complete details).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006850.g003

Multi-objective stepping regulation

Because no uni-objective controller model (with or without added biomechanical constraints) adequately replicated experimental findings, we next considered multi-objective control of 2 candidate parameters. The general 2-state vector-valued goal function [6, 7, 12] for any of these becomes: (9) where q1 and q2 are any two distinct variables selected from q1,2 ∈ {zB, ΔzB, w} and the goal is again to drive F → 0. The corresponding state-update equation becomes: (10) where the q1,2 are the controller states being regulated. We then followed the same analytical derivation process [9, 10, 12] (see Methods), which yielded the following stochastically optimal control input: (11)

Both the state update equation (Eq (10)) and control inputs (Eq (11)) were chosen to have no coupling between their q1 and q2 components (see Methods). Thus, at each new step, each individual optimal controller (for q1 and q2 separately) predicts a different foot placement for the next foot. As one cannot place one’s foot in two places at once, the net result (i.e., final predicted foot placement) becomes a weighted average of the foot placements predicted by each of the uni-variate models for q1 and q2 separately.

We used these multi-objective models to simulate walking trials that we then compared directly to experimental data from humans (Fig 2). We conducted parameter sensitivity analyses (see S2 Appendix) across wide ranges of each of the governing parameters to determine if any of these candidate multi-objective control models could adequately replicate the experimental findings of Fig 2. Here, we emphasized varying the simulation parameter (ρ) that specified the weighted average between q1 control and q2 control (see S2 Appendix for details).

Simulations that regulated combinations of position and heading (i.e., [q1, q2] = [zB, ΔzB]) failed to replicate human stepping dynamics, mainly by regularly violating reasonable step width limits (Fig. S2-2 in S2 Appendix). Across a substantial range of the parameter space tested, simulations that regulated combinations of heading and step width (i.e., [q1, q2] = [ΔzB, w]) did successfully achieve the task goal (Eq (1)) and did so using biomechanically reasonable step widths (Fig. S2-4 in S2 Appendix). Thus, many of these simulations predicted stepping strategies humans could have used to accomplish the task. However, all of these simulations yielded variability and statistical persistence of lateral position (σ(zB) and α(zB)) that far exceeded experimental values (Fig. S2-4 in S2 Appendix). Thus, all simulations regulating heading and step width failed to replicate human stepping dynamics, even though this strategy was physiologically feasible.

Simulations that regulated combinations of position and step width (i.e., [q1, q2] = [zB, w]) also failed to replicate human stepping dynamics over a wide range of ρ (0% ≤ ρ < ~89% and ρ > ~97%; see Supplement S2; Fig. S2-6). However, for baseline parameter values, when the distribution of control was weighted to favor ~93% step width control, vs. ~7% position control, these simulations did replicate the basic statistical properties of human stepping dynamics (namely, both the standard deviations (σ) and statistical persistence (α) for all of the specified variables (zB, ΔzB, and zB) (Fig 4; same as Fig. S2-5 in S2 Appendix).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Typical model simulation results for multi-objective position-step width control.

A) Example time series data for a single representative trial simulated for baseline parameter values (see S2 Appendix) and a control proportion that was weighted at 93% step width / 7% position control. The time series plotted, axes, and axis limits are all the same as in Fig 2. B) Standard deviation (σ) and (C) DFA scaling exponent (α) values for all trials for each variable (zB, ΔzB, and w). Each subplot in (B) and (C) shows boxplots for the experimental data (blue–left; from Fig 2) and for 30 representative simulated trials from the model (red–right) using the same parameter values as in (A). All boxplots were constructed in the same manner as described in Fig 2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006850.g004

Indeed, more refined parameter sensitivity analyses across a narrower range of 89% ≤ ρ ≤ 97% (Fig 5) yielded a range of solutions weighted strongly in favor of step width control that successfully replicated human stepping dynamics (Fig 2) and were also robust to variations in additive noise (σa) and cost function weights (γ/α) (Fig 5A). For all parameter variations tested across the range 89% ≤ ρ ≤ 97%, no simulation ever took any steps that exceeded either the lateral boundary limits (i.e., stepped off the treadmill; Fig 5C) or the step width limits (i.e., took excessively wide or narrow steps; Fig 5D). Thus, across all of these cases, this model successfully achieved the walking task (Eq (1)), did so by taking biomechanically feasible steps, and also successfully replicated all of the same stepping dynamics observed experimentally in humans.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Parameter sensitivity results for multi-objective position-step width control.

A) Standard deviations for each primary output variable (zB, ΔzB, and zB). B) DFA scaling exponents (α) for each primary output variable. In both (A) and (B), horizontal gray bands indicate the mean ± 1SD band exhibited by humans for that variable to indicate the range of values observed experimentally. For display purposes, all vertical axes are scaled to the mean ± 2SD band exhibited by humans. C) Percentage of steps (zL and zR) taken in each simulated trial that exceeded the Lateral Boundary Limits (±0.885 m): i.e., stepped off the treadmill. D) Percentage of steps that exceeded Step Width Limits (±5σ as determined from experimental data; equivalent step width range: −0.15 cm to +25.54 cm), reflecting biomechanically unrealistically wide or narrow steps (see S1 Appendix for details). Stepping data shown here were simulated at multi-objective proportions from 89% step width (11% position) control, every 2% up to 97% step width (3% position) control. Black symbols indicate ‘baseline’ parameter values (see S2 Appendix): σ'a based on experimental values, σ'm = 0.1∙σ'a, and (γ/α)' = 0.1. Red and Green symbols indicate these same baseline parameter values, except γ/α = 0.0 and 0.2, respectively. Blue and Cyan symbols indicate these same baseline parameter values, except σa = 0.9∙σ'a and 1.1∙σ'a, respectively.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006850.g005

Measures of variability (e.g., Fig 5A, etc.) quantify average magnitudes of differences across all steps, but do not indicate how stepping is executed dynamically over time. DFA analyses (e.g., Fig 5B, etc.) quantify statistical persistence (average step-to-step linear dependence) across time [9, 71], but independent of the magnitude of the variance. Thus, neither of these calculations (σ or α) quantifies how the proportionality of subsequent corrections (in time) of step-to-step fluctuations might differ with the magnitude of those initial fluctuations. Conversely, the Minimum Intervention Principle [3] and N-step capturability concept [44] both suggest that corrections to errors could well vary nonlinearly with the magnitudes of those errors: i.e., one could readily ‘ignore’ small errors and correct only larger ones. Neither σ nor α would capture any such dependence. Therefore, we conducted a third independent set of analyses to verify these findings [10].

If steps are adjusted at each step based on the previous step (as they are designed to be in our models; Eqs (7) and (10)), we would hypothesize that a stepping regulator trying to maintain some constant average value of any (Eqs (6) & (9)), when observing any deviation on a given step (i.e., ), should correct that deviation on the subsequent step by making a corresponding change, Δqn+1 = qn+1qn, in the opposite direction [10]. For each simulated and experimental (human) data set, we constructed plots of Δqn+1 vs. q'n for each q ∈ {zB, ΔzB, w}. We computed the linear slopes (using least-squares regression) and strength of correlation (r2) for each corresponding relationship [10]. We assumed our simulations, which enacted precisely this form of control, would exhibit evidence of strong linear control of step width (w), reflected by slopes close to −1 with high correlation, but weak linear control of position (zB), reflected by slopes close to 0 with very low correlation. We hypothesized that if humans exerted step-to-step control in a similar manner, they would exhibit results consistent with our simulations. Indeed, this was precisely the case (Fig 6). All of the same simulations that precisely matched human stepping dynamics in Fig 5 (above) also just as precisely matched the strength of direct step-to-step corrections of all three time series variables (Fig 6).

thumbnail
Fig 6. Direct control analysis results for multi-objective position-step width control.

A-B) Example plots of how errors in relative position (z'Bn) and relative step width (w') were corrected on subsequent strides (ΔzB(n+1) and Δw(n+1)). Data are shown for (A) one typical experimental trial from typical human participant and for (B) one typical trial from the multi-objective position-step width controller adopting baseline parameter values (see Fig 6). C) Linear regression slopes for each corresponding relationship for each primary output variable (zB, ΔzB, and zB). D) Corresponding linear correlation (r2) values for each linear regression for each primary output variable. In both (C) and (D), horizontal gray bands indicate the mean ± 1SD band exhibited by humans for that variable to indicate the range of values observed experimentally. In both (C) and (D), horizontal axis limits and symbol / color designations are the same as shown in Fig 5.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006850.g006

Together, the results of Figs 5 and 6 make clear that the multi-objective position–step width control model accurately replicates the basic step-to-step dynamics of the fundamental variables {zBn, ΔzBn, wn}. Of course, these models not only need to regulate these variables appropriately, but should also generate appropriate stepping movements of the feet themselves {zLn, zRn}, as these are the effectors that enact this regulation. Therefore, as a final confirmation of the preceding results, we assessed how the above dynamics then mapped onto the stepping dynamics of {zLn, zRn}, which is easily accomplished using Eq (5).

The goals to maintain constant zBn = zB* and wn = w*, when considered individually, each yield linear GEMs that are orthogonal to each other in the [zL, zR] plane (Fig 7A). For the multi-objective position–step width control models, the [zBn, wn] dynamics (i.e., including both variability and temporal correlation structure) maps into [zLn, zRn] dynamics precisely as defined by Eq (5). We thus predicted that if humans also adopted control consistent with this policy, their experimental [zBn, wn] dynamics would also map into [zLn, zRn] dynamics in the same way as in the models. Indeed, this was precisely the case (Fig 7B and 7C), further supporting the validity of these multi-objective position–step width control models.

thumbnail
Fig 7. Projection of [zB, w] control variables onto the [zL, zR] stepping plane.

A) Example plots of stepping data for simulations of uni-objective controllers projected onto the [zL, zR] plane (see Eq (5)). One typical simulation trial each is shown for controllers regulating either position (zB) only (left) or step width (w) only (right), each simulated using baseline parameter values (see S1 Appendix). In each plot, diagonal lines indicate the zB* = constant (green, left) or w* = constant (red, right) GEM’s, as determined from the average position or step width (respectively) exhibited on that trial. All combinations of [zL, zR] that lie along either GEM equally satisfy the respective goal. B) Example plots of stepping data for one typical simulated trial from the multi-objective position–step width (zB-w) controller, simulated using baseline parameter values (see S2 Appendix) and for one typical experimental trial. For each plot, the zB* and w* GEM’s were determined from the average position and step width exhibited on that trial. C) Standard deviation (σ) and (D) DFA scaling exponent (α) values for all trials for left (zL) and right (zR) steps, body position (zB), and step width (w) for humans (blue) and for the multi-objective position–step width controller, simulated using baseline parameter values (red). All boxplots were constructed in the same manner as described in Fig 2. Note that as all variables are in units of meters, variability measures can be compared directly, as coordinate dependence is not an issue here. All simulation values are well within the range of experimental results. Thus, the multi-objective position–step width control model captured the observed left/right stepping dynamics in [zL, zR] as it regulated [zB, w].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006850.g007

Discussion

Human walking exhibits considerable variability [14, 74] due to both physiological noise [2] and inherent redundancy [3, 6]. Experimental data alone (e.g., Fig 2) yield no specific insights into which stepping variables (Fig 1C) humans may and/or may not regulate during walking. Thus, appropriate computational models are critical to disambiguate the various possibilities. Existing models of how people regulate lateral foot placement achieve either continuous dynamic balance control [16, 46], or the equifinality of foot placement [18, 44] that people also exhibit [9, 10, 15], but not both. Moreover, existing models that consider only within-step foot placement [16, 3235, 46] do not capture how humans regulate movements from step to step to navigate the environments [5254] they walk in [27, 28, 51]. The present work extends a theoretical and computational framework [68, 12] that previously identified how humans regulate stride-to-stride movements in the sagittal plane [9, 10, 15, 47]. Here, we present new goal functions that posed hypotheses about how humans might regulate lateral stepping movements. We developed and implemented predictive models of lateral stepping dynamics that we then used to test those hypotheses against experimental data.

Simple mechanical models have successfully elucidated many fundamental aspects of locomotion dynamics (e.g., [16, 7577]). These models represent biomechanical “templates” [5759] for the most basic mechanics and dynamics of any biped (human, robot, model, etc.) and reveal how those biomechanics shape locomotor behavior. Similarly, the stepping models presented here and in our previous work [9, 10] act as “control templates” that serve as minimal models to describe how movements of those mechanical templates (e.g., [16, 7577]) should be regulated from step to step if they are to match human behavior. Our models show how fundamental stepping variables can be regulated to satisfy some particular goal-directed strategy (e.g., “stay in the middle of the path”, “maintain heading”, “maintain step width”, etc.). In this sense, these control templates are symbiotic with mechanical templates. To determine how people walk in their environment [5254] requires complimentary descriptions both of what governs walking movements biomechanically within each step and also of how the nervous system regulates those movements from step-to-step to achieve specific goal-directed walking tasks. In a parallel way that mechanical templates can be “anchored” [5759] within more elaborate, higher-dimensional mechanical models, so too could these simple “control templates” be anchored hierarchically within more elaborate neurophysiological control models (e.g., [78, 79], etc.).

Our uni-objective control models (Fig 3 and S1 Appendix) yielded clear evidence of the inherent redundancies involved in regulating any one variable. Several configurations of unconstrained uni-objective control could achieve the required walking task (Eq (1)) with biomechanically feasible stepping movements. All of them could be made to do so by adding appropriate constraints (e.g., Fig 3). However, none of the uni-objective control models tested (S1 Appendix) yielded stepping dynamics comparable to humans. Likewise, multiple configurations of various multi-objective control models (Supplement 2) could achieve the required task goals with feasible stepping movements, but most of these likewise failed to replicate human stepping dynamics. However, over a somewhat narrow range, multi-objective control models that strongly prioritized regulating step width while allowing some degree of lateral position control did successfully replicate all of the relevant step-to-step dynamics observed in humans (Figs 4 and 5). Independent analyses confirmed that this control was consistent with linear error correction (Fig 6) and also replicated step-to-step dynamics of foot placement (Fig 7). Thus, in contrast to how humans regulate sagittal plane stepping movements [9, 10, 40, 47], the regulation of lateral stepping movements is inherently multi-objective and balances task-specific trade-offs between competing task goals.

In the context of control, variables not tightly regulated [9, 12] typically exhibit stronger statistical persistence (α >> ½), tightly regulated variables typically exhibit fluctuations with α ≈ ½, and variables that are ‘over-controlled’ exhibit fluctuations with α < ½ [9, 12]. However, here we show this is not always the case. Here for example, ΔzB is the first-difference of zB (Eq (3)) and it is this relationship that determines how αzB) is related to α(zB). In the uni-objective models, controlling zB (Figs. S1-1 & S1-2 in S1 Appendix) thus yields α(zB) ≈ ½ as expected, but αzB) << ½ not because ΔzB is being ‘over-controlled’, but because ΔzB is the first-difference of zB. Controlling ΔzB (Figs. S1-3 & S1-4 in S1 Appendix) yields αzB) ≈ ½ as expected, but α(zB) ≈ 1½ because integrating the uncorrelated ΔzB yields Brownian motion of zB. Lastly, controlling w (Figs. S1-5 & S1-6 in S1 Appendix) yields both α(zB) ≈ 1½ and αzB) ≈ ½ when neither variable is being controlled. Thus, inferring that ΔzB is ‘tightly regulated’ because we observed αzB) ≈ ½ experimentally (Fig 2), without considering how ΔzB relates to zB would constitute “affirming the consequent” [72]: i.e., just because AB, does not mean BA. Thus, both the experimental findings (Fig 2) and model results (S1 and S2 Appendices) are consistent with how ΔzB and zB are related. However, only the modeling results demonstrate how the step-to-step fluctuation dynamics (as quantified by α) reflect which variables are (and are not) being regulated.

Several recent experimental studies have demonstrated how center of mass state (lateral displacement and velocity) at mid stance predicts subsequent lateral foot placement within a given step [3237]. However, such correlations may not reflect within-step “control”, but instead result mainly from passive dynamics [25, 33]. They also do not consider other possible within-step mechanisms of balance control such as modulation of ankle torques, push-off, etc. [80, 81]. The present work both extends and compliments that work in at least two critical ways. First, we directly identify how humans regulate stepping movements across consecutive steps. Within-step models and/or analyses cannot capture the between-step regulation of stepping movements that is required to achieve goal-directed walking tasks [9, 10, 2628, 3840]. Wang et al. [33] found that position on the treadmill (which they called ‘station keeping’) contributed to lateral foot placement, but dismissed this because it accounted for only a very small amount of the total variance. Here, we show definitively that even though lateral position is regulated much more weakly than step width (by roughly the same percent as in [33]), this contribution remains critical because without it, the walker would eventually drift off of the path (to one side or the other). Such effects could not be appreciated from only within-step analyses, such as those in [3237]. Second, we do this in a way that directly identifies how step-to-step regulation achieves goal-directed walking [5254], which is a necessary component of any comprehensive understanding of how humans (or other bipeds) walk within any environmental context [27, 28, 51].

A critical question in computational neuroscience is to determine how the nervous system makes accurate goal-directed movements in the face of physiological noise [1, 2] and redundancy [35]. The Minimum Intervention Principle [3, 4] suggests the nervous system should correct only deviations that adversely affect task performance and ignore variations that do not. We previously identified evidence for such control in regulating sagittal plane stepping [9, 10, 15, 40, 47] that was independently verified by other studies [4850]. The present work extends these ideas to regulation of lateral stepping. Here, we likewise identified strong evidence of ‘MIP-type’ control in several respects: the clear redundancy between variables (Fig 3 and S1 Appendix), replication of experimental findings by regulating only 2 candidate variables (Figs 47 and S2 Appendix), and prioritization of step width control over lateral position control (Figs 5 and 6). However, our findings also reveal limits to the MIP concept. Given the wide (1.77 m) path (treadmill belt) participants walked on, they easily could have ignored small deviations in lateral position, instead making corrections only for very large deviations (i.e., near the lateral edges of the treadmill). Likewise, given the capture region predictions of [44] and/or margin of stability predictions of [18], participants could have similarly ignored small deviations in step width, instead correcting only much larger deviations. Implementing any such strategy would have led to nonlinear (e.g., piece-wise linear, “deadband”, cubic, etc.) distributions in the plots of Fig 6A and 6B, reflecting greater proportional correction of larger errors compared to smaller ones. However, the linear relationships shown in Fig 6 clearly demonstrate that participants did not do this. Instead, participants strongly corrected both small and large deviations in step width similarly and weakly corrected both small and large deviations in lateral position similarly.

Still other viable alternative control strategies exist to regulate lateral stepping movements while walking. For example, given the redundancies clearly available in both step width and lateral position, there is no physiological necessity to correct errors over only one step, as our models do. A walker (human, robot, model, etc.) could instead correct errors more gradually over multiple steps, as described for example in [44], and still achieve the task goal (Eq (1)) equally as well as the models presented here. However, implementing control one step at a time is mechanically sufficient to maintain stable walking [16, 41, 42, 46] and our work is consistent with those ideas. Our models for regulating lateral stepping are also consistent with our prior models that regulated sagittal plane stepping [9, 10] and complimentary to experimental findings of within-step control of foot placement [32, 33, 36, 80, 81]. Even when navigating far more complex environments containing many targets/obstacles, visual information about these impediments is acquired only 1–3 steps in advance [27, 28]. Regulating movements only one (or a very few) step(s) at a time allows humans to efficiently navigate complex environments using a noisy neuromotor system [1, 2], where trying to predict motor outcomes farther into the future would not be practical.

The stepping regulation simulations presented (Figs 47) hold for the context studied here: i.e., walking on a level surface along a straight path of constant width (i.e.: WP(x) = WTM ≡ constant). These simulations demonstrate what step-to-step regulation is minimally required achieve this task: i.e., regulate ‘mostly step width but some lateral position’ (Figs 5, 6 and 7). Under different task conditions, however, we would fully expect other combinations of regulation to arise. For example, we might expect people to regulate lateral position more tightly when walking on a narrow path [73] or on fixed lines on a treadmill (e.g., as in [34]), etc. People might regulate both position and step width more tightly when subjected to lateral perturbations [23]. People might regulate heading [52, 53, 66] when walking on curved paths, etc. Thus, it is critical to understand that the final control policies revealed here (Figs 5 and 6) do not represent “the” controllers for regulating lateral stepping. Quite the opposite: walking successfully in a complex world [28, 51] requires flexibility in how we regulate our stepping movements. Indeed, different task conditions (those mentioned above and others) would introduce new and/or different task goals. Thus, the most important contribution of this work is to present a coherent goal-directed framework [7, 12] that can generate empirically testable hypotheses about how humans might regulate stepping movements across a wide range of walking tasks and/or conditions.

Methods

Ethics statement

Prior to participating, all participants signed informed consent statements approved by the Institutional Review Boards of both Brooke Army Medical Center and The University of Texas at Austin.

Experimental participants

We compared model predictions to human experimental data taken from the same data set as analyzed in [69, 70]. Thirteen young healthy adults participated (Table 1). All participants were screened to exclude anyone who reported any history of orthopedic problems, recent lower extremity injuries, any visible gait anomalies, or were taking medications that may have influenced their gait.

thumbnail
Table 1. Participant characteristics.

All values except Sex are given as Mean ± Standard Deviation. Walking speeds shown are the participants’ actual speeds [m/s]. These corresponded to the same pre-designated non-dimensional walking speed (Froude speed ≡ Fn = 0.16) [69, 70] for all participants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006850.t001

Experimental protocol and data processing

Experimental protocols were described in detail previously [69, 70]. In brief, all participants walked on a 1.77m wide treadmill in a Computer Assisted Rehabilitation Environment (CAREN) virtual reality system (Motek, Amsterdam, Netherlands). Each participant walked at a constant speed (Table 1), non-dimensionally scaled to their leg length [69]. Following a 6-minute acclimation period, each participant completed five 3-minute walking trials with normal optic flow and visual scene movement matched to the treadmill belt speed.

Kinematic data were collected at 60 Hz using a 24-camera motion capture system (Vicon Motion Systems, Oxford, UK) and 57 reflective markers (Wilken et al., 2012). These kinematic data were post-processed using Vicon Nexus and Visual3D (C-Motion Inc., Germantown, MD). For the analyses presented here, only foot and pelvis marker data were used.

Heel strikes were determined using a velocity-based detection algorithm [82]. To increase precision of these calculations, raw marker data were first interpolated from 60 Hz to 600 Hz using a piecewise cubic spline interpolation [40, 47]. For each step, lateral foot placements (zL and zR) were taken as the lateral location of the heel marker at that step (Fig 1C). From these foot placements, body position (zB), heading (ΔzB), and step width (w) were computed using Eqs (2)(4) (Fig 1C). For consistency, only the first 290 consecutive steps from each trial (e.g., Fig 2A) were analyzed.

Uni-objective stochastic optimal regulator models

We consider three potential stepping control strategies (Fig 1C): while walking, try to maintain approximately constant absolute position (zB) of the body on the path, constant lateral speed or heading (ΔzB), or constant step width (w). Each of these strategies yields a goal function [6, 7, 12] in each variable separately, each having the same form (see also Eq (6)): (12) where q ∈ {zB, ΔzB, w}. The relevant step-to-step dynamics of qn are then modeled as a 1-D map (see also Eq (7)): (13) where q is the controller state being regulated and uq(qn) is the control input to be derived. Here, νm and νa are each independent random variables with zero mean and unit variance that represent multiplicative (νm) and additive (νa) noise, respectively. The σm and σa terms then give the standard deviations of each noise term.

Similar to our prior work [9, 10], this state update equation (Eq (13)) models the discrete-time inter-step dynamics in q. As such, it represents a simple “control template” model that regulates noise-induced fluctuations away from perfect performance by adjusting qn at each consecutive step. The choice of possible states to control (q ∈ {zB, ΔzB, w}; Fig 1) was motivated by the fact that any biped (human, robot, model, etc.) must generate some sequence of these variables to be “walking”. We note that in the absence of noise and control input, successive strides simply repeat (i.e., qn+1 = qn), which is consistent with well-established notions of limit cycle behavior (e.g., [16, 46, 75]) of walking. Thus, many reasonable models of continuous-time within-step walking dynamics, whether relatively very simple (e.g., [75, 76]), or highly complex (e.g., [78]) could be used to generate all of the relevant step-to-step time series (zBn, ΔzBn, wn, zLn, zRn) analyzed here.

Given Eqs (12) and (13), we then follow the same process as described in [9] to derive the control input, uq(qn). As in prior work [9, 10, 12], these controllers were modeled as stochastic optimal single-step controllers with direct error feedback, based on the Minimum Intervention Principle (MIP) [3, 4]. These controllers are optimal with respect to the expected value of the following cost function: (14) in which the first term penalized the error with respect to the goal function (Eq (12)) at the next step (i.e., the deviation of Fq from 0 at step n + 1) and the second term penalized “effort” in terms of the magnitude of the control input (uq) used to drive the state from step n to step n+1 [9]. Here, α and γ were positive constants that weighted the different components in the total cost, C. The form of Eq (14) is sufficiently general to include all cases studied in this work.

To derive uq(qn), we first substitute for e using Fq from Eq (12) into Eq (14): (15)

We then replace qn+1 with the right hand side of the state update map, Eq (13) to give: (16)

We next expand Eq (16) and gather like terms in and uq to obtain: (17)

We then take the expected value of C (i.e., ), noting that the noise processes have zero mean, unit variance, and are uncorrelated. That is, we set:

This then gives: (18)

In the general case, defines the Lagrangian, Λ, for this system. The optimal controller then extremizes . We thus differentiate Eq (18) with respect to uq, set the resulting expression equal to zero, and solve the resulting algebraic equation for our final controller, uq (see also Eq (8)): (19) (20) where the term in brackets, [•], represents the effective gain of the controller that defines the amount of the error, (qnq*), to be corrected at each step (see S1 Appendix for additional details). For each q ∈ {zB, ΔzB, w}, these models (Eqs (13) and (20)) were used to simulate the respective qn time series. For each such simulated qn time series, the relationships between each qn and the other stepping variables (i.e., Eqs (2)(4); Fig 1C) were then used to generate all of the relevant step-to-step time series (zBn, ΔzBn, wn, zLn, zRn) analyzed for each simulation.

Unlike our previous models for sagittal plane stepping regulation [9, 10], the controllers derived here (Eq (20)) were not “unbiased”, meaning they were not required to exhibit perfect performance on average. However, it remains relevant to derive the corresponding optimal control input for the more restrictive unbiased case. Requiring uq(qn) to be unbiased would mean additionally requiring that the expected value of the goal function, Fq, at step n+1 be zero [9]: (21)

Substituting for qn+1 in F and taking expected values, this additional constraint becomes: (22)

Adding this additional constraint to our controller yields the augmented Lagrangian, Λ, as: (23) where μ is a Lagrange multiplier. The optimal controllers again extremize Λ. We thus make the appropriate substitutions in Eq (23), differentiate Λ, and set the resulting expression equal to zero to obtain: (24)

Typically, Eqs (22) and (24) would be used together to solve for both uq and μ [9, 12]. Here, as we are interested only in uq, we obtain the unbiased stochastic optimal uq directly from Eq (22) as: (25)

Thus, this unbiased controller reduces to simple direct proportional feedback control. Alternatively, we could also obtain Eq (25) directly from Eq (20) in the special limiting case of assuming both no multiplicative noise and no cost for control effort (σm = γ = 0).

In particular, we note that for the case of q = w, Eq (25) is the same direct step width controller postulated by Kuo [16]. The present work thus extends that work in at least 3 important ways. First, all controllers considered here were derived from a stochastic optimal control framework, not proposed by conjecture. Second, these derived more general control policies (Eqs (8) and (11)) allow us to consider stochastically optimal controllers in cases less restrictive than the unbiased case. Third, they also allow us to consider stochastic control of other potentially relevant stepping variables beyond just step width.

Multi-objective stochastic optimal regulator models

We consider strategies that control two controller states (q1 & q2) simultaneously, with the goal to drive each variable to its own desired value. Thus, the generic dual-objective goal function has the form of Eq (9), with the expanded two-state update map of Eq (10). We then take the cost function for regulating two variables to be the sum of those used for each single variable: (26)

The subsequent steps in the derivation are then the same as those described above for the single variable case, as outlined in Eqs (15)(20). Here, this yields the two-variable control law of Eq (11). We considered three different specific versions of this general model, corresponding to all the possible combinations of any two of the three potential control state variables: i.e., [q1, q2] ∈ {[zB, ΔzB], [ΔzB, w], [zB, w]}.

We note that the goal function of Eq (9) indicates that each error, ei, in Eq (26) will depend only on the goal level error for its own corresponding control state variable, qi. Likewise, Eq (26) contains no cross terms in either the goal-level errors (e1, e2) or controller inputs (u1, u2). Hence, the final derived control law of Eq (11) and corresponding final state update equation of Eq (10) are both fully diagonal, such that the fluctuation dynamics of q1 and q2 are uncoupled. While it is certainly possible to imagine controller templates that include interactions between q1 and q2, we found in practice that the more parsimonious forms of Eqs (9) and (26) were adequate for our analyses.

Simulations of lateral stepping

In total, we generated simulations for three sets of each of three models. We first assessed the original (unconstrained) uni-objective models for each of q ∈ {zB, ΔzB, w}, for which we imposed no additional task constraints. We assessed each control strategy across a range of parameter values of each model. We then compared the dynamical behavior predicted by each set of simulations directly to experimental values (i.e., Fig 2) to determine the likelihood humans might have adopted any of these uni-objective control strategies. Full details of these simulations and the parameter sensitivity analyses conducted on these models and the results of those analyses are in S1 Appendix.

All of the unconstrained uni-objective models failed to capture human stepping dynamics (S1 Appendix). They did so mainly by either exceeding lateral boundary limits (i.e., they walked off the path) and/or by taking steps that were unrealistically too wide or too narrow.

Because none of the unconstrained uni-objective models captured human stepping dynamics, we then assessed corresponding “constrained” uni-objective models for each of q ∈ {zB, ΔzB, w}. In those simulations we additionally imposed physiologically relevant constraints that the simulations could not take steps that would walk off the path or would yield step widths unrealistically too wide or too narrow. We assessed each of these constrained control strategies across a range of parameter values of each model. We then compared the dynamical behavior predicted by each set of these simulations directly to human experimental values (i.e., Fig 2). Full details of these simulations and the parameter sensitivity analyses conducted on these models and the results of those analyses are in S1 Appendix.

Finally, we assessed each of the three 2-variable multi-objective models (i.e., [q1, q2] ∈ {[zB, ΔzB], [ΔzB, w], [zB, w]}). We assessed each of these control strategies across a range of parameter values of each model. We then compared the dynamical behavior predicted by each set of these simulations directly to experimental values (i.e., Fig 2) to determine the likelihood humans might have adopted any of these multi-objective control strategies. Full details of these simulations and the parameter sensitivity analyses conducted on these models and the results of those analyses are in S2 Appendix.

Calculation of dependent measures

The primary gait variables obtained from each simulated or experimental walking trial consisted of time series of left and right lateral foot placements (zL & zR), lateral body position and heading (zB & ΔzB), and step width (w) (Figs 1C and 2). We then analyzed these times series from both experimental and simulated walking trials in the same ways for each to make direct comparisons between simulation predictions and experimental findings.

First, we computed standard deviations of each relevant time series to quantify the magnitudes of the variance in them. For the computational models, variance in the respective variable(s) being regulated reflected the “goal relevant” variance [6, 7] because fluctuations in each respective controlled qn directly reflected deviations away from the corresponding desired q*. Conversely, any variance in those variables not being directly controlled was “goal equivalent” [6, 7] because fluctuations in those variables did not directly affect the designated task goal(s).

Standard deviations, however, quantify only the average magnitude of differences across all steps, regardless of temporal order. They thus yield no information about how each step affects subsequent steps. Therefore, here we used Detrended Fluctuation Analysis (DFA) to define a convenient lag-independent measure of statistical persistence across successive steps in each time series [9, 12, 71]. In brief, DFA calculates a scaling exponent, α, that quantifies the degree of statistical persistence or anti-persistence in a time series. To compute these exponents, each time series x(n), where n ∈ {1, …, N } steps [35], was first integrated to form a cumulative sum: (27) where x(n) is the value of x for the nth step and is the mean value of x across all N strides. Each integrated series was divided into equal, non-overlapping segments of length j. Each segment was detrended by subtracting a least squares linear fit to that segment. The squares of the residuals were then averaged over the entire data set and the square root of the mean residual, F(j), was calculated: (28)

This process was repeated for different segment lengths, j. Here, fifty values of j evenly distributed between 4 and N/4 were used. Typically, F(j) increases with j and a graph of log[F(j)] versus log(j) will exhibit a power-law relationship indicating the presence of scaling, such that F(j) ≈ jα. These log[F(j)] versus log(j) plots were then fit with a linear function using least squares regression. The slope of this line defined the scaling exponent α [9, 12, 71].

Standard deviation calculations quantify only average magnitudes of fluctuations and DFA α calculations quantify only the degree to which, on average, these deviations are corrected in time (from step-to-step). Neither analysis quantifies how step-to-step corrections of fluctuations might depend nonlinearly on the initial magnitudes of those fluctuations. Thus here, we directly quantified the degree to which deviations, , from the mean value,, of a given time series were corrected on the subsequent step by corresponding changes, Δqn+1 = qn+1qn, in the opposite direction [10]. We performed these analyses on each relevant time series obtained from both our human subjects (Fig 2) and from each of our final multi-objective position-step width control models (Fig 5). For each relevant time series, q ∈ {zB, ΔzB, w}, we constructed plots of Δqn+1 vs. q'n (Fig 6A). We then computed the linear slopes (using least-squares regression) and strength of correlation (r2) [10] for each corresponding relationship (Fig 6B and 6C).

Statistical comparisons

The primary comparisons of interest were between the model predictions and experimental observations (Fig 2). Here however, because the simulated and experimental data were structured in very different ways, standard inferential statistical tests (e.g., t-test, ANOVA, etc.) would not be appropriate. Additionally, because for the models, we could simulate as many trials as we wanted and of any arbitrary length, we could thus ensure p-values for most any comparisons that could be arbitrarily small and thus would not be meaningful. Therefore, to present descriptive data (Figs 2, 3, 4 & 7), for each relevant measure, data were plotted as boxplots indicating the median, 1st and 3rd quartiles, with whiskers extending to 1.5× the inter-quartile range and any values beyond that range indicated by individual data points. To directly compare model predictions to human experimental results (Figs 5 and 6), for each relevant measure, the expected range of values exhibited by humans was taken as the mean ± 1 standard deviation for the experimental data set. Model predictions are then shown as the mean ± 95% confidence interval for that mean for each set of corresponding simulations. We then infer that whenever the mean simulation prediction for a given measure lies within the experimental mean ± 1 SD band, that computational prediction is statistically consistent with experimental findings [10].

Data

All relevant experimental data are available from Dryad (https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.p254480) [83].

Supporting information

S1 Appendix. Uni‐objective models: Results of parameter sensitivity analyses.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006850.s001

(PDF)

S2 Appendix. Multi‐objective models: Results of parameter sensitivity analyses.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006850.s002

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Dr. Jason M. Wilken, Dr. Rainer Beurskens, and Mr. Eduardo J. Beltran for their assistance with data collection. The authors thank Dr. Jason M. Wilken and Dr. Jonathan H. Rylander for their assistance with data processing and initial analyses.

References

  1. 1. Stein RB, Gossen ER, Jones KE. Neuronal Variability: Noise or Part of the Signal? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6(5):389–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1668 pmid:15861181
  2. 2. Faisal AA, Selen LPJ, Wolpert DM. Noise in the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(4):292–303. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2258 pmid:18319728
  3. 3. Todorov E, Jordan MI. Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5(11):1226–35. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn963 pmid:12404008
  4. 4. Scott SH. Optimal Feedback Control and the Neural Basis of Volitional Motor Control. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5(7):532–46. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1427 pmid:15208695
  5. 5. Cohn BA, Szedlák M, Gärtner B, Valero-Cuevas FJ. Feasibility Theory Reconciles and Informs Alternative Approaches to Neuromuscular Control. Front Comput Neurosci. 2018;12:62. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2018.00062 pmid:30254579
  6. 6. Cusumano JP, Cesari P. Body-Goal Variability Mapping in an Aiming Task. Biol Cybern. 2006;94(5):367–79. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00422-006-0052-1 pmid:16501988
  7. 7. Cusumano JP, Dingwell JB. Movement Variability Near Goal Equivalent Manifolds: Fluctuations, Control, and Model-Based Analysis. Hum Mov Sci. 2013;32(5):899–923. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2013.07.019 pmid:24210574
  8. 8. Cusumano JP, Mahoney JM, Dingwell JB. The Dynamical Analysis of Inter-Trial Fluctuations Near Goal Equivalent Manifolds. Adv Exp Med Biol. 2014;826:125–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-1338-1_9 pmid:25330889
  9. 9. Dingwell JB, John J, Cusumano JP. Do Humans Optimally Exploit Redundancy to Control Step Variability in Walking? PLoS Comput Biol. 2010;6(7):e1000856. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000856 pmid:20657664
  10. 10. Dingwell JB, Cusumano JP. Identifying Stride-To-Stride Control Strategies in Human Treadmill Walking. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(4):e0124879. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124879 pmid:25910253
  11. 11. van Beers RJ, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ. Random walk of motor planning in task-irrelevant dimensions. J Neurophysiol. 2013;109(4):969–77. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00706.2012 pmid:23175799
  12. 12. John J, Dingwell JB, Cusumano JP. Error Correction and the Structure of Inter-Trial Fluctuations in a Redundant Movement Task. PLoS Comput Biol. 2016;12(9):e1005118. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005118 pmid:27643895
  13. 13. Winter DA. Kinematic and Kinetic Patterns in Human Gait: Variability and Compensating Effects. Hum Mov Sci. 1984;3(1–2):51–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-9457(84)90005-8.
  14. 14. Kang HG, Dingwell JB. Separating the Effects of Age and Speed on Gait Variability During Treadmill Walking. Gait Posture. 2008;27(4):572–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.07.009 pmid:17768055
  15. 15. Dingwell JB, Salinas MM, Cusumano JP. Increased Gait Variability May Not Imply Impaired Stride-To-Stride Control of Walking in Healthy Older Adults. Gait Posture. 2017;55:131–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.03.018 pmid:28454071
  16. 16. Kuo AD. Stabilization of Lateral Motion in Passive Dynamic Walking. Int J Robotics Res. 1999;18(9):917–30. https://doi.org/10.1177/02783649922066655.
  17. 17. Bauby CE, Kuo AD. Active Control of Lateral Balance in Human Walking. J Biomech. 2000;33(11):1433–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9290(00)00101-9. pmid:10940402
  18. 18. Hof AL. The ‘extrapolated center of mass’ concept suggests a simple control of balance in walking. Hum Mov Sci. 2008;27(1):112–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2007.08.003 pmid:17935808
  19. 19. Parkkari J, Kannus P, Palvanen M, Natri A, Vainio J, Aho H, et al. Majority of Hip Fractures Occur as a Result of a Fall and Impact on the Greater Trochanter of the Femur: A Prospective Controlled Hip Fracture Study with 206 Consecutive Patients. Calcif Tissue Int. 1999;65(3):183–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s002239900679. pmid:10441647
  20. 20. Hilliard MJ, Martinez KM, Janssen I, Edwards B, Mille M-L, Zhang Y, et al. Lateral Balance Factors Predict Future Falls in Community-Living Older Adults. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2008;89(9):1708–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2008.01.023 pmid:18760155
  21. 21. Donelan JM, Shipman DW, Kram R, Kuo AD. Mechanical and metabolic requirements for active lateral stabilization in human walking. J Biomech. 2004;37(6):827–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2003.06.002 pmid:15111070
  22. 22. Dean JC, Alexander NB, Kuo AD. The effect of lateral stabilization on walking in young and old adults. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 2007;54(11):1919–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/TBME.2007.901031 pmid:18018687
  23. 23. McAndrew PM, Wilken JM, Dingwell JB. Dynamic Stability of Human Walking in Visually and Mechanically Destabilizing Environments. J Biomech. 2011;44(4):644–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2010.11.007 pmid:21094944
  24. 24. MacKinnon CD, Winter DA. Control of Whole Body Balance In The Frontal Plane During Human Walking. J Biomech. 1993;26(6):633–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/0021-9290(93)90027-C. pmid:8514809
  25. 25. Bruijn SM, van Dieën JH. Control of human gait stability through foot placement. J R Soc Interface. 2018;15(143):1–11. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2017.0816.
  26. 26. Maeda RS, O’Connor SM, Donelan JM, Marigold DS. Foot placement relies on state estimation during visually guided walking. J Neurophysiol. 2017;117(2):480–91. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00015.2016 pmid:27760813
  27. 27. Matthis JS, Barton SL, Fajen BR. The critical phase for visual control of human walking over complex terrain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2017;114(32):E6720–E9. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1611699114 pmid:28739912
  28. 28. Matthis JS, Yates JL, Hayhoe MM. Gaze and the Control of Foot Placement When Walking in Natural Terrain. Curr Biol. 2018;28(8):1224–33.e5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.03.008 pmid:29657116
  29. 29. Acasio J, Wu MM, Fey NP, Gordon KE. Stability-maneuverability trade-offs during lateral steps. Gait Posture. 2017;52:171–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.11.034 pmid:27915220
  30. 30. Hsieh KL, Sheehan RC, Wilken JM, Dingwell JB. Healthy individuals are more maneuverable when walking slower while navigating a virtual obstacle course. Gait Posture. 2018;61:466–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2018.02.015 pmid:29494819
  31. 31. Wu MM, Brown G, Gordon KE. Control of Locomotor Stability in Stabilizing and Destabilizing Environments. Gait Posture. 2017;55(Supplement C):191–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.04.021.
  32. 32. Rankin BL, Buffo SK, Dean JC. A Neuromechanical Strategy for Mediolateral Foot Placement in Walking Humans. J Neurophysiol. 2014;112(2):374–83. Epub April 30, 2014. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00138.2014 pmid:24790168
  33. 33. Wang Y, Srinivasan M. Stepping in the direction of the fall: the next foot placement can be predicted from current upper body state in steady-state walking. Biol Lett. 2014;10(9):20140405. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2014.0405 pmid:25252834
  34. 34. Perry JA, Srinivasan M. Walking with wider steps changes foot placement control, increases kinematic variability and does not improve linear stability. Royal Society Open Science. 2017;4(9). https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.160627.
  35. 35. Stimpson KH, Heitkamp LN, Horne JS, Dean JC. Effects of walking speed on the step-by-step control of step width. J Biomech. 2018;68:78–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2017.12.026 pmid:29306549
  36. 36. Arvin M, Hoozemans MJM, Pijnappels M, Duysens J, Verschueren SM, van Dieën JH. Where to Step? Contributions of Stance Leg Muscle Spindle Afference to Planning of Mediolateral Foot Placement for Balance Control in Young and Old Adults. Frontiers in Physiology. 2018;9:1134. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2018.01134 pmid:30246780
  37. 37. Dean JC, Kautz SA. Foot Placement Control and Gait Instability Among People with Stroke. J Rehabil Res Develop. 2015;52(5):577–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1682/JRRD.2014.09.0207.
  38. 38. Chapman GJ, Hollands MA. Evidence that older adult fallers prioritise the planning of future stepping actions over the accurate execution of ongoing steps during complex locomotor tasks. Gait Posture. 2007;26(1):59–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.07.010 pmid:16939711
  39. 39. Patla AE, Vickers JN. How far ahead do we look when required to step on specific locations in the travel path during locomotion? Exp Brain Res. 2003;148(1):133–8. pmid:12478404
  40. 40. Bohnsack-McLagan NK, Cusumano JP, Dingwell JB. Adaptability of Stride-To-Stride Control of Stepping Movements in Human Walking. J Biomech. 2016;49(2):229–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2015.12.010 pmid:26725217
  41. 41. Townsend MA. Biped gait stabilization via foot placement. J Biomech. 1985;18(1):21–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0021-9290(85)90042-9. pmid:3980486
  42. 42. Redfern MS, Schumann T. A model of foot placement during gait. J Biomech. 1994;27(11):1339–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0021-9290(94)90043-4. pmid:7798284
  43. 43. Hof AL, Gazendam MGJ, Sinke WE. The condition for dynamic stability. J Biomech. 2005;38(1):1–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2004.03.025 pmid:15519333
  44. 44. Koolen T, de Boer T, Rebula J, Goswami A, Pratt J. Capturability-based analysis and control of legged locomotion, Part 1: Theory and application to three simple gait models. Int J Robotics Res. 2012;31(9):1094–113. https://doi.org/10.1177/0278364912452673.
  45. 45. Zhang L, Fu C. Predicting foot placement for balance through a simple model with swing leg dynamics. J Biomech. 2018;77:155–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2018.07.006 pmid:30029774
  46. 46. Hobbelen DGE, Wisse M. Active Lateral Foot Placement for 3D Stabilization of a Limit Cycle Walker Prototype. Int J Hum Robot. 2009;6(1):93–116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1142/S0219843609001632.
  47. 47. Dingwell JB, Bohnsack-McLagan NK, Cusumano JP. Humans Control Stride-To-Stride Stepping Movements Differently for Walking and Running, Independent of Speed. J Biomech. 2018;76:144–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2018.05.034 pmid:29914740
  48. 48. Decker LM, Cignetti F, Potter JF, Studenski SA, Stergiou N. Use of Motor Abundance in Young and Older Adults during Dual-Task Treadmill Walking. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(7):e41306. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0041306 pmid:22911777
  49. 49. Terrier P, Dériaz O. Persistent and anti-persistent pattern in stride-to-stride variability of treadmill walking: Influence of rhythmic auditory cueing. Hum Mov Sci. 2012;31(6):1585–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2012.05.004 pmid:23164626
  50. 50. Roerdink M, Daffertshofer A, Marmelat V, Beek PJ. How to Sync to the Beat of a Persistent Fractal Metronome without Falling Off the Treadmill? PLoS ONE. 2015;10(7):e0134148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134148 pmid:26230254
  51. 51. Orendurff MS, Schoen JA, Bernatz GC, Segal AD, Klute GK. How Humans Walk: Bout Duration, Steps per Bout, and Rest Duration. J Rehabil Res Develop. 2008;45(7):1077–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1682/JRRD.2007.11.0197.
  52. 52. Moussaïd M, Guillot EG, Moreau M, Fehrenbach J, Chabiron O, Lemercier S, et al. Traffic Instabilities in Self-Organized Pedestrian Crowds. PLoS Comput Biol. 2012;8(3):e1002442. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002442 pmid:22457615
  53. 53. Moussaïd M, Helbing D, Theraulaz G. How simple rules determine pedestrian behavior and crowd disasters. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108(17):6884–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1016507108 pmid:21502518
  54. 54. Degond P, Appert-Rolland C, Moussaïd M, Pettré J, Theraulaz G. A Hierarchy of Heuristic-Based Models of Crowd Dynamics. J Stat Phys. 2013;152(6):1033–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10955-013-0805-x.
  55. 55. Seitz MJ, Dietrich F, Köster G. The effect of stepping on pedestrian trajectories. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications. 2015;421:594–604. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physa.2014.11.064.
  56. 56. Terrier P. Fractal Fluctuations in Human Walking: Comparison Between Auditory and Visually Guided Stepping. Ann Biomed Eng. 2016;44(9):2785–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10439-016-1573-y pmid:26903091
  57. 57. Full RJ, Koditschek DE. Templates and Anchors: Neuromechanical Hypothesis of Legged Locomotion on Land. J Exp Biol. 1999;202(23):3325–32.
  58. 58. Dickinson MH, Farley CT, Full RJ, Koehl MA, Kram R, Lehman S. How Animals Move: An Integrative View. Science. 2000;Apr 7; 288(5463):100–6. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.288.5463.100. pmid:10753108
  59. 59. Holmes P, Full RJ, Koditschek D, Guckenheimer J. The Dynamics of Legged Locomotion: Models, Analyses, and Challenges. SIAM Rev. 2006;48(2):207–304. http://dx.doi.org/10.1137/S0036144504445133.
  60. 60. O’Connor SM, Kuo AD. Direction-Dependent Control of Balance During Walking and Standing. J Neurophysiol. 2009;102(3):1411–9. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00131.2009 pmid:19553493
  61. 61. Kannus P, Leiponen P, Parkkari J, Palvanen M, Järvinen M. A sideways fall and hip fracture. Bone. 2006;39(2):383–4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bone.2006.01.148 pmid:16504612
  62. 62. Donelan JM, Kram R, Kuo AD. Mechanical and metabolic determinants of the preferred step width in human walking. Proc Roy Soc B Biol Sci. 2001;268(1480):1985–92. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2001.1761.
  63. 63. Ankarali MM, Sefati S, Madhav MS, Long A, Bastian AJ, Cowan NJ. Walking dynamics are symmetric (enough). J R Soc Interface. 2015;12(108):20150209. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2015.0209 pmid:26236826
  64. 64. Malone LA, Bastian AJ, Torres-Oviedo G. How does the motor system correct for errors in time and space during locomotor adaptation? J Neurophysiol. 2012;108(2):672–83. pmid:22514294
  65. 65. Balasubramanian CK, Neptune RR, Kautz SA. Foot placement in a body reference frame during walking and its relationship to hemiparetic walking performance. Clin Biomech. 2010;25(5):483–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinbiomech.2010.02.003.
  66. 66. Warren WH, Kay BA, Zosh WD, Duchon AP, Sahuc S. Optic Flow is Used to Control Human Walking. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4(2):213–6. https://doi.org/10.1038/84054 pmid:11175884
  67. 67. Morrone MC, Tosetti M, Montanaro D, Fiorentini A, Cioni G, Burr DC. A Cortical Area That Responds Specifically to Optic Flow, Revealed by fMRI. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3(12):1322–8. https://doi.org/10.1038/81860 pmid:11100154
  68. 68. McAndrew Young PM, Wilken JM, Dingwell JB. Dynamic margins of stability during human walking in destabilizing environments. J Biomech. 2012;45(6):1053–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2011.12.027 pmid:22326059
  69. 69. Beurskens R, Wilken JM, Dingwell JB. Dynamic Stability of Individuals with Transtibial Amputation Walking in Destabilizing Environments. J Biomech. 2014;47(7):1675–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2014.02.033 pmid:24679710
  70. 70. Beltran EJ, Dingwell JB, Wilken JM. Margins of stability in young adults with traumatic transtibial amputation walking in destabilizing environments. J Biomech. 2014;47(5):1138–43. Epub Dec. 26, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2013.12.011 pmid:24444777
  71. 71. Dingwell JB, Cusumano JP. Re-Interpreting Detrended Fluctuation Analyses of Stride-To-Stride Variability in Human Walking. Gait Posture. 2010;32(3):348–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2010.06.004 pmid:20605097
  72. 72. Sjoberg EA. Logical fallacies in animal model research. Behavioral and Brain Functions. 2017;13(1):3. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12993-017-0121-8 pmid:28202023
  73. 73. McAndrew Young PM, Dingwell JB. Voluntarily changing step length or step width affects dynamic stability of human walking. Gait Posture. 2012;35(3):472–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.11.010 pmid:22172233
  74. 74. Dingwell JB, Marin LC. Kinematic Variability and Local Dynamic Stability of Upper Body Motions When Walking at Different Speeds. J Biomech. 2006;39(3):444–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2004.12.014 pmid:16389084
  75. 75. Srinivasan M, Ruina A. Computer optimization of a minimal biped model discovers walking and running. Nature. 2006;439(7072):72–5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature04113 pmid:16155564
  76. 76. Geyer H, Seyfarth A, Blickhan R. Compliant leg behaviour explains basic dynamics of walking and running. Proc Roy Soc B Biol Sci. 2006;273(1603):2861–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2006.3637.
  77. 77. Gan Z, Yesilevskiy Y, Zaytsev P, Remy CD. All common bipedal gaits emerge from a single passive model. J R Soc Interface. 2018;15(146). https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2018.0455.
  78. 78. Manoonpong P, Geng T, Kulvicius T, Porr B, Wörgötter F. Adaptive, Fast Walking in a Biped Robot under Neuronal Control and Learning. PLoS Comput Biol. 2007;3(7):e134. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.0030134 pmid:17630828
  79. 79. Song S, Geyer H. A neural circuitry that emphasizes spinal feedback generates diverse behaviours of human locomotion. J Physiol. 2015;593(16):3493–511. https://doi.org/10.1113/JP270228 pmid:25920414
  80. 80. Reimann H, Fettrow T, Thompson ED, Jeka JJ. Neural Control of Balance During Walking. Frontiers in Physiology. 2018;9:1271. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2018.01271 pmid:30271354
  81. 81. Reimann H, Fettrow TD, Thompson ED, Agada P, McFadyen BJ, Jeka JJ. Complementary mechanisms for upright balance during walking. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(2):e0172215. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172215 pmid:28234936
  82. 82. Zeni JA, Richards JG, Higginson JS. Two simple methods for determining gait events during treadmill and overground walking using kinematic data. Gait Posture. 2008;27(4):710–4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.07.007 pmid:17723303
  83. 83. Dingwell JB, Cusumano JP. Data From: Humans Use Multi-Objective Control to Regulate Lateral Foot Placement When Walking. Dryad Digital Repository. 2019. https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.p254480.