The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
ArticleFull Access

Risk of Violence by Psychiatric Patients: Beyond the "Actuarial Versus Clinical" Assessment Debate

Published Online:

Two statements about risk assessment that were usually regarded as true 30 years ago are not usually regarded as true today. The first is that psychiatric patients as a group are no more likely to engage in violent behavior than the general population ( 1 ). The second is that psychiatrists and psychologists are not able to predict which of their patients will act violently ( 2 ). The truth of the first statement is now seen as depending on, among other things, the group of patients being described ( 3 ). The second statement is now seen as untrue. A range of methods consistently predict violence at levels of accuracy better than chance ( 4 ).

These methods are usually characterized as actuarial, in which the prediction derives from a mathematical combining of known risk factors, or clinical ( 5 ). In violence risk research the adoption and adaptation of statistical approaches including the receiver operator characteristic (ROC) curve and the "number needed to treat" measure have improved researchers' ability to describe predictive accuracy. This article describes the levels of accuracy that can be achieved, examines the prospects for improvement, discusses the implications for clinical practice, and makes suggestions for future research.

The state of knowledge

Overall accuracy

The accuracy of validated actuarial and clinical approaches to the prediction of violence in populations with mental disorders improved between 1970 and 2000 ( Figure 1 ). The improvement was limited and characterized by a wide range in the published results. Actuarial methods outperformed clinical methods ( 4 ).

Figure 1 Indices of effectiveness of validated prediction studies of violence between 1970 and 2000

Before research can inform clinical practice, however, clinicians need to know not only that an improvement has occurred but also what level of accuracy that improvement now permits. Recent suggestions that actuarial approaches are inapplicable to the management of individual cases ( 6 ) have been challenged ( 7 , 8 ). Instead, actuarial methods and the instruments they employ are usually held to possess a predictive validity that can be measured using the same statistical techniques that are used elsewhere in medicine.

The area under the ROC curve (AUC) ranges from .5 (random) to 1.0 (perfect prediction) and is used in medicine and surgery to describe the accuracy of screening techniques ( 9 , 10 ). Its particular value as a measure of predictive accuracy derives from its being independent of the base rate and of the "cutting score," the point on a continuous scale at which a person is classified as dangerous in each validation study. Validation of the revised version of Hare's Psychopathy Checklist ( 11 ), the Violence Risk Assessment Guide (VRAG) ( 12 ), and the Historical, Clinical and Risk Management Scheme (HCR-20) ( 13 ) in the 1990s demonstrated AUCs of .72, .75, and .76 ( 14 , 15 , 16 ). This level of accuracy is similar to that achieved by criminal justice agencies in predicting sex-offender recidivism ( 17 ).

Since 2000 further validation of these instruments has generated AUCs ranging from .61 ( 18 ) to .82 ( 19 ). Their accuracy seems to generalize to nonforensic populations. The HCR-20 predicted violence in a general psychiatric sample with an AUC of .72 for men and .77 for women ( 20 ). It generalizes also to samples outside North America. A Danish validation of the VRAG reported an AUC of .73 ( 21 ). Newer instruments perform to a similar standard ( 22 ). A replication of the iterative classification tree, the algorithm on which Classification of Violence Risk software ( 23 ) operates, was published in 2005 and demonstrated AUCs of .63 and .70, depending on the outcome measure ( 24 ).

Calculations of the AUC most often assume an optimal cutoff score, something that can only be calculated after the outcome for each subject is known. Where the optimal score is not known, accuracy will be less ( 25 ). Translating figures for the AUC into numbers that are clinically meaningful is difficult for other reasons, too, not least because the clinical judgments that they might inform, such as whether to admit a patient to the hospital, derive from many considerations, not just one. One approach is to ask the following question: if a particular instrument was used as a screening test and those identified as likely to be violent were not discharged, over any given period how many patients would need to be detained to prevent one unwanted act?

This statistic, the number needed to detain, is the inverse of positive predictive value and analogous to the number needed to treat. It derives from sensitivity, specificity, and base rate ( 26 ). The AUC of .75, of the VRAG, was generated with a procedure that classified patients as violent or nonviolent with a sensitivity level of .73 and a specificity level of .63 ( 15 ). Used as a screening test where the base rate of violence is 10% and where, as a result, an unselective approach would lead to the detention of ten people in order to prevent one from acting violently, the VRAG would require the detention of five people to achieve the same end.

The number needed to detain rises as prevalence of violence falls ( Figure 2 ). One consequence is that the number of mistakes rises when unusual acts, such as acts of serious violence, are sought to be prevented. At the base rate recorded in the Epidemiologic Catchment Area study, where 17% of the sample self-reported violence in the previous 12 months, the number needed to detain to prevent an act of violence is 3.5. The outcome measure in the study was not conditional on violence that resulted in injury, however. When injury is required by the outcome measure, the base rate falls. In the CATIE (Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness) study, the six-month prevalence of assault with a weapon or causing serious injury was 3.6% ( 27 ). Here the number needed to detain at a sensitivity of .73 and a specificity of .63 is 15.

Figure 2 Relationship between the number needed to detain and the prevalence of violence

A second consequence of the relationship between number needed to detain and prevalence shown in Figure 2 is that at low base rates, prevalence becomes more important, and the psychometric qualities of a test less important, in determining the number of correct predictions. At the prevalence rates seen in most psychiatric outpatient settings, even a substantial improvement in an instrument's psychometric qualities may have limited effect. At the 3.6% base rate in the CATIE study, for instance, a 20% increase in sensitivity, all other things being equal, reduces the number needed to detain only to 13.

Relative accuracy in particular situations

Different risk factors apply and the same factors apply with different weights in different samples ( 28 ). Mental disorder seems to be more important, for instance, when base rates are low ( 29 ). Presumably for these reasons, the same instruments achieve different levels of accuracy in different populations ( 30 , 31 ). This has cost implications for actuarial approaches: the same techniques must be revalidated before their accuracies can be stated. By implication, although it is mentioned less often, equivalent concerns apply to clinical approaches. This heterogeneity also suggests, however, that in particular situations greater accuracy may be achievable.

For instance, are predictions more accurate when they cover only the near future? Reviews that control for base rates suggest that the accuracy of clinical and actuarial predictions is unaffected by the duration over which the prediction is made ( 32 ). Clinicians have expressed the opposite view, that predictions are more accurate when they are made over short periods ( 33 , 34 , 35 ). In theory, because clinicians talk to patients, they can have access to the reasons someone might act violently ( 5 ). Because much violence associated with a psychiatric disorder is not planned, however, it may be that any consequent improvement in the ability to predict violence is too short-lived to be demonstrated by the usual research methods ( 32 ).

Prediction may also be more feasible when patients have particular complaints. "Threat control override" symptoms, combining persecutory ideas and passivity experiences, have been linked to violent behavior in patient and community samples from the United States ( 36 , 37 ) and Israel ( 38 ). The association has not been demonstrated consistently, however, and if it does exist it seems to operate differently in different populations. At least for threat control override symptoms and violence occurring within the same 12-month period, the association seems to disappear when substance abuse is controlled for ( 37 ). It may disappear also when the client is in treatment ( 39 ). Studies of discharges from inpatient units have also shown no consistent association ( 40 , 41 ), although this may be due to a failure to allow for a differential effect among men and women ( 42 ).

A third possibility is that particular types of offending "breed true," offering the potential to reduce violence by identifying particular situations and potential victims. Criminological research demonstrates little of the "specialization" in offending that would allow this, however ( 43 , 44 , 45 ), and what there is may not help with prevention. Although sexual offenders are more likely than others to commit the same type of offense on successive occasions ( 46 ), a sexual offense against a child is still seven times more likely to be committed by a general offender than by someone with a history of sexual convictions ( 47 ). Fewer data are available for psychiatric samples. It may be that patients who have assaulted people before hospitalization are more likely to choose the same victims when they assault people after they are discharged ( 48 ). Those who offend after release from maximum-security hospitals, on the other hand, seem to display the same heterogeneity, between pre- and postdischarge offenses, evident in criminological research ( 49 ).

The potential for change in the current state of knowledge

Used as screening tests to detain the risky at the rates of violence usually encountered in psychiatric populations, therefore, current approaches can prevent the violent acts of a few only by detaining many. Combining the results of several instruments seems not to help ( 50 ). There are some reasons to expect, however, that accuracy will improve.

First, some of the validation studies that have been conducted, for instance on people who have already committed criminal acts, are victims of Gordon's partialing error ( 51 ). Gordon noted that the accuracy of any prediction is reduced when it is applied to a sample, such as a group of patients detained on the grounds of dangerousness, which is less heterogeneous, in terms of the independent variables linked to offending, than the general population. Where all of the patients have criminal records, for instance, criminal record is a poor predictor. Second, the distribution of predictive accuracy when plotted against variables that contribute to risk, such as gender, is not known. There are suggestions that clinicians are more accurate when predicting violence among men ( 52 ), and it may be that there are other patient groups for whom more accurate predictions are possible.

There are also, however, reasons to suspect that any future improvement in predictive accuracy will be limited. First, although any improvement shown in Figure 1 appears marginal, what seems unequivocally to have improved is methodology. Early reports used retrospective methods and, as independent variables, either release decisions ( 53 , 54 ) or variables obtained for other purposes ( 55 ). The outcome variable typically derived from a single source ( 56 ).

The MacArthur Violence Risk Assessment Study, by contrast, used a sample of 1,136 hospital discharges from multiple sites, 134 variables chosen for their relationship to violence, a prospective method, and an outcome measure derived from multiple interviews and collateral information ( 24 , 57 , 58 ). Most methodological flaws—the lack of a reliable measure of violent behavior, for instance—will reduce accuracy. The improvement in accuracy shown in Figure 1 may be a product of improved methodology. The important questions then become what further improvement is possible and whether the same results can be achieved in clinical settings.

Second, most models of prediction have as a premise that violence is stochastic, or random, only at the level of the population. That is to say, certain individuals within each discharge cohort will offend, and any limits to predictive accuracy derive from an inability to identify those individuals. It is possible, however, that violence is stochastic also at the level of the individual ( 51 ) and that if the tape could somehow be rerun, a different group of patients would offend. Even an intermediate position, where violence is stochastic at both levels, implies a ceiling on the accuracy that can be achieved by both clinical and actuarial approaches.

Potential ways forward

Psychiatric texts nevertheless continue to emphasize the contribution of violence risk assessment to clinical management ( 59 , 60 ). As a result, the question of how this should be done remains an important one for psychiatric research. One way forward would be to make a clearer distinction between two types of variables that correlate with patient violence.

Epidemiological and behavioral variables

Violent behavior is commoner, by a factor of 3, among psychiatric patients than among people who have never received psychiatric treatment ( 61 ). Men with schizophrenia are four times more likely than other men to be convicted of a crime of violence ( 62 ). Violence is commoner, by a factor of 6, in cross-sectional community surveys when the individual has a mental disorder ( 63 ). Using control groups from the same neighborhood reduces the effect of social drift and leads to lower odds ratios ( 64 ). Higher ratios are reported in countries where the crime rate is lower ( 65 , 66 , 67 ). These reports are forms of epidemiology. They demonstrate patterns of association between violence and mental disorder.

Odds ratios associated with past behavior are typically higher. Members of the 1945 Philadelphia Birth Cohort who were chronically delinquent as teenagers were 14 times more likely to be convicted four or more times as adults ( 68 ). English inner-city boys convicted as juveniles were nine times more likely than their unconvicted peers to be convicted again between the ages of 25 and 32 ( 69 , 70 ). An adult with a previous violent conviction is 14 times more likely than someone with a nonviolent conviction to commit a violent offense in the future ( 71 ). Substance abuse in community samples has been linked to violence at odds ratios of 20 for men and 32 for women ( 72 ).

The scale of the odds ratios associated with criminal behavior and drug use may explain why past violence alone, taken in conjunction with sociodemographic variables, is capable of predicting a majority of violent acts that can be predicted using more exhaustive methods ( 73 ). It seems likely that criminal history variables, or others acting as proxies for these, contribute much of the predictive power of actuarial instruments. The predictive accuracy of even the simplest behavioral measures seems to exceed the predictive power of diagnosis. Patients who are violent in the week before admission are nine times more likely to be violent in the two weeks after discharge ( 48 ).

These two types of variable, the one derived from the patient's diagnosis or overall clinical condition and the other derived from his or her behavior, may in the future be seen as serving different roles. Behaviorally derived variables have proved better able to distinguish those patients who will go on to act violently, albeit not yet at levels that can prevent high numbers needed to detain at the base rates of violence seen in clinical practice. Clinically derived variables, on the other hand, seem better placed to offer insights into the mechanisms by which a minority of people with mental disorders act violently.

Insights into the mechanism of violence

Risk management requires some understanding of the "causal nexus" ( 74 , 75 ) linking mental disorder to violence. Research has controlled for demographic characteristics, including age, sex, race, and marital status. But other variables linked to violence in the general population, such as an unstable family environment ( 76 ), unemployment ( 77 ), and television viewing ( 78 ), have received less attention. At least two of these, victimization ( 79 ) and community disorganization ( 80 ), correlate with mental disorder ( 81 ). When included in epidemiological analyses, they may go some way to explain previously observed associations. Threat control override symptoms, for instance, may be more strongly associated with violence in particular domestic settings.

It is also the case that potential confounders, such as substance abuse, can be present to a greater or lesser extent. An association between mental disorder and violence that persists after controlling for drug dependence does not exclude the possibility that a disproportionate number of individuals with mental disorders were using substances at a nondependent level. Violence is associated with substance use at levels that fail to meet diagnostic criteria ( 82 ). Residual confounding ( 83 ) of this kind may explain why Scandinavian studies that show the highest odds ratios for substance abuse also report high odds ratios with respect to mental disorder.

Further epidemiological research in these areas may assist clinicians in three ways. One is by offering a better understanding of the temporal relationship between psychotic symptoms and violence. The causal implications of symptoms occurring before and after a violent act are obviously different, but methodologies have not always distinguished the two ( 27 ). A second concerns the roles of delusions, command-type hallucinations, and flattening of affect ( 84 , 85 ) and their interaction with protective factors, such as treatment adherence ( 86 , 87 ). Of particular importance is the nature of the association between violence and substance use among persons with mental illness. It is still unclear whether the association stems from the effects of intoxication, from exacerbation of symptoms, or from participation in a social milieu where violence is common.

Practice-based research

The value to clinical management of behaviorally derived variables that have been linked to violence should be investigated by practice-based research. This could validate the use of actuarial instruments in clinical settings. Clinicians are more accurate when they base their judgments on test scores and not on the data that generate those scores ( 88 ), perhaps because tests are less likely than people to be overwhelmed by large amounts of information ( 89 ).

These validation studies should select the most relevant outcome variables for each area of clinical practice. If an instrument is to be used in civil commitment determinations, for instance, its predictive validity needs to be established not in relation to all violent acts but in relation to the kind of behavior occurring sufficiently close to the point of discharge that, had it been foreseen, would have justified continued hospitalization ( 90 ). Research should examine also what types of information are most useful. There is evidence, for instance, that clinicians' judgments have predictive power even when they are made without knowledge of the patient's history of violence ( 52 ).

Conclusions

The accuracy of clinical and actuarial predictions of violence among patients consistently exceeds chance, but the number of errors is related to the base rate of violence in the population. At the rates seen in outpatient settings, current approaches, if used to screen and detain within hospitals those predicted to be violent, would result in the detention of many people to prevent each violent act. In these circumstances predictions of violence have to remain only some of the many considerations guiding clinical judgment. The relation between prevalence of violence and the number needed to detain means that this state of affairs is unlikely to change even if the psychometric qualities of prediction methods show further substantial improvement.

This is different from saying that research in this area does not benefit clinical practice ( 91 , 92 ). Unless a risk of harm to others ceases to be a criterion for voluntary and compulsory admission to a hospital, that risk will continue to require to be assessed using the best methods available. There is a need to improve the quality of those assessments and to address the implications of what research has already shown—for instance, that the same approaches achieve different levels of accuracy in different circumstances.

Risk management that means more than detaining people requires the ability to anticipate dangerous situations and prevent the circumstances from recurring. By describing the correlates of violence and the roles of confounding variables, epidemiological methods offer the prospect of clarifying the causal nexus behind some violent acts. Some correlates of violence, such as drug use, seem to offer the opportunity to intervene regardless of the nature of the causal link ( 28 , 93 ). Practice-based research can test these opportunities and, by concentrating on behavioral variables, may be able to offer greater predictive accuracy to clinicians.

Practice-based research should investigate also how actuarial and clinical approaches can best be combined. It is likely that this will vary according to the circumstances in which a prediction is required. Actuarial schemes will never detect most instances where someone acts violently for the first time because the most powerful predictor in actuarial schemes is past behavior. The same actuarial approaches may nevertheless be all that is feasible when no clinical assessment has been conducted ( 94 ). To be widely adopted, any approach will have to take into account the limitations that apply to staff time and resources in most community psychiatric settings.

The question of whether, as a result of their improved accuracy, actuarial instruments now warrant more widespread clinical use is an empirical one. The authors of an equivalent tool for use by criminal justice agencies have recommended that the score it generates be used as a benchmark against which a clinical assessment and recommendation can be reviewed ( 95 ). In the present state of knowledge, a similar approach seems a reasonable response to clinical uncertainty in psychiatry. In the future, practice-based research may show that actuarial instruments should have a more extensive role and what that role might be.

Acknowledgments and disclosures

The author reports no competing interests.

Dr. Buchanan is with the Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, 34 Park St., New Haven, CT 06519 (e-mail: [email protected]).

References

1. Rabkin J: Criminal behavior of discharged mental patients: a critical appraisal of the research. Psychological Bulletin 86:1–27, 1979Google Scholar

2. American Psychiatric Association: Task Force Report Number 8: Clinical Aspects of the Violent Individual. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 1974Google Scholar

3. Marzuk P: Violence, crime and mental illness: how strong a link? Archives of General Psychiatry 53:481–486, 1996Google Scholar

4. Mossman D: Assessing predictions of violence: being accurate about accuracy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 62:783–792, 1994Google Scholar

5. Buchanan A: Risk and dangerousness. Psychological Medicine 29:465–473, 1999Google Scholar

6. Hart S, Michie C, Cooke D: Precision of actuarial risk assessment instruments. British Journal of Psychiatry 190(suppl): s60–s65, 2007Google Scholar

7. Mossman D, Sellke TM: Avoiding errors about "margins of error." British Journal of Psychiatry, 2007. Available at bjp.rcpsych.org/cgi/eletters/190/49/s60Google Scholar

8. Harris G, Rice M, Quinsey V: Abandoning evidence-based risk appraisal in forensic practice: comments on Hart et al. British Journal of Psychiatry, 2007. Available at bjp.rcpsych.org/cgi/eletters/190/49/s60Google Scholar

9. Umans-Eckenhausen M, Defesche J, Sijbrands E, et al: Review of first 5 years of screening for familial hypercholesterolaemia in the Netherlands. Lancet 357:165–168, 2001Google Scholar

10. Partin A, Kattan M, Subong E, et al: Combination of prostate-specific antigen, clinical stage, and Gleason score to predict pathological stage of localized prostate cancer. JAMA 277:1445–1451, 1997Google Scholar

11. Hare R: The Revised Psychopathy Checklist. Toronto, Canada, Multi-Health Systems, 1991Google Scholar

12. Harris G, Rice M, Quinsey V: Violent recidivism of mentally disordered offenders: the development of a statistical prediction instrument. Criminal Justice and Behavior 20:315–335, 1993Google Scholar

13. Webster C, Douglas K, Eaves D, et al: HCR–20: Assessing Risk for Violence (Version 2). Vancouver, Canada, Simon Fraser University, Mental Health, Law, and Policy Institute, 1997Google Scholar

14. Grann M, Långström N, Tengström A, et al: Psychopathy (PCL-R) predicts violent recidivism among criminal offenders with personality disorders in Sweden. Law and Human Behavior 23:205–217, 1999Google Scholar

15. Rice M, Harris G: Violent recidivism: assessing predictive validity. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 63:737–748, 1995Google Scholar

16. Douglas KS, Ogloff JRP, Nicholls TL, et al: Assessing risk for violence among psychiatric patients: the HCR-20 violence risk assessment scheme and the Psychopathy Checklist: screening version. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 67:917–930, 1999Google Scholar

17. Janus E, Prentky R: Forensic use of actuarial risk assessment with sex offenders: accuracy, admissibility and accountability. American Criminal Law Review 40:1443–1499, 2003Google Scholar

18. McNeil D, Gregory A, Lam J, et al: Utility of decision support tools for assessing acute risk of violence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 71:945–953, 2003Google Scholar

19. Douglas K, Yeomans M, Boer D: Comparative validity analysis of multiple measures of violence risk in a sample of criminal offenders. Criminal Justice and Behavior 32:479–510, 2005Google Scholar

20. Nicholls T, Ogloff J, Douglas K: Assessing risk for violence among male and female civil psychiatric patients: the HCR-20, PCL:SV, and VSC. Behavioral Sciences and the Law 22:127–158, 2004Google Scholar

21. Urbaniok F, Noll T, Grunewald S, et al: Prediction of violent and sexual offences: a replication study of the VRAG in Switzerland. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology 17:23–31, 2006Google Scholar

22. Grann M, Sturidsson K, Haggård-Grann U, et al: Methodological development: structured outcome assessment and community risk monitoring (SORM). International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 28:442–456, 2005Google Scholar

23. Monahan J, Steadman H, Appelbaum P, et al: The classification of violence risk. Behavioral Sciences and the Law 24:721–730, 2006Google Scholar

24. Monahan J, Steadman H, Robbins P, et al: An actuarial model of violence risk assessment for persons with mental disorders. Psychiatric Services 56:810–815, 2005Google Scholar

25. Vrieze S, Grove W: Predicting sex offender recidivism: I. correcting for item overselection and accuracy overestimation in scale development. II. sampling error-induced attenuation of predictive validity over base rate information. Law and Human Behavior, in pressGoogle Scholar

26. Buchanan A, Leese M: Detention of people with dangerous severe personality disorders. Lancet 358:1955–1959, 2001Google Scholar

27. Swanson J, Swartz M, Van Dorn R, et al: A national study of violent behavior in persons with schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 63:490–499, 2006Google Scholar

28. Kraemer H, Kazdin A, Offord D, et al: Coming to terms with the terms of risk. Archives of General Psychiatry 54:337–343, 1997Google Scholar

29. Coid J: The epidemiology of abnormal homicide and murder followed by suicide. Psychological Medicine 13:855–860, 1983Google Scholar

30. Mossman D: Another look at interpreting risk categories. Sexual Abuse 18:41–63, 2006Google Scholar

31. Fujii D, Tokioka A, Lichton A, et al: Ethnic differences in prediction of violence risk with the HCR-20 among psychiatric inpatients. Psychiatric Services 56:711–716, 2005Google Scholar

32. Mossman D: Commentary: assessing the risk of violence: are accurate predictions useful? Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 28:272–281, 2000Google Scholar

33. Binder R: Are the mentally ill dangerous? Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 27:189–201, 1999Google Scholar

34. Hoptman M, Yates K, Patalinjug M, et al: Clinical prediction of assaultive behavior among male psychiatric patients at a maximum-security forensic facility. Psychiatric Services 50:1461–1466, 1999Google Scholar

35. Monahan J: The prediction of violent behavior: toward a second generation of theory and policy. American Journal of Psychiatry 141:10–15, 1984Google Scholar

36. Link B, Stueve A: Psychotic symptoms and the violent/illegal behavior of mental patients compared to community controls, in Violence and Mental Disorder. Edited by Monahan J, Steadman H. Chicago, University of Chicago Press, 1994Google Scholar

37. Swanson J, Borum R, Swartz M, et al: Psychotic symptoms and disorders and the risk of violent behavior in the community. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health 6:309–329, 1996Google Scholar

38. Link B, Monahan J, Stueve A, et al: Real in their consequences: a sociological approach to understanding the association between psychotic symptoms and violence. American Sociological Review 64:316–332, 1999Google Scholar

39. Swanson J, Estroff S, Swartz M et al: Violence and severe mental disorder in clinical and community populations: the effects of psychotic symptoms, co-morbidity and lack of treatment. Psychiatry 60:1–22, 1997Google Scholar

40. Appelbaum P, Robbins P, Monahan J: Violence and delusions: data from the MacArthur Violence Risk Assessment Study. American Journal of Psychiatry 157:566–572, 2000Google Scholar

41. Stompe T, Ortwein-Swoboda G, Schanda H: Schizophrenia, delusional symptoms, and violence: the threat/control override concept reexamined. Schizophrenia Bulletin 30:31–44, 2004Google Scholar

42. Teasdale B, Silver E, Monahan J: Gender, threat/control-override delusions and violence. Law and Human Behavior 30:649–658, 2006Google Scholar

43. Farrington D: Human development and criminal careers, in The Oxford Handbook of Criminology, 2nd ed. Edited by Maguire M, Morgan R, Reiner R. Oxford, United Kingdom, Oxford University Press, 1997Google Scholar

44. Hanson R, Bussière M: Predicting relapse: a meta-analysis of sexual offender recidivism studies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 66:348–362, 1998Google Scholar

45. Chaiken J, Chaiken M, Rhodes W: Predicting violent behavior and classifying violent offenders, in Understanding and Preventing Violence, Vol 4: Consequences and Control. Edited by Reiss A, Roth J. Washington, DC, National Academies Press, 1994Google Scholar

46. Stander J, Farrington D, Hill G, et al: Markov chain analysis and specialization in criminal careers. British Journal of Criminology 29:317–335, 1989Google Scholar

47. Bureau of Justice Statistics: Recidivism of Sex Offenders Released From Prison in 1994. Washington, DC, US Department of Justice, 2003Google Scholar

48. Tardiff K, Marzuk P, Leon A, et al: A prospective study of violence by psychiatric patients after hospital discharge. Psychiatric Services 48:678–681, 1997Google Scholar

49. Buchanan A, Reiss D, Taylor PJ: Does "like predict like" when patients discharged from high secure hospitals re-offend? An instrument to describe serious offences. Psychological Medicine 33:549–553, 2003Google Scholar

50. Seto M: Is more better? Combining actuarial risk scales to predict recidivism among adult sex offenders. Psychological Assessment 17:156–167, 2005Google Scholar

51. Gordon R: A critique of the evaluation of Patuxent Institution, with particular attention to the issues of dangerousness and recidivism. Bulletin of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 5:210–255, 1977Google Scholar

52. Lidz C, Mulvey E, Gardner W: The accuracy of predictions of violence to others. JAMA 269:1007–1011, 1993Google Scholar

53. Cocozza J, Steadman H: The failure of psychiatric predictions of dangerousness: clear and convincing evidence. Rutgers Law Review 29:1084–1101, 1976Google Scholar

54. Steadman H: A new look at recidivism among Patuxent inmates. Bulletin of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 5:200–209, 1977Google Scholar

55. Hedlund J, Sletten I, Altman H, et al: Prediction of patients who are dangerous to others. Journal of Clinical Psychology 29:443–447, 1973Google Scholar

56. Kozol H, Boucher R, Garofalo R: The diagnosis and treatment of dangerousness. Crime and Delinquency 18:371–392, 1972Google Scholar

57. Steadman H, Mulvey E, Monahan J, et al: Violence by people discharged from acute psychiatric inpatient facilities and by others in the same neighborhoods. Archives of General Psychiatry 55:393–401, 1998Google Scholar

58. Monahan J, Steadman J, Appelbaum P, et al: Developing a clinically useful actuarial tool for assessing violence risk. British Journal of Psychiatry 176:312–319, 2000Google Scholar

59. Gelder M, López-Ibor J, Andreasen N (eds): New Oxford Textbook of Psychiatry. Oxford, United Kingdom, Oxford University Press, 2003Google Scholar

60. Hales R, Yudofsky S (eds): Textbook of Clinical Psychiatry, 4th ed. Arlington, Va, Amercian Psychiatric Publishing, 2003Google Scholar

61. Link B, Andrews H, Cullen F: The violent and illegal behavior of mental patients reconsidered. American Sociological Review 57:275–292, 1992Google Scholar

62. Wallace C, Mullen P, Burgess P, et al:, Serious criminal offending and mental disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry 172:477–484, 1998Google Scholar

63. Swanson J, Holzer C, Ganju V, et al: Violence and psychiatric disorder in the community: evidence from the Epidemiologic Catchment Area surveys. Hospital and Community Psychiatry 41:761–770, 1990Google Scholar

64. Wessely SC, Castle D, Douglas AJ, et al: The criminal careers of incident cases of schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 24:483–502, 1994Google Scholar

65. Eronen M, Hakola P, Tiihonen J: Mental disorders and homicidal behavior in Finland. Archives of General Psychiatry 53:497–501, 1996Google Scholar

66. Tiihonen J, Isohanni M, Räsänen P, et al: Specific major mental disorders and criminality: a 26 year prospective study of the 1966 northern Finland birth cohort. American Journal of Psychiatry 154:840–845, 1997Google Scholar

67. Hodgins S, Mednick S, Brennan P, et al: Mental disorder and crime. Archives of General Psychiatry 53:489–496, 1996Google Scholar

68. Wolfgang M, Thornberry T, Figlio R: From Boy to Man, From Delinquency to Crime. Chicago, University of Chicago Press, 1987Google Scholar

69. Farrington D: Human development and criminal careers, in Oxford Handbook of Criminology. Edited by Maguire R, Morgan R, Reiner R. Oxford, United Kingdom, Oxford University Press, 1994Google Scholar

70. Farrington D, Loeber R, Elliot D, et al: Advancing knowledge about the onset of delinquency and crime. Advances in Clinical Child Psychology 13:283–342, 1990Google Scholar

71. Walker N, Hammond W, Steer D: Repeated violence. Criminal Law Review, 465–473, 1967Google Scholar

72. Hodgins S: Mental disorder, intellectual deficiency, and crime: evidence from a birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry 49:476–483, 1992Google Scholar

73. Buchanan A, Leese M: Quantifying the contributions of three types of information to the prediction of criminal conviction using the receiver operating characteristic. British Journal of Psychiatry 188:472–478, 2006Google Scholar

74. Mullen P: Forensic mental health. British Journal of Psychiatry 176:307–311, 2000Google Scholar

75. Arboleda-Flórez J, Stuart H: The future for risk research. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry 11:506–509, 2000Google Scholar

76. Raine A, Brennan P, Mednick B, et al: High rates of violence, crime, academic problems and behavioral problems in males with both early neuromotor deficits and unstable family environments. Archives of General Psychiatry 53:544–549, 1996Google Scholar

77. Catalano R, Dooley D, Novaco R, et al: Using ECA survey data to examine the effect of job layoffs on violent behavior. Hospital and Community Psychiatry 44:874–879, 1993Google Scholar

78. Johnson J, Cohen P, Smailes E, et al: Television viewing and aggressive behavior during adolescence and adulthood. Science 295:2468–2471, 2002Google Scholar

79. Kessler R, Molnar B, Feurer I, et al: Patterns and mental health predictors of domestic violence in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 24:487–508, 2001Google Scholar

80. Silver E: Extending social disorganization theory: a multilevel approach to the study of violence among persons with mental illnesses. Criminology 38:1043–1074, 2000Google Scholar

81. Hiday VA: Putting community risk in perspective: a look at correlations causes and controls. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 29:316–331, 2006Google Scholar

82. Swanson J, Van Dorn R, Swartz M et al: Alternative pathways to violence in persons with schizophrenia: the role of childhood antisocial behavior problems. Law and Human Behavior, in pressGoogle Scholar

83. Leon D: Failed or misleading adjustments for confounding. Lancet 342:479–481, 1993Google Scholar

84. Gur R, Kohler C, Ragland J, et al: Flat affect in schizophrenia: relation to emotion processing and neurocognitive measures. Schizophrenia Bulletin 32:279–287, 2006Google Scholar

85. Junginger J, McGuire L: Psychotic motivation and the paradox of current research on serious mental illness and rates of violence. Schizophrenia Bulletin 30:21–30, 2004Google Scholar

86. Elbogen E, Van Dorn R, Swanson J: Treatment engagement and violence risk in mental disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry 189:354–360, 2006Google Scholar

87. Monahan J, Steadman H, Silver E, et al: Rethinking Risk Assessment. Oxford, United Kingdom, Oxford University Press, 2001Google Scholar

88. Sawyer J: Measurement and prediction, clinical and statistical. Psychological Bulletin 66:178–200, 1966Google Scholar

89. Estes W: Of models and men. American Psychologist 12:609–617, 1957Google Scholar

90. Litwack T: Actuarial versus clinical assessments of dangerousness. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law 7:409–443, 2001Google Scholar

91. Steadman H: From dangerousness to risk assessment of community violence: taking stock at the turn of the century. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 28:265–271, 2000Google Scholar

92. Norko M: Commentary: dangerousness—a failed paradigm for clinical practice and service delivery. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 28:282–289, 2000Google Scholar

93. Lamberti JS: Understanding and preventing criminal recidivism among adults with psychotic disorders. Psychiatric Services 58:773–781, 2007Google Scholar

94. Meehl P: Clinical and Statistical Prediction. Minneapolis, University of Minnesota Press, 1966Google Scholar

95. Copas J, Marshall P: The Offender Group Reconviction Scale: a statistical reconviction score for use by probation officers. Applied Statistics 47:159–171, 1998Google Scholar