Skip to main content

Decreased functional connectivity between ventral tegmental area and nucleus accumbens in Internet gaming disorder: evidence from resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging

Abstract

Background

Internet gaming disorder (IGD) has become an increasing mental health problem worldwide. Decreased resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) between the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) has been found in substance use and is thought to play an important role in the development of substance addiction. However, rsFC between the VTA and NAcc in a non-substance addiction, such as IGD, has not been assessed previously. The current study aimed to investigate: (1) if individuals with IGD exhibit alterations in VTA-NAcc functional connectivity; and (2) whether VTA-NAcc functional connectivity is associated with subjective Internet craving.

Methods

Thirty-five male participants with IGD and 24 healthy control (HC) individuals participated in resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging. Regions of interest (left NAcc, right NAcc and VTA) were selected based on the literature and were defined by placing spheres centered on Talairach Daemon coordinates.

Results

In comparison with HCs, individuals with IGD had significantly decreased rsFC between the VTA and right NAcc. Resting-state functional connectivity strength between the VTA and right NAcc was negatively correlated with self-reported subjective craving for the Internet.

Conclusions

These results suggest possible neural functional similarities between individuals with IGD and individuals with substance addictions.

Background

Internet gaming disorder (IGD) is defined as a failure to control one’s impulses to excessively use online gaming. IGD results in serious negative outcomes, such as decreased physical and mental health, low academic achievement, and problems in interpersonal relationships [1, 2]. IGD is an increasing mental health problem worldwide [3], and has been highlighted as a topic deserving further study in Section III of the DSM-5 [4].

Resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) analyses of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data allow for the assessment of dynamic interactions between brain regions while the brain is ‘at rest’, and this methodology has increasingly been used to study the neurobiology of addictions [58]. Researchers have proposed that reduced mesolimbic rsFC may relate to impaired dopamine signaling which would result in a blunting of activity in regions within the reward pathway, or a reduced hedonic set point, among individuals with addictions [5, 9]. Dopaminergic (DAergic) reward pathways have been intensively studied and are associated with generation of craving and addictive behaviors [1013]. Tomasi and Volkow have suggested that ventral tegmental area (VTA) and substantia nigra (SN) connectivity may be a potential biomarker for DAergic dysfunction disorders [14]. Gu et al. reported decreased rsFC between the VTA and the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) in chronic cocaine users [5]. Dopamine neurons within the VTA project to the NAcc as part of the mesolimbic pathway and communication between these regions is essential for acute drug reward [13, 15] and implicated in incentive motivation [16], such as substance-related craving [1719]. In a study of adult smokers, Hong et al. found a weakened rsFC strength between the dorsal anterior cingulate and striatum [20].

Previous studies have demonstrated that IGD individuals have alterations in resting-state patterns of brain activity involving regions responsible for reward, impulse control and sensory-motor coordination, similar with neural mechanisms underlying drug abuse [21, 22]. Several lines of evidence implicated the dopamine system in IGD. For example, Han et al. found that the Taq1A1 allelic variant of the DRD2 gene is associated with excessive playing of Internet games [23]. Increased NAcc activity in response to gaming-related cues has been reported among individuals with IGD (compared to healthy controls), and is positively correlated with self-reported craving for Internet gaming [24]. Therefore, it is possible that IGD and craving for Internet gaming may be associated with alterations in NAcc–VTA functional connectivity, as has been reported among individuals with substance addictions.

This study had two primary aims: First, to examine and compare rsFC between the NAcc and VTA using seed-based functional connectivity analyses among IGD participants and healthy comparison (HCs) participants. Second, to explore the relationship between VTA and NAcc functional connectivity and the Internet gaming-related craving. Based on the previous studies discussed above, we hypothesized that rsFC between the NAcc and VTA would be decreased among IGD participants in comparison to HC participants. In addition, we hypothesized that connectivity strength would be negatively correlated with subjective craving for internet use.

Methods

Participants

Four hundred and thirty-two individuals were recruited from several universities in Beijing city by online advertisement. Given the increased incidence of IGD among men versus women [25], and in order to control for any possible gender effects on neural activity, only male participants were included in this study. Based on the inclusionary/exclusionary criteria described below, thirty-five IGD subjects and 24 HC subjects were selected to participate in this study. The Ethics Committee of State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University approved the study. All participants provided written informed consent for the study and were compensated for their participation.

Inclusionary criteria for IGD and HC participants

IGD participants had to meet the criteria of Internet addiction proposed by Ko and Chen [26], as assessed using the Chinese Internet Addiction Scale (CIAS) [27]. This scale consists of 26 self-report items associated with online Internet use problems, including items related to tolerance, withdrawal, compulsive Internet use, time management, interpersonal relationship problems and health problems. Each item uses a scale from ‘Strongly disagree’ (score = 1) to ‘Strongly agree’ (score = 4), and the CIAS total score is calculated by the sum of the items. The reliability and validity of the CIAS among college students has been demonstrated previously [27]. According to Ko et al. [26], people who have a score of 67 or greater meet criteria for Internet addiction. Other inclusion criteria for IGD participants were that their primary Internet activity was Internet gaming, that the majority of their time spent on the Internet (i.e., more than 50 % of hours of Internet use) involved Internet gaming [28, 29], and that the time spent on Internet gaming per week was greater than 14 h (i.e., average of more than 2 h per day) [30].

Inclusion criteria for HC participants were a score less than 60 on the CIAS (an optimal cutoff point for screening of Internet addiction is 63/64, according to Ko et al. [26]), and occasionally Internet gaming (or no lifetime gaming) over the history of their normal Internet use.

Exclusion criteria for all participants included a history of neurological or psychiatric disease including all other types of addiction, and current and past use of psychotropic medications. The Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence (FTND) [31] and the Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test (MAST) [32] were used to make sure all the participants were free of smoking and drinking problems. The Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) [33] and the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) [34] were used to assess the participants’ current anxiety and depression state. Participants with scores of 17 or more on the BAI and of 21 or more on the BDI were excluded [35].

Subjective craving for internet gaming

An eight-item Likert scale was adapted from the brief Questionnaire of Smoking urges (QSU-brief) [36]. All participants were asked to evaluate their craving for Internet use (for HCs) or online gaming (for IGD participants) on a 7-grade Likert scale, from “1 = I don’t want to at all” to “7 = I extremely want to”. A higher score on this scale indicates a higher craving for Internet use or online gaming. The questionnaire was administered immediately prior to scanning.

Data acquisition

Resting-state fMRI data were collected on a 3.0T Siemens Trio scanner at Beijing Normal University. Participants were instructed to keep their head motionless and eyes open and not to think about anything systematically during the scanning. A helmet and earplugs were also used to minimize head motion and machine noise. Scanning parameters were as follows: repetition time (TR) = 2000 ms, echo time (TE) = 30 ms, flip angle (FA) = 90°, thickness = 3.5 mm, field of view (FOV) = 192 mm × 192 mm, acquisition matrix = 64 × 64, voxel size = 3.1 × 3.1 × 3.5 mm3, gap = 0.7 mm, NIslice = 33, time point = 200.

Data processing

The software DPARSF Version 2.1 (Data Processing Assistant for Resting-state fMRI, http://www.restfmri.net) [37] was used to pre-process imaging data. Prior to pre-processing, the first 10 s of data were discarded for each participant due to shimming time. Slice timing correction, realignment and spatial normalization and smoothing (full-width-half-maximum, FWHM = 4 mm) were conducted. Nuisance covariates including cerebrospinal fluid signals, global mean signals, white matter signals, and six head motion parameters were regressed from the functional MR imaging data. Participants whose head motion translation values were more than 3.0 mm in any direction of x, y, z or rotation values more than 3° of any angle were excluded. The frame-wise displacement (FD) of head position [38] was not different between groups (t = 1.32, p = 0.192, Table 1). We applied a scrubbing regression to reduce the effects of head motion. Time points with FD of 0.2 or higher were assigned values of 0, and other time points were assigned values of 1. Scrubbing regression allows for the same number of time points across participants while controlling for excess head motion at specific time points [3941].

Table 1 Demographic characteristics and behavioral information for IGD and HC groups

REST version 1.8 (http://restfmri.net/forum/REST_V1.8) [42] was used for later data processing. Data were de-trended and filtered (0.01–0.08 Hz) to reduce both low-frequency drift and high-frequency physiologic noise.

Consistent with methods published by Gu et al. [5], regions of interest (ROIs) were defined by placing spheres centered on coordinates: left NAcc (Talairach Daemon coordinate: −12, 8, −8, 6 mm radius), right NAcc (Talairach Daemon coordinate: 12, 8, −8, 6 mm radius) and VTA (Talairach Daemon coordinate: 0, −16, −7, 3 mm radius) [15] (Fig. 1), which were provided by the Talairach Daemon database [43]. Then, rsFC between the three seeds was calculated by the Pearson’s correlation coefficient of each of the seed’s time courses; correlation coefficients had already been converted to Fisher’s Z values to promote the normality of data distribution.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Regions of interest (ROIs) of NAcc (± 12, 8, −8, 6 mm radius) and VTA (0, −16, −7, 3 mm radius) in Talairach Daemon coordinate

Data analysis

Group analysis

A two-sample t test was used to explore differences in the rsFC strength (Fisher Z value) between the three ROIs between the IGD and HC groups. In addition, we also compared the whole brain rsFC with each ROI as a seed, using alphasim correction with voxel-level p < 0.01 and cluster-level p < 0.05 was used (results shown in Additional file 1).

Association with clinical variables

Correlation analysis involving rsFC strength (Fisher Z value) and the score of CIAS/craving was performed to investigate the relationship between rsFC strength and severity of IGD/craving for the Internet (for HC participants) or Internet gaming (for IGD participants) across groups and in each group separately.

Results

Demographic characteristics and behavioral information are shown in Table 1.

Group difference in rsFC

Findings from between-group comparisons are shown in Table 2 and Fig. 2. In comparison with HC participants, the IGD participants exhibited significantly decreased rsFC strength between the VTA and right NAcc (p = 0.021). By contrast, rsFC between the VTA and left NAcc was not significantly different between HCs and IGD participants (p = 0.591). Whole-brain between-group differences in rsFC are shown in the Additional file 1: Table S2; Figures S1, S2.

Table 2 Resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC, Fisher Z value) difference between IGD and HC
Fig. 2
figure 2

Resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) between the VTA and left/right NAcc. The label’ Left NAcc and VTA’ refers to the rsFC strength between these two regions of interest. Bars with oblique lines show the mean values of rsFC between the VTA and left/right NAcc in IGD, bars with dots show the mean value of rsFC between the VTA and left/right NAcc in HC, error bars the standard errors of the mean

IGD subjects had significant higher BDI and BAI scores than HC subjects. Given concern related to the post hoc inclusion of covariates [44], we did not include these variables as covariates in our primary data analyses. Exploratory follow-up analyses including these variables yielded the following results for between-group differences in right NAcc and VTA connectivity: anxiety as covariate: F = 3.92, p = .053; depression as covariate: F = 1.38, p = .245. However, it is important to note that BDI and BAI scores were both significantly related to game-playing hours per week (r = 0.47, p < 0.001; r = .38, p = 0.003) and CIAS scores (r = 0.49, p < .001; r = 0.41, p = .001, Additional file 1: Table S1). Therefore, including these measures as covariates may have removed variance explained by problematic Internet game-playing or IGD severity.

The relationship between connectivity strength and severity of IGD/craving

The relationships between rsFC strength of the three regions and severity of IGD/craving for the Internet (for HC participants) or Internet gaming (for IGD participants) were assessed using correlation analyses. The results are shown in Table 3 and Fig. 3.

Table 3 Correlation between resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) strengths and CIAS/craving
Fig. 3
figure 3

Scatterplot showing association between right NAcc–VTA functional connectivity and subject craving across groups. Black squares indicate the values for IGD participants, black circles indicate the values for HC participants. Dotted line represents the best-fit regressions for all subjects

In the correlation analysis, the rsFC strength between the VTA and right NAcc was significant negatively correlated with the severity of IGD (CIAS, p = 0.974 for VTA and left NAcc and p = 0.020 for VTA and right NAcc) and with craving (p = 0.816 for VTA and left NAcc and p = 0.013 for VTA and right NAcc).

We also tested the correlation between rsFC strength and severity of IGD/craving in each group separately, and found no significant correlations within either group. Comparison of r values using Fisher’s r-to-z transformation (http://vassarstats.net) indicated that r values did not differ between groups for associations with CIAS (VTA–left NAcc: Z = −0.94, p = 0.348; VTA–right NAcc: Z = 0.81, p = 0.416) or craving (VTA–left NAcc: Z = −0.40, p = .689; VTA–right NAcc: Z = 0.25, p = 0.801). See results in Additional file 1: Table S3.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate rsFC between the VTA and NAcc among individuals with IGD versus controls, and to assess associations between rsFC and subjective craving. In comparison to HC participants, individuals with IGD had significantly decreased rsFC between the VTA and right NAcc. In addition, rsFC between these ROIs was significantly negatively associated with craving ratings for Internet-gaming.

RsFC between the VTA and NAcc

Our findings are consistent with the decreased rsFC between the VTA and the NAcc in chronic cocaine users, reported by Gu et al. [5]. The mesolimbic dopamine system—involving connections originating in the VTA and projecting to the NAcc has been consistently implicated in the neurobiology of addictions [45, 46]. Hypothesized decreases in mesolimbic dopamine signaling may be related to impaired dopamine signaling [5] and alterations in reward learning frequently observed among individuals with addictions [47], and are consistent with theoretical models proposing blunting of reward pathways, or reduced hedonic set points, among individuals with addictions, as noted by Gu and colleagues [9].

Functional alterations in mesolimbic pathways among individuals with drug or alcohol addictions are often interpreted as a consequence of frequent, long-term exposure to an exogenous drug. However, none of the IGD participants in this study had a history of drug or alcohol problems. Therefore, it is also possible that reduced VTA–NAcc connectivity in this population may represent a vulnerability factor for the development of IGD. In addition, the strength of VTA–right NAcc was negatively correlated with the severity of IGD, suggesting this rsFC could reflect the state of IGD, consistent with the finding that VTA and SN connectivity serves as a potential biomarker for DAergic dysfunction disorders [14].

IGD participants in this study had significantly higher anxiety and depression scores than did control participants. Both anxiety and depression were significantly associated with IGD severity and after including these variables in post hoc analyses the between-group difference in VTA and right NAcc connectivity was no longer significant. Prior studies have reported elevated anxiety and depression scores among frequent Internet users [48, 49], which were positively related with problematic Internet use [5052]. In addition, among psychological variables considered, depression has been most strongly associated with the development of IGD [53]. Thus for individuals with IGD, higher depression and anxiety might be a representative indicator of problematic Internet game-playing or IGD severity. Further studies explicitly recruiting individuals with IGD with low levels of anxiety and depression are needed to disentangle the effects of these variables on rsFC.

The association between rsFC strengths and craving

RsFC between the bilateral NAcc and VTA was significantly negatively correlated with craving, such that individuals with greater subjective Internet-craving had decreased rsFC. This inverse association between mesolimbic rsFC and subjective craving intensities is consistent with previous studies implicating rsFC between the bilateral NAcc and VTA in substance-related craving [1719], and can be explained by the theoretical models positing blunted DAergic transmission among individuals with both drug and behavioral addictions [54]. Within this framework, it is possible that long-time Internet gaming could impair VTA–NAcc connectivity.

There was no significant linear correlation between VTA and right NAcc connectivity and CIAS or craving among individuals with IGD. Within the context of the incentive-habit model of addiction [55], it may be argued that with increases in severity of IGD, Internet gaming may shift from a reward-directed behavior to a habitual compulsive behavior, and thus may be more closely involved in the maintenance of Internet gaming playing, rather than with craving specifically [56]. Further research assessing changes in craving processes in relation to rsFC over time among individuals with IGD is needed to explore this hypothesis.

Increased NAcc activity in response to gaming-related cues has been reported among individuals with IGD (compared to HCs), and is positively correlated with self-reported craving for Internet gaming [24]. Thus further research is needed to determine how alterations in functional connectivity might relate to cue-induced activity of the VTA and NAcc in this population.

Our findings have significant clinical implications: Craving is a central component of both substance and behavioral addictions [5759], and is an important potential therapeutic target. Our finding of a negative association between VTA and NAcc connectivity and subjective craving suggests that interventions specifically targeting craving processes in IGD may also impact on mesolimbic connectivity, or vice versa. Our findings suggest possible overlapping pathways of substance addictions and IGD; thus effective interventions for substance dependence may also be effective for IGD. More generally, these data suggest a possible general mechanism that may exist across addictions, which could therefore be a potentially sensitive biomarker for diagnoses as well as a target for treatment.

Conclusions and limitations

This study has several limitations. First, the cross-sectional design prevents us from determining whether alterations in rsFC are a vulnerability factor for, or a consequence of, IGD. Second, group sizes are unbalanced in our study, which may have influenced the results. Third, this study did not include female participants. While IGD is most prevalent among men, the exclusion of women limits the generalizability of our findings. In addition, we mainly focused on findings from our ROI-based connectivity analysis (rather than a whole-brain approach). However, this method was more appropriate to our research questions and allowed for comparison with other studies in substance addictions which have used VTA and NAcc ROIs [5]. We also conducted a whole-brain analyses for comparison and discussion in future studies.

The present study is the first to demonstrate decreased rsFC strength between the VTA and NAcc among individuals with IGD, in comparison to HCs. This is also the first study to report associations between VTA and NAcc connectivity and self-reported craving among individuals with IGD. These results parallel findings from substance addictions, suggesting possibly shared neurobiological substrates between IGD and substance addictions and providing additional support to the notion that IGD is a behavioral addiction.

References

  1. Ko C-H, Yen J-Y, Chen S-H, Wang P-W, Chen C-S, Yen C-F. Evaluation of the diagnostic criteria of Internet gaming disorder in the DSM-5 among young adults in Taiwan. J Psychiatr Res. 2014;53:103–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Petry NM, Rehbein F, Gentile DA, Lemmens JS, Rumpf HJ, Mößle T, et al. An international consensus for assessing internet gaming disorder using the new DSM-5 approach. Addiction. 2014. doi:10.1111/add.12457.

    Google Scholar 

  3. King DL, Delfabbro PH. The cognitive psychology of Internet gaming disorder. Clin Psychol Rev. 2014;34(4):298–308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. AP Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, (DSM-5®). Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013.

  5. Gu H, Salmeron BJ, Ross TJ, Geng X, Zhan W, Stein EA, et al. Mesocorticolimbic circuits are impaired in chronic cocaine users as demonstrated by resting-state functional connectivity. Neuroimage. 2010;53(2):593–601.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Biswal B, Zerrin Yetkin F, Haughton VM, Hyde JS. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson Med. 1995;34(4):537–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Sutherland MT, McHugh MJ, Pariyadath V, Stein EA. Resting state functional connectivity in addiction: lessons learned and a road ahead. Neuroimage. 2012;62(4):2281–95.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Van Den Heuvel MP, Pol HEH. Exploring the brain network: a review on resting-state fMRI functional connectivity. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010;20(8):519–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Koob GF, Le Moal M. Drug abuse: hedonic homeostatic dysregulation. Science. 1997;278(5335):52–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Rev. 1993;18(3):247–91.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Wise RA. Cocaine reward and cocaine craving: the role of dopamine in perspective. NIDA Res Monogr. 1994;145:191–6.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Koob GF. Cocaine reward and dopamine receptors: love at first site. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56(12):1107–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Ramaekers JG, Evers EA, Theunissen EL, Kuypers K, Goulas A, Stiers P. Methylphenidate reduces functional connectivity of nucleus accumbens in brain reward circuit. Psychopharmacology. 2013;229(2):219–26.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Tomasi D, Volkow ND. Functional connectivity of substantia nigra and ventral tegmental area: maturation during adolescence and effects of ADHD. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24(4):935–44.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Nestler EJ. Is there a common molecular pathway for addiction? Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(11):1445–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Berridge KC, Kringelbach ML. Affective neuroscience of pleasure: reward in humans and animals. Psychopharmacology. 2008;199(3):457–80.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Berridge KC. Pleasures of the brain. Brain Cogn. 2003;52(1):106–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Myrick H, Anton RF, Li X, Henderson S, Drobes D, Voronin K, et al. Differential brain activity in alcoholics and social drinkers to alcohol cues: relationship to craving. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29(2):393–402.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. David SP, Munafò MR, Johansen-Berg H, Smith SM, Rogers RD, Matthews PM, et al. Ventral striatum/nucleus accumbens activation to smoking-related pictorial cues in smokers and nonsmokers: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58(6):488–94.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Hong LE, Gu H, Yang Y, Ross TJ, Salmeron BJ, Buchholz B, et al. Association of nicotine addiction and nicotine’s actions with separate cingulate cortex functional circuits. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(4):431–41.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Ding W-N, Sun J-H, Sun Y-W, Zhou Y, Li L, Xu J-R, et al. Altered default network resting-state functional connectivity in adolescents with Internet gaming addiction. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(3):e59902.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Weinstein A, Lejoyeux M. New developments on the neurobiological and pharmaco-genetic mechanisms underlying internet and videogame addiction. Am J Addict. 2013. doi:10.1111/j.1521-0391.2013.12110.x.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Han DH, Lee YS, Yang KC, Kim EY, Lyoo IK, Renshaw PF. Dopamine genes and reward dependence in adolescents with excessive internet video game play. J Addict Med. 2007;1(3):133–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Ko C-H, Liu G-C, Hsiao S, Yen J-Y, Yang M-J, Lin W-C, et al. Brain activities associated with gaming urge of online gaming addiction. J Psychiatr Res. 2009;43(7):739–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Ko C-H, Yen J-Y, Chen C-C, Chen S-H, Yen C-F. Proposed diagnostic criteria of Internet addiction for adolescents. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2005;193(11):728–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Ko C-H, Yen J-Y, Chen S-H, Yang M-J, Lin H-C, Yen C-F. Proposed diagnostic criteria and the screening and diagnosing tool of Internet addiction in college students. Compr Psychiatry. 2009;50(4):378–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Chen S, Weng L, Su Y, Wu H, Yang P. Development of a Chinese Internet addiction scale and its psychometric study. Chin J Psychol. 2003;45(3):279.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Lin X, Dong G, Wang Q, Du X. Abnormal gray matter and white matter volume in ‘Internet gaming addicts’. Addict Behav. 2015;40:137–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Weng C-B, Qian R-B, Fu X-M, Lin B, Han X-P, Niu C-S, et al. Gray matter and white matter abnormalities in online game addiction. Eur J Radiol. 2013;82(8):1308–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Zhang JT, Yao YW, Li CSR, Zang YF, Shen ZJ, Liu L, et al. Altered resting-state functional connectivity of the insula in young adults with Internet gaming disorder. Addict Biol. 2015. doi:10.1111/adb.12247.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Fagerström K-O. Measuring degree of physical dependence to tobacco smoking with reference to individualization of treatment. Addict Behav. 1978;3(3):235–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Selzer ML. The Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test: the quest for a new diagnostic instrument. Am J Psychiatry. 1971;127(12):1653–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1988;56(6):893.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh JK. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1961;4(6):561–71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Wang X, Wang X, Ma H. Manual of mental health assessment scale (revised edition). Beijing: Chinese Mental Health Magazine; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Cox LS, Tiffany ST, Christen AG. Evaluation of the brief questionnaire of smoking urges (QSU-brief) in laboratory and clinical settings. Nicotine Tob Res. 2001;3(1):7–16.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Chao-Gan Y, Yu-Feng Z. DPARSF: a MATLAB toolbox for “pipeline” data analysis of resting-state fMRI. Front Syst Neurosci. 2010. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2010.00013.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage. 2012;59(3):2142–54.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Steps toward optimizing motion artifact removal in functional connectivity MRI; a reply to Carp. Neuroimage. 2013;76:439–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Lemieux L, Salek-Haddadi A, Lund TE, Laufs H, Carmichael D. Modelling large motion events in fMRI studies of patients with epilepsy. Magn Reson Imaging. 2007;25(6):894–901.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Satterthwaite TD, Wolf DH, Ruparel K, Erus G, Elliott MA, Eickhoff SB, et al. Heterogeneous impact of motion on fundamental patterns of developmental changes in functional connectivity during youth. Neuroimage. 2013;83:45–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Song X-W, Dong Z-Y, Long X-Y, Li S-F, Zuo X-N, Zhu C-Z, et al. REST: a toolkit for resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging data processing. PLoS One. 2011;6(9):e25031.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Lancaster JL, Woldorff MG, Parsons LM, Liotti M, Freitas CS, Rainey L, et al. Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Hum Brain Mapp. 2000;10(3):120–31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Kraemer HC. A Source of false findings in published research studies: adjusting for covariates. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;34(5):631–48.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Cousins DA, Butts K, Young AH. The role of dopamine in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2009;11(8):787–806.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Wahlstrom D, White T, Luciana M. Neurobehavioral evidence for changes in dopamine system activity during adolescence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;34(5):631–48.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Schultz W. Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron. 2002;36(2):241–63.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Dalbudak E, Evren C, Aldemir S, Taymur I, Evren B, Topcu M. The impact of sensation seeking on the relationship between attention deficit/hyperactivity symptoms and severity of Internet addiction risk. Psychiatry Res. 2015;228(1):156–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Demirci K, Akgönül M, Akpinar A. Relationship of smartphone use severity with sleep quality, depression, and anxiety in university students. J Behav Addict. 2015;4(2):85–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Reed P, Vile R, Osborne LA, Romano M, Truzoli R. Problematic Internet usage and immune function. PLoS One. 2015;10(8):e0134538.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Moreno MA, Jelenchick LA, Breland DJ. Exploring depression and problematic internet use among college females: a multisite study. Comput Hum Behav. 2015;49:601–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Błachnio A, Przepiórka A, Pantic I. Internet use, Facebook intrusion, and depression: results of a cross-sectional study. Eur Psychiatry. 2015;30:681–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Hyun GJ, Han DH, Lee YS, Kang KD, Yoo SK, Chung U-S, et al. Risk factors associated with online game addiction: a hierarchical model. Comput Hum Behav. 2015;48:706–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Blum K, Febo M, McLaughlin T, Cronjé FJ, Han D, Gold MS. Hatching the behavioral addiction egg: reward deficiency solution system (RDSS)™ as a function of dopaminergic neurogenetics and brain functional connectivity linking all addictions under a common rubric. J Behav Addict. 2014;3(3):149–56.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(11):1481–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Tiffany ST, Carter BL. Is craving the source of compulsive drug use? J Psychopharmacol. 1998;12(1):23–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Burton SM, Tiffany ST. The effect of alcohol consumption on craving to smoke. Addiction. 1997;92(1):15–26.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. De Wit H. Laboratory‐based assessment of alcohol craving in social drinkers. Addiction. 2000;95(8s2):165–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Wölfling K, Mörsen CP, Duven E, Albrecht U, Grüsser SM, Flor H. To gamble or not to gamble: at risk for craving and relapse-learned motivated attention in pathological gambling. Biol Psychol. 2011;87(2):275–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors’ contributions

JTZ, XYF: research design, manuscript writing and manuscript revisions. SSM, LJW: literature review, data collection, data analysis, revising and editing manuscript. SWY: data interpretation and manuscript revisions. CC: literature review, data collection, data analysis, composing the first draft, etc. LL, BL: literature review, data collection. CGY: data analysis guidance and manuscript revisions. LYD, QXL: manuscript revisions. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 31170990 to X-Y.F and No. 81100992 to J-T.Z), project for young scholar supported by State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, and Project of Humanities and Social Sciences supported by Ministry of Education in China (No. 15YJC190035 to J-T.Z), and The Hundred Talents Program of the Chinese Academy of Sciences (Y5CX072006 to C-G.Y).

Competing interests

All authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Xiao-Yi Fang.

Additional file

12993_2015_82_MOESM1_ESM.docx

Additional file 1. Correlations between VTA-NACC connectivity and IGD severity/subjective craving within each group, and the whole-brain between-group differences in resting state functional connectivity of VTA/NACC.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zhang, JT., Ma, SS., Yip, S.W. et al. Decreased functional connectivity between ventral tegmental area and nucleus accumbens in Internet gaming disorder: evidence from resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging. Behav Brain Funct 11, 37 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12993-015-0082-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12993-015-0082-8

Keywords