Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Association between self-control and health risk behaviors: a cross-sectional study with 9th grade adolescents in São Paulo

Abstract

Background

Self-control (SC) has been consistently found associated with diverse health risk behaviors (HRBs), but little research refers to low- and middle-income countries. Furthermore, there is evidence that some HRBs tend to aggregate, however studies with the specific purpose of addressing the relation between SC and multiple health risk behaviors (MHRBs) are rare. The objective of this study is to analyze these associations and provide evidence to help filling these gaps.

Methods

A sample of 2106 9th grade students from the city of São Paulo responded a self-administered questionnaire in 2017. We tested the association of SC measured as an ordinal variable with four levels (higher, high, medium and low) with six HRBs (binge drinking, marijuana use, smoking, high consumption of ultra-processed food, sedentary behavior and bullying perpetration), in both separated and aggregated forms (MHRBs), controlling for potential confounders. Binary logistic regression was used to test the association between exposure (SC) and single outcomes. In order to analyze the association of SC with MHRBs, multinomial logistic regression was employed.

Results

SC was associated with five of six HRBs investigated and with MHRBs. The effect size of the association of SC and MHRBs increased in a steep pattern with accumulation of more HRBs.

Conclusion

Low self-control is associated with most HRBs investigated and the magnitude of the association increases when more than two or three HRBs are accumulated. There seems to be a group of adolescents in a position of pronounced vulnerability for MHRBs. This should be considered when designing public policy and prevention programs. In contexts of limited or scarce resources and public funds, interventions focusing the most vulnerable groups, instead of universal interventions, should be considered.

Peer Review reports

Background

It has long been recognized that much of the burden of health problems can be attributed to behavioral risk factors [1,2,3]. Common risk behaviors for several Noncommunicable Diseases (NCD) include tobacco and alcohol use, physical inactivity, as well as unhealthy diet [4]. Many of these behaviors tend to begin during the adolescence and persist throughout life [5]. Risk behaviors “can be defined as those that are potentially capable of threatening physical or mental health, in both present and future” [6].

Studies available in the literature indicate that different HRBs tend to co-occur and form clusters [7,8,9]. Among Brazilian adolescents, evidence suggests that the accumulation of two or more HRBs increases with age [10,11,12] and is more frequent among children attending public schools [12]. Despite some considerable accumulation of research [8], to our knowledge, studies on the association between socioemotional factors and clusters of HRBs are scarce and mainly concentrated in high income countries.

One important socioemotional factor is self-control, “the ability to self-regulate behavior and inhibit impulses” [13] or “the ability to pursue overarching goals despite short-term temptations, distractions or aversive states” [14].

Low self-control has been found to be associated with many negative outcomes [15]. In particular, among adolescents around the globe, low SC has been found to be positively associated with criminal offending, aggressive or delinquent behavior [13, 16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25], use of legal and illegal psychoactive substances [26,27,28,29], bullying perpetration [30, 31] and negatively associated with health promoting behaviors [32]. Similar associations have been found regarding dietary habits [20, 33] and sedentary behavior [34, 35]. Some longitudinal studies demonstrated that low SC during childhood is associated with negative outcomes in adulthood, with impairments in health, wealth and social adjustment, such as greater involvement in criminal activities [15, 36]. Studies with different research designs, samples and measurement instruments, show evidence of the association between SC and specific HRBs or a restricted set of similar behaviors, but few tested different HRBs simultaneously; exceptions worth noting are Moffit et al. [15] and de Winter et al. [37]. Furthermore, the relation of the SC and MHRBs has received scarce attention [37].

The current study

The present study aims to investigate the association between self-control and six different HRBs (binge drinking, marijuana use, smoking, high consumption of ultra-processed food, sedentary behavior, and bullying perpetration), in both individual and aggregated forms (MHRBs), among adolescents in São Paulo, Brazil.

There is little evidence of the association of SC and a wide set of HRBs coming from the same study and evidence for the effect of SC on the accumulation of HRBs among adolescents is scarce [37]. There are important reasons for which these gaps should be filled. Additionally, to our knowledge, no study considered the effect of SC on HRBs in the Brazilian context.Footnote 1

A better understanding of the association of SC with MHRBs can be useful for the development of prevention programs. Due to an alleged gradient effect of SC across the entire population, it has been argued that universal programs are the most interesting option from a public policy point of view [15]. Interventions based on opt-out schemes modify environmental conditions to induce the adoption of healthy behaviors as the default option [15]. An example is the prohibition of cigarette consumption indoors or in public buildings, making the decision to smoke more costly and thus inducing individuals to choose not to smoke. This type of policy does not intend to change individual traits, but only behavior and is applicable to the society at large.

Alternatively, to opt-out schemes, there are interventions aiming at the development of social skills. This model is individually based and believed to work best when applied early in childhood (or even during pregnancy). The Nurse-Family Partnership is the best evaluated program of early childhood development and it is based on intensive services to high-risk families and delivered by highly skilled professionals [38]. Comparatively, social skill interventions targeting middle childhood or adolescence, have shown more modest results [39]. A recent meta-analysis of randomized trials found consistent positive results for universal interventions (physical activity, mindfulness and yoga, family-based and other social and personal skills interventions) to improve self-regulation of children and adolescents [40]. However, greater effect-size was found among individuals considered at risk at baseline [40]. When it comes to the design of interventions aiming at the development of social skills, three major factors must be addressed: who should be invited (target versus universal approaches), when to intervene and the profile of personnel [38].

It does not seem that any of these models are mutually exclusive, but can be adopted in different forms and combinations in each particular context. In contexts with high rates of serious violence and social inequality [41], intensive and target programs for adolescents at risk for MHRB should be considered.

Also, if SC is related to such a wide variety of HRBs, it could replace programs targeting specific behaviors (such as abusive substance use). To sum up, understanding how levels of SC affect the occurrence of MHRBs is essential to guide public policy design.

The first hypothesis of the present study is that SC is associated with all six HRBs investigated. Consequently, an association of SC with the occurrence of MHRBs should also be expected. The second hypothesis is that the magnitude of the association of SC and MHRBs should increase manifold after more than two or three HRBs are accumulated.

Methods

Data was collected in the research “São Paulo Project for the social development of children and adolescents” (SP-PROSO). SP-PROSO is the result of a partnership between the Department of Preventive Medicine of the University of São Paulo Medical School and the Violence Research Centre of the University of Cambridge.

The target population of SP-PROSO was composed of adolescents enrolled in the 9th grade in the city of São Paulo in 2017. The sample was stratified according to school type: state public, municipal public and private, and by conglomerate, taking each class as a draw unit. 156 classes, one class from each school, were randomly selected and 119 agreed to participate in the study. The minimum sample size in São Paulo was determined as 2849 students to allow estimates as low as 15% with a precision of 0.06 and deff = 1.7. Any adolescent present in classroom on the day of data collection, whose parents had not proscribed their child’s participation and who did not present any serious impairment that could prevent them from understanding or answering the questionnaire anonymously, was considered eligible. Considering the 2816 students present on the day of the survey; 96 refused to participate and 18 were excluded due to failed questionnaire completion, which resulted in a sample of 2702 adolescents, 94.5% of the estimated sample. A total of 2680 adolescents answered more than 80% of the questionnaire and were included in the data analysis. For this study, only questionnaires with all answers filled in the variables of interest were included, yielding a sample of 2106 adolescents.Footnote 2

Students, individually, filled the printed questionnaires, in the presence of trained researchers and without the presence of teachers or other school professionals. All the questionnaires were reviewed upon completion to detect inconsistencies and missing values when students were asked to complete the section. All the questionnaires were anonymous for students and schools.

For the most part, SP-PROSO instrument is the same as the 6th wave of the “Zurich Project on the Social Development of Children” (Z-PROSO) [26] and the “Proyecto Montevideo para el desarrollo social de niños y adolescents” (M- PROSO) [42]. Additional questions were included to account for specific interests regarding the Brazilian context. All the questions used in this study are available in the Supplementary File. The translation process followed recommendations for culturally sensitive translation [43, 44].

Outcome variables

Binge drinking: Adolescents were asked if they had taken five or more drinks in a single occasion over the 30 days prior to the survey. Possible responses were yes and no.

Smoking tobacco and marijuana use: Adolescents were asked about the frequency of use in the previous 12 months. Possible answers were “never”, “once”, “2 to 5 times”, “every month”, “every week” and “(almost) daily”. Responses were recorded as no (never) or yes (all other answers) as a binary variable.

High consumption of ultra-processed foods: Adolescents were asked how often they ate five types of foods in a regular week: sausages, crackers, packet snacks, treats (sweets, candies, chocolates) and sugary drinks. The possible answers ranged from never to 7 days. A score was calculated by the sum of answers. The sample was then divided into quartiles and the upper quartile was labelled as high consumption of ultra-processed foods.

Sedentary habits: Individuals were asked how much time each day they sat watching television, using the computer, talking to friends or doing other things (apart from time at school). Possible answers ranged from “less than one hour a day” to “more than eight hours a day”. Those who answered more than eight hours a day were considered sedentary.

Bullying perpetration: Following Alsaker [45, 46], five questions were asked about the repeated practice in the 12 months prior to the survey verifying: the practice of exclusion/ostracism; make fun/offend; hit/kick/pull hair; destroy/steal/hide belongings and sexually harassing. Bullying was considered to have occurred when the respondent reported at least one of those behaviours, in a frequency of at least once a month in the previous year.

Multiple health risk behaviors: A score was made summing the type of behaviors declared by each respondent. Scores of 4, 5 and 6 were combined because of the low number of cases in each score, so that the final variable ranges from 0 (no health risk behavior) to 4 (4 or more health risk behaviors).

Exposure variable: self-control

Self-control was measured through a 10-item scale adapted from the self-control measure proposed by Grasmick et al. [47]. This reduced scale was used at the 6th wave of the longitudinal Z-PROSO project [48]. Respondents were presented with statements (i.e: “I often act on the spur of the moment without stopping to think”, “Sometimes I will take a risk just for the fun of it” and “If things I do upset people, it’s their problem, not mine”) and asked to choose from a Likert scale, ranging from 1 “totally disagree” to 4 “totally agree”. A score was calculated from the mean value of all answers for each participant (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.75; CFA: Loadings: 0.25–1.13; chi2 = 267.969 (31df); p < 0.001; RMSEA = 0.05; CFI: 0.95). The sample was then divided in quartiles named: highest, high, medium and low.

Adjustment variables

Sociodemographic variables: Gender (0-female/1-male) and age (in years) were investigated. The variable Socioeconomic Status of the Family (SES) was created based on the questions from the School-Based Health Survey (PeNSE) in Brazil [49]. A score was calculated with seven items: ownership of consumer goods in the household (landline and car), personal belongings (cellphone, computer with internet and television), number of bathrooms in the house and having a monthly paid house keeper working at home. A summative weighted score (frequent items were given lower weights while most infrequent were given higher weights) was calculated as proposed by Levy et al. [50].

Important variables regularly associated with HRBs are moral values, attitude and beliefs towards violence [22, 24, 25], parenting techniques [22, 23, 31], peer delinquency [13, 18, 22, 28, 31, 51], school disorder [31], and violence/disorder in the neighborhood [13, 22]. Such variables were included in the analysis in order to isolate the possible independent association of SC with HRBs.

For the moral values scale, students were presented with a list with seven different actions someone his/her age could do and asked to evaluate in a scale ranging from “not bad at all” [1] to “very bad” [7]. Situations included lying to parents, teachers and other adults, playing truant at school, hitting someone for being insulted, stealing something worth about 5$ and insulting another adolescent for not liking him/her. Comparing to Z-PROSO, the SP-PROSO questionnaire presents two additional questions concerning the use of a gun to assault or rob. A mean score was calculated for the seven questions (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.83; CFA: Loadings: 1.28–1.82; chi2 = 88.834 (10df); p < 0.001; RMSEA = 0.05; CFI: 0.99).

Perceptions about positive parenting: Three questions (whether parents recognized and appreciated the adolescents’ efforts or achievements) that compose the positive parenting sub-dimension from the Alabama Parenting Questionnaire [52] were employed. Possible answers ranged from “never” to “often” in a four-point Likert scale. A mean score was calculated (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.82; CFA: Loadings: 0.41–1.62; chi2 = 341.843 (41df); p < 0.001; RMSEA = 0.05; CFI: 0.96).

Delinquent peer group: membership to a delinquent/transgressive peer group was assessed with two questions from the Eurogang Survey [53]. Initially, it was asked whether they considered themselves part of a group of friends. For those who had a positive answer, they were asked if any of their friends in the group took part in a list of nine actions, such as selling illegal drugs, getting protection money and carrying weapons. If the respondent answered that at least one member of the group has done at least one of the nine items, it was considered that he/she was part of a delinquent peer group.

School administrative status refer to whether the institution is private or public. Public schools include municipal and states government administration, but here both were included into one single category.

Exposure to school violence and disorder is a mean score containing 12 items assessing the frequency of witnessing or hearing about school violence, or school disorder, within the previous 12 months of the survey created by SP-PROSO team. Items included physical fights, drug selling, gun possessions and others. Answers were in a 4-point Likert scale from “never” to “often (5 or more times) (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.83; CFA: Loadings: 0.20–1.31; chi2 = 295.979 (35df); p < 0.001; RMSEA = 0.05; CFI: 0.97).

The scale of exposure to community violence was adapted from Children’s Exposure to Community Violence scale [54]. Students were asked if they had heard about or seen violent events that had occurred in the previous 12 months in the neighbourhood where they lived. Events included shots or shooting, arrests by the police, drug selling, assassinations, people carrying firearms in the street (other than the police or those authorised to use firearms), robbery and police bribery, in a total of 14 items (Cronbach’s α: 0.92). Possible answers ranged from never to often (five or more times) and a mean for the 14 questions was computed. (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.92; CFA: Loadings: 0.80–1.34; chi2 = 505.675 (59df); p < 0.001; RMSEA = 0.05; CFI: 0.98).

Table 1 summarizes the variables used in the analysis.

Table 1 Description of variables

Statistical analysis

Analyses were performed using the software Stata 15.1 All the analyses considered sampling weights and were run using the svy module. A descriptive analysis with the calculation of proportions for categorical variables and means for continuous and discrete variables, followed by 95% Confidence Intervals was conducted.

To test the association between exposure (self-control) and single outcomes (binge drinking, marijuana use, smoking, high consumption of ultra-processed food, sedentary behavior, and bullying perpetration), binary logistic regression, adjusting for covariates, has been used. To analyze the association of SC with the accumulation of HRBs, the multinomial logistic regression has been employed.

Robustness checks

We chose to categorize SC according to quartiles to calculate specific OR according to the capacity to exercise self-control. However, this approach has some disadvantages, such as the arbitrary definition of a cut-off point for the mean SC categories. To check for the consistency of our results, we re-estimated the adjusted model having SC as a continuous variable. This model results in a single OR for the SC variable that was expected to be positive (OR > 1), in the case, lowering the capacity of self-control showed to be associated with a higher odds of health risk behaviors. The result is reported in Tables 6 and 7 in the appendix. The results remained the same: each unity increase in low self-control score (meaning lowering the self-control capacity) is associated with higher odds for all HRBs.

Results

Sample descriptions

As shown in Tables 2, 50.6% of respondents in our sample were boys and the mean age was 14.8 years. Socioeconomic status ranged from 0 to 13.56 and its mean was 7, whereas 33% of adolescents attended private schools. The mean score for SC for the whole sample was 2.2 (CI 95% 2.2–2.3).Footnote 3 Mean score for each quartile of SC and the descriptive statistics of the adjusting variables are displayed in Table 2.

Table 2 Characteristics of the sample – control and explanatory variables - consisting in 9th grade students from elementary schools in São Paulo, Brazil, 2017 (SP-Proso study; n = 2106)

Table 3 presents the description of the sample concerning HRBs. The most common HRBs referred in the sample were binge drinking (28.1%) and high consumption of ultra-processed food (22.9%) while the least common was marijuana use (10.8%). Concerning MHRBs, 42.4% of respondents referred none of the six HRBs of interest. For 28.3%, one of the behaviors was present, and for 5.8%, four or more behaviors were reported.

Table 3 Characteristics of the sample – outcome variables - consisting in 9th grade students from elementary schools in São Paulo, Brazil, 2017 (SP-Proso study; n = 2106)

Bivariate and multivariate analysis

Table 4 presents crude and adjusted odds ratios (OR) for the association between SC and each HRB investigated. SC is associated with all outcomes in the bivariate analysis (crude OR). For binge drinking and smoking, any decrease in SC, as compared to the highest level, was associated with higher odds of the outcome with statistical significance. For marijuana, high consumption of ultra-processed food and bullying perpetration, a negative and significant association was found only for those in the medium and low SC groups as compared to those with highest SC capacity. For sedentary habits, a negative association was found only for those in the low SC group.

Table 4 Raw and adjusted association between self-control and HRBs among 9th grade elementary students. São Paulo, Brazil (SP-Proso study). N = 2106. São Paulo, 2017. OR adjusted (CI 95%)

When control variables were added to each model, the general tendency for an increase in the odds ratio persisted. SC is associated with all HRBs except for sedentary habits. For high and medium SC groups, the strongest associations were with smoking (OR = 1.8 and 2.6, respectively). In the low SC group, the strongest association was with binge drinking. Bullying perpetration presented a pattern similar to binge drinking, since only associations of SC in the medium and low groups were significant. The association of marijuana use and high consumption of ultra-processed food and SC were significant only for the lower SC group and the respective OR were 2.4 and 2.3.

Regarding the sedentary behaviour, the association with SC lost its significance in the adjusted model for São Paulo adolescents, despite being positive. According to Gottfredson & Hirschi [55], the preference for physical activity over contemplative activity is one of the characteristics of people with low-self control. Following this, the scale proposed by Grasmick et al. [47] includes one dimension that reflects this preference as does the adapted version used in this study. This could explain our discrepant result: once preference for physical activity is common to those with low SC, they would tend to be less sedentary. To deal with this possibility, we ran logistic regressions again (Appendix, Table 8) without the two items that composes the physical activity dimension in the SC scale and an OR of 1.6 (IC95% 1.03–2.55) appeared with significance for the low SC group.

Table 5 presents the association between self-control and MHRBs. It is possible to note an increase in the OR across self-control categories from the highest to the low group for one, two, three and four or more HRBs. High SC was not associated with any amount of risky behavior. Low SC, in its turn, was associated with every number of MHRBs, and the more behaviors were included, the stronger was the association. Medium level of SC was associated with two or more health risk behaviors, and the association was stronger in groups of respondents who concentrated four risk behaviors or more.

Table 5 Association (adjusted only) between self-control and MHRBs among 9th grade elementary students. São Paulo, Brazil (SP-Proso study). N = 2106. São Paulo, 2017

Being in high SC capacity group does not increase the odds of presenting one HRB as compared to the highest SC group (OR: 1, CI 95% 0.7–1.4). For the medium SC capacity group, associations are positive for any number of HRBs, but not significant for presenting one HRB. For the low SC capacity group, the association was positive and significant for one, two, three and four or more HRBs, with OR expressing a stronger magnitude for the association. As compared to the reference category, the odds of presenting one HRB in this group was 1.8, 5.9 for two HRBs, 7.1 for three HRBs and 15.2 for four or more HRBs.

Discussion

In the past 30 years, self-control has been found related to a wide set of undesirable outcomes, across different disciplines and population samples [15]. In the present study, we investigated the association of SC with six different HRBs (binge drinking, marijuana use, smoking, ultra-processed food consumption, bullying perpetration and sedentary behavior), separately and as an aggregated measure of MHRBs, in a sample composed of adolescents in the city of São Paulo.

In our study, adolescents belonging to the low SC group presented higher odds of engagement in binge drinking, smoking tobacco, marijuana use, bullying perpetration and of presenting a pattern of high consumption of ultra-processed food, when compared to adolescents in the reference group (highest SC group).

Our findings are in accordance with those in international literature. The use of legal and illegal psychoactive substances has been found to be associated with low SC among adolescents in western high-income countries [26, 27, 29] and east Asia [28], bullying perpetration was found associated with low SC in a sample of Hispanic adolescents in a US city [31] and Macanese teenagers [30]. Concerning ultra-processed food, our results point in a similar direction to the findings for Australian boys and girls among whom negative control was associated with binge eating [34] and for American adolescents, among whom poor self-control was positively associated with saturated fat intake and good self-control with fruit, vegetable, and fiber intake [33].

The positive association of low SC and sedentary habits lost significance in the adjusted model and was found significant with an adapted scale that did not have the “preference for physical activities” dimension. Xiang et al. [35] found a positive association of self-control and sedentary behavior. It is also interesting to compare our result with Wills et al. (2007) who observed that good self-control was found to be negatively associated with sedentary behaviour, but poor self-control was not associated, either negatively or positively. Thus, the relationship of SC with sedentary behavior demands more in-depth studies, preferably with a sample including adults for whom the association might appear and with different forms of variable operationalization.

The hypothesis that SC would be associated with so many different human behaviors was present in the General Theory of Crime [55]. So far, the evidence of the association of SC and HRBs has mostly come in pieces, with each study testing specific constructs of SC and a few similar outcomes of interest, whereas only a handful of research addressed together HRBs that are very different from each other such as Moffit [15] and de Winter [37]. This study adds evidence that the association found between SC and so many different behaviors is not the product of scales tailor-made to present association with certain outcomes, but that this is a construct that captures the relation between the capacity to “delay gratification, control impulses, and modulate emotional expression” [15] and human behavior resulting from the conflict of desire and a higher-order goal [56].

While Moffit et al. [15] found a significant linear associations of SC and health, wealth, and crime, Mears et al. [21] found nonlinear two-threshold effect of SC on offending among US adolescents. Our study was not designed to account for the linearity of those associations, but our findings are more compatible with those presented by Mears et al. [21]. Based on the present results, it is not possible to ascertain that any amount of change in SC would produce a change in the frequency of outcomes of interest. For most of the outcomes investigated, except smoking, the increase in OR was significant only for those in the medium and low SC groups. In other words, our results point to the existence of a threshold, below which self-control begins to act as risk factor for HRBs in adolescence.

Our second hypothesis was that the association of SC with MHRBs would increase when new HRB were added. This hypothesis was also supported by the present results. So, even if future evidence supports the linear association hypothesis, since SC is related to many different outcomes, it is important to consider how the magnitude of the association changes when those different outcomes are considered together.

This specific discussion has been largely neglected by the literature, however such approach is of great importance because of its practical implications. Based on the finding that “health, wealth, and crime outcomes followed a gradient across the full distribution of self-control in the population”, Moffit et al. [15] suggest that universal type interventions to improve SC or mitigating its negative effects would be the most valuable, considering the net effect in prosperity and well-being of the population, with the advantage of not stigmatizing any groups and gaining widespread citizen support.

Our results do not challenge this position, but shed light to the complementary nature of different public policy designs. Public funds are always limited or scarce and interventions focusing the most vulnerable youths should be considered, specially taking in account that early child development programs are, at present, far from universal. In contexts with high rates of violence and inequality, the benefits of interventions focused on specific groups should be even higher, tackling a number of different undesirable outcomes. Working with a small portion of the population, even the most expensive programs would probably worth the investments. However, for solid conclusions, more evidence must be gathered and, for public policies, the population attributable risk must be estimated.

Another consideration should be made concerning adolescents in the other extreme, the upper intermediate level of SC. Jessor [1] states that the rationale underneath risk behaviors are not restricted to their outcomes, how ever adverse or undesirable they may be. Binge drinking, smoking and marijuana use can serve typical adolescent goals as peer acceptance and a sense of maturity and independence. Ultra-processed food consumption is pleasurable, and consumption related risk behaviors can be a way of dealing with anxiety. While the rationality of bullying perpetration can be more difficult to admit, given its consequences to others, and sedentary behavior can only be explained by immediate gratification, these two behaviours are not, necessarily, irrational or pathological. Improving SC of adolescents through universal interventions, as proposed by Moffit et al. [15] would, certainly minimize the frequency of negative behaviors and, consequently, their risks. However, if we consider the upper intermediate level of SC, improving this individual trait may not have such important gains as providing adolescents with alternative channels to achieve the same goals, since those behaviors might have been rationally chosen rather than just unintended consequence of immediate temptation.

Lastly, it is important to address the context in which the study was carried out. Since the Brazilian context is different from those where most part of other studies on SC were carried out (high-income and/or Asian countries), these results add relevant confirmatory evidence to the hypothesis that SC is a variable associated with a wide range of undesirable behaviors. The practical implications of these results, specifically concerning the adoption of combinations of universal and focused strategies, are of particular importance to other Latin-American countries that share similar social structures and patterns of violence and inequality and scarce public funds.

Limitation of the study

Our results should be interpreted considering some limitations of the study. The SP-PROSO study is a school-based cross-sectional survey and for that matter, its first limitation is that it does not provide evidence on temporal sequence between exposure and outcomes. Longitudinal studies carried elsewhere provided evidence to the hypothesis that the causal path is from SC to HRBs [15, 19, 26]. A cohort study among a sample of adolescents in Brazil would be, nevertheless, a gain to our understanding of the phenomenon.

Secondly, the generalization of results is restricted, since the sample was representative of those present on the day of the data collection and that have not refused to participate in the survey. Furthermore, some of the selected schools refused to participate. Since information about either nonparticipant schools or absent adolescents is not available, it is not possible to estimate such bias in the study. Adolescents can be absent from school for a variety of reasons that could affect anyone regardless of their level of SC, from sickness, to feeling unsafe in the path to school. However, it is reasonable to suppose that students with lower SC are more prone to truancy and that they were overrepresented in the group of absent students, causing selection bias. In fact, in our sample, the correlation of the variable measuring truancy and SC is 0.23 and significant. However, it is also reasonable to suppose that truant students are also more likely to engage in most of the HRBs investigated here, such as binge drinking, smoking, use of marijuana and bullying perpetration. In that sense, we have no good reason to suppose that the association of exposure and outcome could be underestimated. On the other hand, we can expect that prevalence is underestimated, for both exposure and outcomes investigated.

Scales used in this study have been validated and used in multiple samples from different parts of the world, but they have not been fully validated in Brazil.

Conclusion

The first hypothesis of the present study was that SC is associated with six HRBs investigated, which is largely supported by our results. Furthermore, the results fully supported the second hypothesis that the magnitude of the association of SC and MHRBs should increase once more than two or three HRBs accumulate.

We believe that the findings and discussions presented in this study make relevant contributions to the knowledge about SC and public policy design concerning the wellbeing and health of adolescents across the globe.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Notes

  1. To our knowledge, only Rebellon [51] included subjects from Brazil, adolescents or otherwise, in a study of crime. However, it was a convenience sample of less than 400 college students.

  2. For the scales constructed by mean of responses, a minimum of 80% response was adopted as criterion of inclusion. For summative scales, only questionnaires with all answers in the scale were included.

  3. Not shown.

Abbreviations

SC:

Self-control

HRB:

health risk behaviors

MHRB:

multiple health risk behavior

NCD:

Noncommunicable diseases

SP-PROSO:

São Paulo Project for the social development of children and adolescents

Z-PROSO:

Zurich Project on Social Development of Children

M-PROSO:

Montevideo project on Social Development of Children

PeNSE:

School-Based Health Survey (Brazil)

UPF:

Ultra-processed foods

OR:

Odds ratio

References

  1. Jessor R. Risk behavior in adolescence: a psychosocial framework for understanding and action. J Adolesc Health. 1991;12(September):597–605. https://doi.org/10.1016/1054-139X(91)90007-K.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Park MJ, Scott JT, Adams SH, Brindis CD, Irwin CE. Adolescent and young adult health in the United States in the past decade: little improvement and young adults remain worse off than adolescents. J Adolesc Heal [Internet] 2014;55(1):3–16. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2014.04.003

  3. Richardson LP, Zhou C, Gersh E, Spielvogle H, Taylor JA, McCarty CA. Effect of electronic screening with personalized feedback on adolescent health risk behaviors in a primary care setting: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Netw Open. 2019;2(5):e193581. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.3581.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. World Health Organization. Global Status Report On Noncommunicable Diseases. 2014:2014.

  5. Harris KM, Gordon-Larsen P, Chantala K, Udry JR. Longitudinal trends in race/ethnic disparities in leading health indicators from adolescence to young adulthood. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2006;160(1):74–81. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.160.1.74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Zappe JG, Alves CF, Dell’Aglio DD. Comportamentos de risco na adolescência: Revisão sistemática de estudos empíricos. Psicol em Rev. 2018;24(1):79–100. https://doi.org/10.5752/P.1678-9563.2018v24n1p79-100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Busch V, Van Stel HF, Schrijvers AJ, De Leeuw JR. Clustering of health-related behaviors, health outcomes and demographics in Dutch adolescents: A cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. 2013;13(1).

  8. Moura LR de, Torres LM, Cadete MMM, Cunha C de F. Fatores associados aos comportamentos de risco à saúde entre adolescentes brasileiros: uma revisão integrativa. Rev da Esc Enferm da USP 2018;52(0):1–11.

  9. van Nieuwenhuijzen M, Junger M, Velderman MK, Wiefferink KH, Paulussen TWGM, Hox J, Reijneveld SA Clustering of health-compromising behavior and delinquency in adolescents and adults in the Dutch population. Prev Med (Baltim) [Internet] 2009;48(6):572–578. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2009.04.008

  10. da S Brito AL, Hardman CM, de Barros MVG. Prevalência e fatores associados à simultaneidade de comportamentos de risco à saúde em adolescentes. Rev Paul Pediatr [Internet] 2015;33(4):423–430. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rpped.2015.02.002

  11. Cureau FV, Duarte P. Lopes dos Santos D, Reichert FF. Clustering of risk factors for noncommunicable diseases in Brazilian adolescents: prevalence and correlates. J Phys Act Health. 2014;11(5):942–9. https://doi.org/10.1123/jpah.2012-0247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Ricardo CZ, Azeredo CM, de Rezende LFM, Levy RB. Co-occurrence and clustering of the four major non-communicable disease risk factors in Brazilian adolescents: analysis of a national school-based survey. PLoS One. 2019;14(7):1–13. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219370.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Anderson S, Donlan AE, McDermott ER, Zaff JF. Ecology matters: neighborhood differences in the protective role of self-control and social support for adolescent antisocial behavior. Am J Orthop. 2015;85(6):536–49. https://doi.org/10.1037/ort0000124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Paschke LM, Dörfel D, Steimke R, Trempler I, Magrabi A, Ludwig VU, et al. Individual differences in self-reported self-control predict successful emotion regulation. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2016;11(8):1193–204. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw036.

  15. Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky D, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington HL, et al. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(7):2693–8. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1010076108.

  16. Agbaria Q, Daher W. School violence among Arab adolescents in Israel and its relation to self-control skills and social support. Psychol Rep. 2015;117(1):1–7. https://doi.org/10.2466/16.21.PR0.117c12z2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Harris C, Scarpate JM, Vazsonyi AT. Parental and kinship ties, and low self-control: violence perpetration among rural African American adolescents from the Black Belt. J Adolesc [internet]. 2020;85(October):115–9. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2020.10.002, 2020.

  18. Cretacci MA. A general test of self-control theory: has its importance been exaggerated? Int J Offender Ther Comp Criminol. 2008;52(5):538–53. https://doi.org/10.1177/0306624X07308665.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Fine A, Steinberg L, Frick PJ, Cauffman E. Self-control assessments and implications for predicting adolescent offending. J Youth Adolesc. 2016;45(4):701–12. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-016-0425-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Kim TE, Guerra NG, Williams KR. Preventing youth problem behaviors and enhancing physical health by promoting Core competencies. J Adolesc Health. 2008;43(4):401–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.02.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Mears DP, Cochran JC, Beaver KM. Self-control theory and nonlinear effects on offending. J Quant Criminol. 2013;29(3):447–76. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-012-9187-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Muftić LR, Updegrove AH. The mediating effect of self-control on parenting and delinquency: a gendered approach with a multinational sample. Int J Offender Ther Comp Criminol. 2017;62(10):3058–76. https://doi.org/10.1177/0306624X17725732.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Özdemir Y, Vazsonyi AT, Çok F. Parenting processes and aggression: the role of self-control among Turkish adolescents. J Adolesc. 2013;36(1):65–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.09.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Wang FM, Chen JQ, Xiao WQ, Ma YT, Zhang M. Peer physical aggression and its association with aggressive beliefs, empathy, self-control, and cooperation skills among students in a rural town of China. J Interpers Violence. 2012;27(16):3252–67. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260512441256.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Wikström POH, Svensson R. When does self-control matter? The interaction between morality and self-control in crime causation. Eur J Criminol. 2010;7(5):395–410. https://doi.org/10.1177/1477370810372132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Ribeaud D, Eisner M. The ‘drug–crime link’ from a self-control perspective: an empirical test in a Swiss youth sample. Eur J Criminol. 2006;3(1):33–67. https://doi.org/10.1177/1477370806059080.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Wood PB, Pfefferbaum B, Arneklev BJ. Risk-taking and self-control: social psychological correlates of delinquency. J Crime Justice. 1993;16(1):111–30. https://doi.org/10.1080/0735648X.1993.9721481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Yun I, Kim SG, Kwon S. Low self-control among south Korean adolescents. Int J Offender Ther Comp Criminol. 2015;60(10):1185–208. https://doi.org/10.1177/0306624X15574683.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Leimberg A, Lehmann PS. Unstructured socializing with peers, low self-control, and substance use. Int J Offender Ther Comp Criminol. 2020:0306624X2096793. https://doi.org/10.1177/0306624X20967939.

  30. Chui WH, Chan HC (Oliver). Association between self-control and school bullying behaviors among Macanese adolescents. Child Abus Negl [Internet]. 2013;37(4):237–42. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2012.12.003

  31. Moon B, Alarid LF. School bullying, low self-control, and opportunity. J Interpers Violence. 2014;30(5):839–56. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260514536281.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Kang NG, You MA. Association of perceived stress and self-control with health-promoting behaviors in adolescents: a cross-sectional study. Med (United States). 2018;97(34):1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Wills TA, Isasi CR, Mendoza D, Ainette MG. Self-control constructs related to measures of dietary intake and physical activity in adolescents. J Adolesc Health. 2007;41(6):551–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.06.013.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Williams RJ, Ricciardelli LA. Negative perceptions about self-control and identification with gender-role stereotypes related to binge eating, problem drinking, and to co-morbidity among adolescents. J Adolesc Health. 2003;32(1):66–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1054-139X(02)00454-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Xiang MQ, Lin L, Wang ZR, Li J, Xu Z, Hu M. Sedentary behavior and problematic smartphone use in Chinese adolescents: the moderating role of self-control. Front Psychol. 2020;10(January):1–9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.03032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Miller H, Barnes JC, Beaver K. Self-control and health outcomes in a nationally representative sample. Am J Health Behav. 2011;35(1):15–27. https://doi.org/10.5993/ajhb.35.1.2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. de Winter AF, Visser L, Verhulst FC, Vollebergh WAM, Reijneveld SA. Longitudinal patterns and predictors of multiple health risk behaviors among adolescents: The TRAILS study. Prev Med (Baltim) [Internet]. 2016;84:76–82. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2015.11.028.

  38. Schneider A. Visitas domiciliares para promoção do desenvolvimento infantil: lições do programa Nurse-Family Partnership. In: Cardia N, Alves R, Astolfi R, editors. Visistação domiciliar. São Paulo: Edusp; 2016. p. 512.

  39. Doyle O, Harmon CP, Heckman JJ, Tremblay RE. Investing in early human development: timing and economic efficiency. Econ Hum Biol [Internet]. 2009 Mar;7(1):1–6. Available from: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1570677X09000045. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ehb.2009.01.002.

  40. Pandey A, Hale D, Das S, Goddings AL, Blakemore SJ, Viner RM. Effectiveness of universal self-regulation-based interventions in children and adolescents a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2018;172(6):566–75. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.0232.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Wanzinack C, Signorelli MC, Reis C. Homicides and socio-environmental determinants of health in Brazil: A systematic literature review. Cad Saude Publica. 2018;34(12).

  42. Trajtenberg N, Eisner M. Towards a more Effective Violence Prevention Policy in Uruguay. Montevidéu; 2015.

  43. Behr D, Shishido K. The translation of measurement instruments for cross-cultural surveys. In: Wolf C, Joye D, Smith TW, Fu Y, editors. Handbook of survey Methodology. SAGE Publications Ltd; 2016. p. 269–87, The Translation of Measurement Instruments for Cross-Cultural Surveys, https://doi.org/10.4135/9781473957893.n19.

  44. Van Widenfelt BM, Treffers PDA, De Beurs E, Siebelink BM, Koudijs E. Translation and cross-cultural adaptation of assessment instruments used in psychological research with children and families. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2005;8(2):135–47. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-005-4752-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Alsaker F. Mutig gegen Mobbing in Kindergarten und Schule [Brave against mobbing in kindergarten and school]. Bern, Switzerland: Hans Huber; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Murray AL, Eisner M, Ribeaud D, Kaiser D, McKenzie K, Murray G. Validation of a Brief Self-Report Measure of Adolescent Bullying Perpetration and Victimization. Assessment. 2019, Validation of a Brief Self-Report Measure of Adolescent Bullying Perpetration and Victimization;

  47. Grasmick HG, Tittle CR, Bursik RJ, Arneklev BJ. Testing the core empirical implications of gottfredson and hirschi’s general theory of crime. J Res Crime Delinq. 1993;30(1):5–29. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022427893030001002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Murray AL, Obsuth I, Eisner M, Ribeaud D. Shaping aggressive personality in adolescence: Exploring cross-lagged relations between aggressive thoughts, aggressive behaviour and self-control. Pers Individ Dif [Internet]. 2016;97:1–7. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.022.

  49. IBGE - Instituto Brasileiro de Estatística e Geografia. PeNSE 2012: Pesquisa Nacional de Saúde do Escolar 2012 [Internet]. 2012. Available from: http://www.ibge.gov.br/

  50. Levy RB, Castro IRR. de, Cardoso L de O, Tavares LF, Sardinha LMV, Gomes F da S, et al. Consumo e comportamento alimentar entre adolescentes brasileiros: Pesquisa Nacional de Saúde do Escolar (PeNSE), 2009. Cien Saude Colet. 2010;15(suppl 2):3085–97. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-81232010000800013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Rebellon CJ, Straus MA, Medeiros R. Self-control in global perspective: An empirical assessment of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory within and across 32 national settings. Vol. 5, Eur J Crim 2008. 331–361 p.

  52. Shelton KK. Assessment of parenting practices in families of elementary school-age children. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 1996;33:590–600.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Weerman FM, Maxson CL, Esbensen F-A, Aldridge J, Medina J, van Gemer F. Eurogang program manual; 2009. 38 p.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Dahlberg LL, Toal SB, Swahn M, Behrens CB. Measuring violence-related attitudes, behaviors, and influence among youths: A compendium of assessment tools [Internet]. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Injury Prevention and Control. 2005. Available from: https://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/pdf/yv_compendium.pdf

  55. Gottfredson M. Hirschi T. The General Theory of Crime: Stanford University Press; 1990.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Kotabe HP, Hofmann W. On integrating the components of self-control. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015;10(5):618–38. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615593382.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

The British Academy/Newton Foundation (Award Reference AF160099) and Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo – FAPESP (The Foundation for Research Support of the State of São Paulo) (Process Nr. 2016/222594) provided the funds for the data collection, treatment and article revision upon respective funding. Neither institutions had any role on research decisions such as study design, the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data. Fapesp strongly advises funded researchers to publish results of funded projects, but with no intervention on or preview of content.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

This article is part of a broader study, the São Paulo Project for the Social Development of Children and Adolescents. The design of the larger study was performed by M.F.T.P. and M.E. MFTP coordinated data collection and treatment. MAL, CHGP and MR performed data treatment. All authors participated in the design of the analysis in this paper. The first draft of this manuscript was written by RCA and all authors commented on previous versions. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Roberta Corradi Astolfi.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The project was approved by the Committee of Ethics and Research of the School of Medicine of the University of São Paulo (process n. 1.719.856); by the National Commission on Ethics in Research (CONEP) (process n. 2.014.816); and by the Committee on Ethics in Research of the Institute of Criminology of the University of Cambridge. It was performed in accordance with the Education Department of the State of São Paulo (SEEP) and the Municipal Education Department of the Government of the City of São Paulo (SME/ SP).

For selected schools that accepted to take part, written information was provided to parents or guardians through a letter and a folder with information on the research objectives, methodological procedures adopted, and possible risks for the participants, guarantee of anonymity and confidentiality and contact with the researchers. Additionally, the parents were invited to a face-to-face meeting with the research team in each of the schools, before applying the questionnaires, when the information was again given and doubts were clarified. Parents or guardians were informed that participation was voluntary and they could express their refusal to participate through direct communication with school principals, with the research team by telephone or email, or even at the face-to-face meeting or after it, at any time. For all participants, parental consent was in the form of not opting out. This procedure was adopted to minimize selection bias and was approved by the ethics committee, once risk involved for participating adolescents was considered low. Written informed consent was given to adolescents and read in their presence and only those who agreed to participate were included. In the questionnaire, schools and students are identified by a number, so that anonymity cannot be disrespected. Data will be used for research purposes only.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The author(s) declare(s) that there is no conflict of interest regarding the publication of this article.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Appendix

Appendix

Table 6 Association between self-control (continuous) and HRBs among 9th grade elementary students. São Paulo, Brazil (SP-Proso study). N = 2106. São Paulo, 2017. OR adjusted (CI 95%)
Table 7 Association (adjusted only) between self-control (continuous) and MHRBs among 9th grade elementary students. São Paulo, Brazil (SP-Proso study). N = 2106. São Paulo, 2017
Table 8 Association between self-control (without items of physical activity) and sedentary behaviour among 9th grade elementary students. São Paulo, Brazil (SP-Proso study)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Astolfi, R.C., Leite, M.A., Papa, C.H.G. et al. Association between self-control and health risk behaviors: a cross-sectional study with 9th grade adolescents in São Paulo. BMC Public Health 21, 1706 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-11718-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-11718-4

Keywords