ALL Metrics
-
Views
-
Downloads
Get PDF
Get XML
Cite
Export
Track
Research Article
Revised

Kairomonal communication in mice is concentration-dependent with a proportional discrimination threshold

[version 2; peer review: 3 approved]
PUBLISHED 09 Dec 2013
Author details Author details
OPEN PEER REVIEW
REVIEWER STATUS

Abstract

Odors of predators are often co-opted by prey species to serve as warning signals. Perceptual properties of such kairomonal communication are under studied despite their common use in many mammals. We demonstrate that the kairomonal response in mice to rat odors varies monotonically with the volume of rat odor. Moreover, the ability of mice to differentiate between two strengths of rat odors is dependent on the ratio of the two concentrations. These results show that mice can compare kairomonal strength over a large range of values, and that kairomonal communication follows Weber’s law.

Keywords

Major urinary proteins; Olfaction; Pheromone; Predator; Rat; Weber’s law

Revised Amendments from Version 1

The main modifications to the text based on comments from the referees are to the materials and methods section of the paper. In response to referee 1, we have clarified the way in which the various urine doses were prepared. In response to referee 3, we have made additions to the methods that clarify the procedure of how we ensured constant volume of the urine stimuli presented to the animals. In addition, we have provided dimensions of the bisects of the arena that was used to define the area from which time spent near the rat urine stimuli was calculated. For referee 2, we have changed the phrase "detection threshold" to "discrimination threshold" in figure 2 legend and in the "Proportional discrimination threshold" section in the results. We have explained our rationale in the comments section.

See the authors' detailed response to the review by Fernando Martínez-García and Lluis Fortes-Marco
See the authors' detailed response to the review by Kazumi Osada
See the authors' detailed response to the review by Joanne Yew

Introduction

Foraging animals continually face a conflict between 1) the need to seek opportunities such as food and mating partners; and, 2) the need to avoid exposure to predators. In response to predation pressure, many prey species have co-opted predator odors as kairomones; chemicals emitted by one species, usually for inter-species communication, but intercepted by other species resulting in benefit for the receiver and detriment of the emitter. In this role, predator odors such as urine, fecal material or body odors initiate a rapid avoidance response in prey, thus reducing the probability of successful predation1,2. Such avoidance of predator cues needs to be ‘traded-off’ against foraging opportunities. In view of this, it can be speculated that a kairomonal responses may not be an absolute all-or-nothing phenomenon. Rather avoidance is expected to be relative to the intensity of the predator cue. Implicit in this speculation is the idea that animals can quantitatively perceive differences in kairomonal strength.

A wide variety of animals can make quantitative estimates of percepts such as time, foraging opportunities, efforts and rewards37. These quantitative estimates are often derived using a comparative representational system that is dependent on the ratios between opposing quantities37. The ability to make quantitative estimates is important because it allows calibration of behavioral responses to incipient environmental opportunities and challenges. In accordance with the comparative nature of such perceptual systems, it can be predicted that greater quantities of kairomones evoke greater response i.e. that the response is dose-dependent. More importantly, sensitivity to changes in the magnitude of a stimulus decreases when stimulus magnitude increases. In other words, the discrimination threshold (i.e. the ‘just-noticeable difference’ between two stimuli of different intensities) is smaller when both stimuli are weak compared to when both stimuli are strong. This formulation is often termed Weber’s law8, and is a fundamental property of many percepts.

Kairomonal communication has been widely studied in insects912. Additionally, the neurobiology and physiology of rodent kairomones has attracted significant scientific interest in the recent past1,13. Yet, the perceptual properties of kairomonal communication in mammals have so far been under studied, including, the dose-responsivity of kairomonal communication14 and the relationship of discrimination threshold to stimulus magnitude.

House mice (Mus musculus) are predated by rats (Rattus norvegicus)1517 and accordingly, the mice express innate avoidance to rat odors18,19. In this report, we investigate the dose-responsivity and discrimination threshold of kairomonal communication in mice.

Materials and methods

Animals

The Nanyang Technological University (IACUC number: ARF SBS/NIE-A-0106AZ) institutional animal care and use committee reviewed and approved all procedures. Fifteen male Balb/c mice (7–8 weeks old, housed five per cage (369 x 156 x 132 mm; 1145T, Tecniplast, UK)) were obtained from the vivarium of the National University of Singapore. Eight male Wistar rats (48 days old, housed two per cage (425 x 266 x 185 mm; 1291H, Tecniplast, UK)) were obtained from the same vivarium and used as a kairomonal source. Standard corn cob cage bedding was changed twice a week. Animals were maintained on a 12 hours light-dark cycle, with temperature and relative humidity ranging between 20–25 degree celsius and 70–80%, respectively. Experiments were conducted during the light phase. Food and water was available ad libitum. The diet consisted of standard laboratory chow (PicoLab Rodent Diet 20, 5053) with 20% protein content.

Kairomone collection

Rat urine was collected using metabolic cages (Harvard Apparatus). A single pool of rat urine was used for all subsequent experimentations. Rat urine contains volatile compounds and major urinary proteins (MUPs). The urine was treated with menadione (M5625 Sigma-Aldrich, Singapore) to competitively displace volatile compounds bound to the MUPs, followed by centrifugation (Millipore, 3000 g for 5 minutes) through a size-exclusion column (>3 kDa). Only the high molecular-weight fraction containing MUPs and devoid of volatiles was used, in accordance with the prior demonstration that rat MUPs serve as kairomones to mice13.

Dose-responsivity to kairomones

The response of mice (n = 10) to increasing doses of MUP fraction of rat urine ((henceforth referred to as rat urine) was studied (trial duration = 600s). Avoidance was quantified by comparing time spent by mice in two opposing bisects of an arena (76 × 9 cm; 15 cm high). The bisects were defined by a virtual division of the arena in two equal halves (38 × 9 cm), with exploration in either half being considered as time spent near the stimulus presented in that bisect. The stimuli were presented at the terminal end of the bisects. Data on time spent in each bisect was collected by automated behavioral tracking software (ANY-maze, version 4.3, Stoelting). Opposing arms contained either rat urine or phosphate-buffered saline. The amount of rat urine was systematically varied from 1X to 16X (3.125, 6.25, 12.5, 25 and 50 µl). X was arbitrarily defined as 3.125 µl of rat urine. The different stimuli were made by a twofold serial dilution to ensure that a constant volume was presented (50 µl) while the concentration varied. The stimuli was dotted (5 drops of 10 µl) on filter paper and positioned at the terminal ends of the two bisect. The animals had direct access to the stimuli. The same set of mice was used in successive testing for all doses (starting from lower to higher doses) with 24 hours elapsing between two successive trials).

Discrimination threshold

Both arms of the arena contained rat urine in this condition. The amount of rat urine in one arm was varied in five discrete doses (6.25–25 μl), equidistant on a Log2 scale. The stimuli was prepared in the similar way as before ensuring an equal volume was used, with only the concentration being varied. The opposing arm contained volume that was greater by ratio of either 1.2 or 1.3. The percentage of time that mice (n = 15, the same mice that were used in the previous experiment) spent in the arm with the greater volume of urine, was quantified. The same set of mice were used in successive testing for all doses (starting from lower to higher doses), with two successive trials (24 hours apart).

Statistics

All statistical tests were conducted using IBM SPSS software (version 20) One-way analysis of variance followed by ‘Fisher’s least significant difference’ (LSD) post-hoc test was used to analyze increasing doses of kairomones on mouse behaviour. A two-way analysis of variance was carried out to determine main effect and/or interaction of kairomone dose and its corresponding ratios.

Results

Dose responsivity of kairomonal communication

One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed that the amount of time spent near rat urine decreased with an incremental increase in the amount of rat urine (Figure 1; n = 10; F(4,45) = 6.9; p = 0.0002). Animals spent significantly more time near the weakest odor, compared to the strongest (LSD, Fisher's least significant difference, p = 0.0002). The stimulus-response curve exhibited a robust fit to sigmoidal curve (Figure 1; R2 > 0.99; p < 0.01), showing a monotonic linear response between concentrations 2X to 8X.

In order to rule out carry-over effects during repeated trials, we tested a separate set of five mice repeatedly at a singular dose (4X) over five days. This set of mice exhibited a comparable aversion to rat urine across all trials, showing a lack of habituation, sensitization or conditioning during repeated testing (one-way ANOVA; F(4,20) = 0.314, p > 0.8).

c8855275-4291-404e-8115-dd74f737d88e_figure1.gif

Figure 1. Kairomonal communication in mice is dose-dependent (mean±SEM).

Aversion of mice to increasing doses of rat urine was quantified by comparing the time spent in two opposing arms of an arena, with one arm containing incremental doses of rat urine and the other containing buffered saline (trial duration = 600 s, n = 10 mice). The graph depicts average time spent in rat urine arm. The gray line depicts sigmoidal fit. Abscissa depicts dose of rat urine employed (log2 scale; x arbitrarily set as 3.125 µl of rat urine).

Proportional discrimination threshold

The discrimination threshold (i.e the ‘just-noticeable difference’) was studied for five equidistant doses (Log2 scale) encompassing the linear part of the dose-response curve (Figure 2). One arm of the arena in this case contained the dose depicted in the abscissa. The discrimination around this dose was studied in two successive trials by providing a greater amount of kairomone in the opposing arm, differing by a ratio of either 1.2 or 1.3. A positive discrimination was noted as less time was spent in the arm containing the greater volume of urine.

c8855275-4291-404e-8115-dd74f737d88e_figure2.gif

Figure 2. Discrimination threshold in mice is proportional to kairomone strength (mean±SEM).

The discrimination threshold at varying doses of rat urine was further examined by setting up an avoidance-avoidance conflict, where mice chose to spend time in arms containing either lower or higher amounts of rat urine. The higher dose was of either a 20% (un-shaded bars) or 30% (shaded bars) greater magnitude (e.g. 120% or 130% of 2X). Abscissa depicts the lower dose used in each of the comparisons (e.g. 2X). Ordinate depicts time spent in arm with the greater amount of rat urine divided by the sum of the time spent in both arms (gray line = 50% chance). N = 15 mice for all comparisons.Log2 scale; arbitrarily set as 3.125 µl of rat urine.

A two-way ANOVA for dose and ratio revealed a significant main effect of the ratio (Figure 2A; F(1,138) = 32.1, p = 0.00000008). The main effect of doses themselves did not reach statistical significance (F(4,138) = 1.347, p = 0.256). Similarly, interaction between doses and ratios was not significant (F(4,138) = 1.214, p = 0.308). Thus, regardless of the dose studied, discrimination threshold was constantly proportional to the kairomone strength (Figure 2B) by a ratio ≤ 1.3 but above >1.2. In other words, the discrimination threshold was smaller for weaker stimuli and bigger for stronger stimuli.

Discussion

Kairomones are compounds emitted by one species and co-opted by another receiving species, resulting in benefit for the receiver and detriment for the emitter. Kairomones can be used by both predators to locate prey and by prey to secure advanced warning of predator presence. Odors used as pheromones in intra-species communication are often the most vulnerable for co-option as kairomones. This is because use in conspecific communication requires robust expression of odors, making them more liable for eavesdropping by other species (reviewed in Kolluru and Zuk20). In agreement with this formulation, rats use urine marks to communicate status and sexual attractiveness2123; and, proteins secreted with rat urine are sufficient to initiate innate avoidance in mice13, a prey species of rats1517. The salient kairomone in this case has been identified to be a major urinary protein, MUP1313. These involatile rodent kairomones provide a unique opportunity to study kairomonal perception by virtue of their stability in the ambient environment and due to their ease of reliably controlling their dosing. This is in contrast with the volatile nature of many kairomones that require onerous delivery methods using olfactometers.

Several pheromonal responses in insects and mammals show dose responsivity whereby a stronger pheromonal stimulus evokes a greater response (e.g. Coureaud et al., He et al. and Perna et al.2426). In contrast, the dose responsivity of kairomonal communication has not been well-studied. In this report, we demonstrate that mice perceive rat kairomones in a dose-dependent manner. We speculate that the relative nature of kairomonal communication permits prey to calibrate foraging responses according to perceived predatory threats. For example, a weak kairomonal stimulus might signal the passage of a long period of time since the urine mark was laid, and thereby evoke lesser avoidance. In contrast, a stronger odor is likely to be fresh and a better indicator of predator presence. Similar dose-responsivity has been previously described during the perception of cat odors by rats14.

The ability to calibrate avoidance also suggests that mice are able to differentiate between various amounts of kairomones. Animals can indeed discriminate between different amounts of many percepts, including olfactory sensations37. In the absence of an absolute numerical system, these discriminations are often dependant on a relative estimation based on comparative perceptions. Weber and Fletcher8 formalized one of the hallmarks of such relative estimation by showing that the discrimination threshold is a constant fraction of the stimulus intensity in many perceptual systems (k = ∆I/I; where k is a constant, I is intensity and ∆I is just-noticeable difference). In this report, we demonstrate that kairomonal communication in mice follows Weber’s law with Weber’s fraction valued at greater than 0.2 but smaller than 0.3. Weber’s law has been previously studied in human olfaction for volatile odors, yielding a comparable sensitivity of 0.287. Similarly, pheromonal communication in Argentine Ants (Linepithema humile) has been recently shown to follow Weber’s law26. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first demonstration of Weber’s law applied to kairomones.

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 2
VERSION 2 PUBLISHED 23 Sep 2013
Comment
Author details Author details
Competing interests
Grant information
Copyright
Download
 
Export To
metrics
Views Downloads
F1000Research - -
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
- -
Citations
CITE
how to cite this article
Vasudevan A and Vyas A. Kairomonal communication in mice is concentration-dependent with a proportional discrimination threshold [version 2; peer review: 3 approved] F1000Research 2013, 2:195 (https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.2-195.v2)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
track
receive updates on this article
Track an article to receive email alerts on any updates to this article.

Open Peer Review

Current Reviewer Status: ?
Key to Reviewer Statuses VIEW
ApprovedThe paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approvedFundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Version 2
VERSION 2
PUBLISHED 09 Dec 2013
Revised
Views
13
Cite
Reviewer Report 30 Dec 2013
Kazumi Osada, Division of Physiology, Health Sciences University of Hokkaido, Hokkaido, Japan 
Approved
VIEWS 13
Although I do not feel the paper is perfectly acceptable, I admit it has ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Osada K. Reviewer Report For: Kairomonal communication in mice is concentration-dependent with a proportional discrimination threshold [version 2; peer review: 3 approved]. F1000Research 2013, 2:195 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.3209.r2915)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
Views
21
Cite
Reviewer Report 10 Dec 2013
Joanne Yew, Temasek Life Sciences Laboratory, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore 
Approved
VIEWS 21
My concerns have been adequately addressed ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Yew J. Reviewer Report For: Kairomonal communication in mice is concentration-dependent with a proportional discrimination threshold [version 2; peer review: 3 approved]. F1000Research 2013, 2:195 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.3209.r2693)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
Views
16
Cite
Reviewer Report 10 Dec 2013
Fernando Martínez-García, Faculty of Biology, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain 
Lluis Fortes-Marco, Faculty of Biology, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain 
Approved
VIEWS 16
After the comments and amendments of the authors, it becomes now clear that they are testing the aversion of mice to different concentrations (rather than different volumes) of the high molecular weight fraction of rat urine, and they are doing ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Martínez-García F and Fortes-Marco L. Reviewer Report For: Kairomonal communication in mice is concentration-dependent with a proportional discrimination threshold [version 2; peer review: 3 approved]. F1000Research 2013, 2:195 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.3209.r2692)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
Version 1
VERSION 1
PUBLISHED 23 Sep 2013
Views
25
Cite
Reviewer Report 12 Nov 2013
Fernando Martínez-García, Faculty of Biology, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain 
Lluis Fortes-Marco, Faculty of Biology, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain 
Approved with Reservations
VIEWS 25
The manuscript by Vasudevan and Vyas explores the interesting hypothesis that the response of mice to urine-borne kairomones of rats is dose dependent according to the Weber’s law. To do so the authors basically analyse the avoidance of mice to ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Martínez-García F and Fortes-Marco L. Reviewer Report For: Kairomonal communication in mice is concentration-dependent with a proportional discrimination threshold [version 2; peer review: 3 approved]. F1000Research 2013, 2:195 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.2236.r2397)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 30 Nov 2013
    Ajai Vyas, School of Biological Sciences, Nanyang Technological University, 637551, Singapore
    30 Nov 2013
    Author Response
    We have attempted to address the concerns of referee #3 in second version of the manuscript. The referee’s comments are based on two strands of criticisms, both of which arise ... Continue reading
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 30 Nov 2013
    Ajai Vyas, School of Biological Sciences, Nanyang Technological University, 637551, Singapore
    30 Nov 2013
    Author Response
    We have attempted to address the concerns of referee #3 in second version of the manuscript. The referee’s comments are based on two strands of criticisms, both of which arise ... Continue reading
Views
57
Cite
Reviewer Report 25 Oct 2013
Kazumi Osada, Division of Physiology, Health Sciences University of Hokkaido, Hokkaido, Japan 
Not Approved
VIEWS 57
This paper showed the quantitative relationship between the amount of crude rat urinary kairomone fraction, and aversive behavior in mice. The figure 1 data is reasonable, but not interesting.

The figure 2 data fails to demonstrate the relationship between the amount ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Osada K. Reviewer Report For: Kairomonal communication in mice is concentration-dependent with a proportional discrimination threshold [version 2; peer review: 3 approved]. F1000Research 2013, 2:195 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.2236.r2187)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 30 Nov 2013
    Ajai Vyas, School of Biological Sciences, Nanyang Technological University, 637551, Singapore
    30 Nov 2013
    Author Response
    We believe revisions to the script address the main concerns raised by referee #2.
    The referee posits that creating an avoidance-avoidance conflict in figure two does not differentiate between possibilities that ... Continue reading
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 30 Nov 2013
    Ajai Vyas, School of Biological Sciences, Nanyang Technological University, 637551, Singapore
    30 Nov 2013
    Author Response
    We believe revisions to the script address the main concerns raised by referee #2.
    The referee posits that creating an avoidance-avoidance conflict in figure two does not differentiate between possibilities that ... Continue reading
Views
22
Cite
Reviewer Report 14 Oct 2013
Joanne Yew, Temasek Life Sciences Laboratory, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore 
Approved
VIEWS 22
This article addresses and characterizes rat urine kairomonal effects on mouse avoidance behavior. The study convincingly shows that 1) the effect is dose-dependent and 2) mice display proportional discrimination across several different concentrations.

The title of the study and abstract provide ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Yew J. Reviewer Report For: Kairomonal communication in mice is concentration-dependent with a proportional discrimination threshold [version 2; peer review: 3 approved]. F1000Research 2013, 2:195 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.2236.r1883)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 30 Nov 2013
    Ajai Vyas, School of Biological Sciences, Nanyang Technological University, 637551, Singapore
    30 Nov 2013
    Author Response
    The same urine sample was indeed used to create all doses during this experiment. The second version of the manuscript now clearly states this in material and methods section.
    Competing Interests: I declare that there are no competing interests that might be construed to influence my judgment of the article's or referee response's validity or importance
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 30 Nov 2013
    Ajai Vyas, School of Biological Sciences, Nanyang Technological University, 637551, Singapore
    30 Nov 2013
    Author Response
    The same urine sample was indeed used to create all doses during this experiment. The second version of the manuscript now clearly states this in material and methods section.
    Competing Interests: I declare that there are no competing interests that might be construed to influence my judgment of the article's or referee response's validity or importance

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 2
VERSION 2 PUBLISHED 23 Sep 2013
Comment
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Approved - the paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations - A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approved - fundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Sign In
If you've forgotten your password, please enter your email address below and we'll send you instructions on how to reset your password.

The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.

Email address not valid, please try again

You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.

You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.

Code not correct, please try again
Email us for further assistance.
Server error, please try again.