February 2020
Volume 9, Issue 3
Open Access
Articles  |   February 2020
The Effect of Simulated Visual Field Loss on Optokinetic Nystagmus
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Soheil M. Doustkouhi
    School of Optometry & Vision Science, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
    New Zealand National Eye Centre, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
  • Philip R. K. Turnbull
    School of Optometry & Vision Science, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
    New Zealand National Eye Centre, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
  • Steven C. Dakin
    School of Optometry & Vision Science, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
    New Zealand National Eye Centre, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
    UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, University College London, London, UK
  • Correspondence: Steven Dakin, School of Optometry & Vision Science, University of Auckland, 89 Grafton Road, Grafton, Auckland 1010, New Zealand. e-mail: s.dakin@auckland.ac.nz 
Translational Vision Science & Technology February 2020, Vol.9, 25. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/tvst.9.3.25
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Soheil M. Doustkouhi, Philip R. K. Turnbull, Steven C. Dakin; The Effect of Simulated Visual Field Loss on Optokinetic Nystagmus. Trans. Vis. Sci. Tech. 2020;9(3):25. https://doi.org/10.1167/tvst.9.3.25.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Purpose: Assessment of functional vision across the visual field is hampered by a reliance on patients' subjective judgement of the presence of a stimulus, and the accompanying demands (time and attention) this places on them. As a first step toward determining whether an objective measure of an involuntary eye movement (optokinetic nystagmus [OKN]) could provide an objective measure of field loss, we determined how various measures of OKN depend on the extent of simulated visual field loss (SVFL).

Methods: We used infrared eye-tracking to measure the eye movements of 16 healthy participants viewing horizontally translating 2-dimensional noise patterns over trials of varying contrasts and different levels of SVFL. We quantified the strength of OKN by estimating the velocity of tracking eye movements compared to the stimulus (OKN gain). These measurements were made using an open-loop SVFL paradigm, where a varying amount of gaze-contingent peripheral stimuli was occluded.

Results: Full-field stimulation led to an average OKN gain of 0.92 ± 0.15. This value fell steadily with increasing SVFL to a value of 0.38 ± 0.20 when the periphery was not stimulated at all (i.e., the stimulus was a 5-deg. diameter foveal patch). We note considerable individual variation in OKN gain in all conditions.

Conclusions: Measuring the extent of visual field loss using an objective measure of OKN gain is feasible.

Translational Relevance: Simulated visual field loss reduces optokinetic nystagmus, but further refinement of this technique would be required to overcome individual differences and to pick up clinically relevant field defects.

Introduction
Glaucoma is a progressive eye disease that causes loss of retinal ganglion cells RGCs1 and is the leading cause of irreversible blindness worldwide.2 Vision loss typically starts from the mid-periphery of the retina and may spread across the entire visual field.3 The function of the visual field is typically measured using white-on-white standard automated perimetry (SAP), which relies on the patient's ability to report a slight luminance difference of a stimulus against a uniform background.4 Although SAP remains the gold standard for assessing the integrity of the visual field,5 its sensitivity and accuracy are limited by several factors. These include the experience of the examiner,6 the time of day when testing is conducted,6 refractive blur,7,8 severity of field loss,9 and the specific instructions given to patients.10 Alternatives to SAP include short-wavelength automated perimetry, which probes the integrity of the short-wavelength cone system,4 frequency doubling, an illusion based on perceived rate of flicker,11 and motion automated perimetry, which attempts to isolate dysfunctional magnocellular processing using moving targets.12 These approaches can all detect functional vision loss arising from glaucoma at least as early as SAP.1215 
A variety of objective methods have been presented as alternatives to SAP. These include pattern ERG,16 multifocal ERG,17,18 and multifocal VEP,19,20 all of which require a high level of operator expertise and patient compliance and cooperation.21 It has also been proposed that eye movements can provide an objective assessment of a patient's visual status. For example, in eye movement perimetry (EMP) a patient's saccades to peripherally presented targets are used to quantify visual function across the field.2227 Using this paradigm, glaucoma patients require more contrast to initiate saccades, and those saccades tend to be less accurate than those made by controls.27 
Saccades are voluntary eye movements so that EMP (like SAP) requires a level of cooperation from the patient. In contrast, optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) is a reflexive eye movement made in response to stimulus motion. It consists of periods of smooth tracking in the stimulus-direction, interspersed with saccades in the opposite direction. OKN serves to reduce “retinal slip” by partially stabilising the moving image on the retina. The strength of OKN is measured using OKN gain, the ratio of the velocity of the slow phase of OKN to the velocity of the stimulus. Another way of quantifying OKN is by classifying the pattern of eye movements (tracking and saccades) as being consistent or inconsistent with an optokinetic response to the stimulus direction. This measure can serve as a proxy for “seen” or “unseen” responses from patients, respectively. Note that because OKN is an involuntary reflex, this procedure reduces the level of cooperation required from patients. 
Perimetric techniques measure local visual dysfunction by using stimuli that cover only a small portion of the visual field. OKN, on the other hand, requires relatively large stimuli.28 This means that detection of dysfunction arising from glaucoma would have to focus on quantifying a change in the optokinetic response to larger, global stimuli. Previous studies have used psychophysical motion coherence paradigms to show that glaucoma does compromise visual processing of global motion. Using full field (60- × 60-deg.) motion stimuli29,30 patients with glaucoma had motion thresholds 70% higher than the control group.29 
In terms of quantifying how different parts of the visual field might contribute to OKN, several groups have used selective retinal stimulation to address this issue, typically using masks to selectively occlude different regions of the stimulus within either an open- or closed-loop eye-tracking paradigm.28,3136 In open-loop experiments, the position of the mask is gaze-contingent, whereas in closed loop experiments the mask location is fixed. Such work has demonstrated that central vision plays a dominant role in the generation of OKN.28,3335,37 However, measurement of OKN in patients with central scotomas indicates that peripheral retina also contributes to OKN.32,3840 Here—as a first step toward developing objective measures of glaucomatous field loss based on eye-tracking—we sought to measure the effect of simulated visual field loss (SVFL) on OKN. 
Method
Study Design
Experiment 1 examined the effect of SVFL on OKN gain. Experiment 2 investigated the effect of simulated contrast loss for stimuli presented at 2 levels of SVFL. The 2 experiments were completed in a single visit, in a randomized order for each participant, with a minimum 5-minute break between each. Total testing time was approximately 17 minutes per participant. 
The experimental protocol and procedure were approved by the University of Auckland Human Research Ethics Committee (ref no. 019326). The experiment complied with the Declaration of Helsinki. Written consent was obtained from each participant, and participants were free to withdraw at any stage without giving a reason. 
Participants
Sixteen participants (21–50 years old, 9 females) took part in experiments to measure the OKN response of their right eyes. All participants either had had a recent eye examination (within 6 months) or, if they had not had a recent examination, were examined at the time of testing. Our exclusion criteria were amblyopia, or neurological disorders such as epilepsy. Fourteen of the 16 participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision in the tested (right) eye. This meant that they either did not habitually require correction (5/14), wore their correction (3/14), or that their residual refractive error (at the 1-m test distance) was between −0.13 and +1.13D (6/14). Hyperopic participants in this group could accommodate to overcome low levels of residual refractive error. The remaining 2 participants (P2 & P3) did not wear their habitual optical correction because it interfered with eye-tracking, and these individuals had residual refractive errors of −0.50D (P3) and −0.75D (P2) tested at 1.0 m. Such a modest level of residual refractive error will have a negligible impact on the visibility of the patterns as we discuss in the Discussion section. 
Experimental Setup
Stimuli were presented on a 621 × 341 mm LCD monitor (S2817Q; Dell, Round Rock, TX, USA) with a 3840 × 2160 pixel resolution running at 60Hz. At the one-meter test distance, the display subtended 32- × 19-deg. and had a pixel density of 120 pixels per degree. The monitor was linearized in software based on measurements made with a photometer (LS100, Konica Minolta, Japan). Experiments were written on the stimulus computer in Matlab (version 2017b, MathWorks, Natick, MA) using Psychophysics Toolbox41 and the Eyelink Toolbox.42 Monocular eye movements were recorded on a separate computer, using an infrared eye-tracker (Eyelink 1000 Plus; SR Research, Kanata, Ontario, Canada) in remote mode, that is, without the use of a chin rest, at 500 Hz. Data were streamed to the stimulus computer over a direct ethernet connection. The eye-tracker stream was sampled at 60 Hz, using the sample immediately after the screen refresh. Light levels in the study room were low (5.8 lux), with windows covered and monitors (other than the stimulus monitor) rotated away from the participant to minimize distraction. 
Stimulus
The stimulus was a spatial frequency filtered 2-dimensional random noise carrier, with a superimposed peripheral gray mask that occluded the carrier and served to simulate visual field loss (Fig. 1). 
Figure 1.
 
Stimulus generation. (a) The stimulus was constructed using two layers. The layer at the back is the isotropic noise carrier which drifted to the left or the right. The layer at the front is a masking image—note the carrier is visible through the transparent part—which moved with the patient's gaze. (b) The resulting stimulus after occluding the carrier with the mask. From left to right, either 3%, 39%, or 88% of the carrier is visible through the mask.
Figure 1.
 
Stimulus generation. (a) The stimulus was constructed using two layers. The layer at the back is the isotropic noise carrier which drifted to the left or the right. The layer at the front is a masking image—note the carrier is visible through the transparent part—which moved with the patient's gaze. (b) The resulting stimulus after occluding the carrier with the mask. From left to right, either 3%, 39%, or 88% of the carrier is visible through the mask.
The carrier stimulus was generated in Matlab using 2-dimensional Gaussian noise that was filtered with a logGabor filter to contain a narrow range of spatial-frequencies43 and all orientations. Random fluctuations in local contrast across the stimulus were minimized using a demodulation technique described previously.44 The mean gray-level of the carrier was equal to the mid gray-level of the screen (60 cd/m2). To minimize any influence of the visual acuity of our participants, the stimulus was highly supra-threshold with a peak spatial-frequency of 1.3 c/deg, a bandwidth of 0.5 octaves and a contrast of 100% (in Experiment 1). 
The gray mask (Fig. 1a) was also generated using logGabor filtered 2-dimensional Gaussian noise (0.26 c/deg, bandwidth 0.5 octaves), with a threshold imposed to binarize the stimulus. Values above the mean gray-level were set to the mean gray-level of the stimulus, and values below made transparent. A central disk (5-deg. diameter) within the mask of the gray mask was made transparent so that the carrier was always visible at fixation. The sharp edges of the mask were smoothed using a Gaussian blur kernel with a standard deviation of 4 pixels (2.4 arc min). 
The mask shifted position in a gaze-contingent manner (i.e., it moved independently of the carrier). The carrier moved horizontally (left or right) at 10 deg/s, for a duration of 2 seconds. Stimulus direction was randomly stratified to ensure equal numbers of leftwards and rightwards trials. A gray screen appeared between trials to minimize any build-up of motion adaptation. 
Experiment 1 had participants perform 8 trials at each of 9 different levels (3.3%, 15.3%, 27.6%, 39.5%, 51.6%, 63.8%, 75.8%, 88%, and 100%) of simulated visual field loss (i.e., 72 total trials). In this experiment, the carrier was presented at full contrast. Experiment 2 used just 2 levels of SVFL—either no loss (full field) or 50% loss—and modified the contrast of the carrier over seven log-spaced levels (1.6%, 3.1%, 6.3%, 12.5%, 25%, 50%, and 100%). Each stimulus was repeated 8 times (2 levels of SVFL × 7 contrasts × 8 trials = 112 trials). 
Procedure
We performed a 9-point eye-tracking calibration procedure for each participant before each experiment. During the test phase, participants were instructed to attempt to maintain fixation on the center of the screen, that is, “stare OKN.”45 For participants unfamiliar with the task, we ran a demo experiment before data collection that consisted of a sequence of 2-second trials that were run until the participant was comfortable with the task. 
The main test consisted of a sequence of 2-second trials. After the presentation of the test stimulus the screen returned to mid-gray and participants were required to indicate the perceived direction (left or right) of the carrier using the computer keyboard. Participants then received immediate feedback for their response (to promote vigilance) through the color (green or red) of a rectangular frame around the edge of the screen. We used a peripheral frame to avoid providing an explicit fixation marker for subsequent trials. Trials were automatically repeated when less than 80% of possible eye-tracking samples were recorded (e.g., because the participant looked away). 
Quantifying OKN
To measure the strength of OKN, we quantified the velocity of participants’ tracking eye-movements relative to the velocity of the stimulus, using an automated method. We refer to this measure as OKN gain
Eye movement data were analyzed offline after the experiment was complete. For ease of analysis, in trials with a leftward moving stimulus, eye position data were rotated 180°, so all eye movements were relative to a common stimulus direction. Eye-tracking data were first filtered to remove artefacts such as blinks. We did this by removing outlier where the estimated instantaneous pupillary area deviated by more than 2.75 × SD from the mean pupillary area for the trial. To be conservative and allow time for the eyetracker to accurately reacquire the eyes, we also removed 5 samples (83.3 ms = 5 × 16.7 ms of the 60 Hz sampled data) from either side of these samples. Such filtering necessarily breaks up the sequence of eye-tracking data, but we analyzed remaining data by simply joining any resulting subsequences within a trial's dataset. 
To reduce noise in our estimates of eye position, we applied a second order Savitzky-Golay filter46 with a frame length of five samples. The derivative of eye position was used to calculate the instantaneous velocity of eye movements. We then classified all points as either “tracking” or “saccade”: using a range of candidate “saccadic thresholds” (the minimum velocity required for a datum to be classed as a saccade), we calculated the total distance the eye moved during tracking, minus that traveled during a saccade, and selected the threshold that maximized the aggregate distance (Fig. 2).43 
Figure 2.
 
(a) Gaze position across a 2-second trial, classified as “tracking” (green) or “saccade” (red) using the procedure illustrated below. (b) Estimated eye velocity. Dashed lines indicate the velocity threshold. Velocities whose magnitude exceeded the magnitude of the threshold were classed as saccades (red symbols). (c) Adaptive selection of saccade threshold from a range of candidate thresholds. The plot shows the aggregate distance moved by the eye during tracking and saccade, for each candidate saccade-threshold. The threshold selected maximized this distance. Aggregate distance is the distance eye moved along with the stimulus minus the distance eye moved against the stimulus drifting direction.
Figure 2.
 
(a) Gaze position across a 2-second trial, classified as “tracking” (green) or “saccade” (red) using the procedure illustrated below. (b) Estimated eye velocity. Dashed lines indicate the velocity threshold. Velocities whose magnitude exceeded the magnitude of the threshold were classed as saccades (red symbols). (c) Adaptive selection of saccade threshold from a range of candidate thresholds. The plot shows the aggregate distance moved by the eye during tracking and saccade, for each candidate saccade-threshold. The threshold selected maximized this distance. Aggregate distance is the distance eye moved along with the stimulus minus the distance eye moved against the stimulus drifting direction.
To estimate OKN gain, we first accommodated the rise time for OKN by excluding data collected with the first 500 ms of each trial. We then fit estimated tracking velocities using a first-order generalized linear model with a slope of 0. The y-axis intercept of the fitted line is the estimated average tracking velocity of the eye during that trial. Finally, the gain of OKN is defined as the ratio between this estimated velocity and the velocity of the stimulus. 
Statistical Analysis
Unless otherwise stated, statistical analyses were conducted in Matlab. Our main outcome measure was OKN gain, versus the proportion of the stimulus that was visible (Experiment 1) or the contrast level (Experiment 2). We calculated the mean OKN gain for each participant over eight repetitions of each tested level. The mean OKN gain data across all participants for each condition (i.e., extent of SVFL in Experiment 1, contrast in Experiment 2) were fit with a Naka-Rushton function47 defined as:  
\begin{eqnarray*} R = {R_{max}} \; \frac{{{L^n}}} {L^n} + {L_{50}}^{n}. \end{eqnarray*}
 
Here R is the response (OKN gain), Rmax is the maximum response achieved, L is the stimulus level (SVFL or contrast), L50 is the stimulus level which elicits an OKN gain of 50% of Rmax and n is the exponent. We fit the mean OKN gain for each participant, and across all participants to estimate Rmax, L50 and n. To improve the fit of individual participants, we defined the upper boundary of Rmax as the observed maximum mean gain for each participant. Note that we settled on this as a good fit to our data after comparing other simpler fits, such as a log-linear fit to log (OKN-gain) versus SVFL. 
In both experiments, we analyzed the mean OKN gain across condition to compute a repeated measure analysis of variance (ANOVA) (with a significance threshold of P < 0.05), using Mauchly's sphericity test with Greenhouse-Geisser correction, as required. 
Monte-Carlo simulation (using data from Experiment 1) was used to determine the sensitivity and specificity of our method for detecting SVFL. We synthesized 800,000 samples (8 repetitions of 100,000 simulated participants repetitions) of predicted OKN gains across 64 synthesized VF extents (from 0.03 to full field). We then predicted OKN gains using the Naka-Rushton fit parameters from Experiment 1 and matched within-subject and between-subject variability (using standard deviations calculated from Experiment 1). Next, for determining the sensitivity and specificity of our method, we used a range of detection criteria and calculated the hit rate and false-positive rate for each level of visual field and criterion level. The range of cutoff criteria was from of mean OKN gainmin field – 3 × SD to mean OKN gainmax field + 3 × SD (mean OKN gainmin field and mean OKN gainmax field is the predicted OKN gain form Naka-Rushton fit for the 5-deg. central field and full field OKN respectively, and the SD is within-observer standard deviation). The false-positive and hit rates were used to plot ROC curves and corresponding d′ values. 
Results
Experiment 1: Effect of Simulated Visual Field Loss (SVFL) on OKN Gain
Figure 3 plots estimated OKN-gain against the percentage of visible-stimulus for our 16 participants. Visually, the data appear to be well captured by the Naka-Rushton fits, although we note considerable variation in the maximum OKN-response obtained and in the rate of increase of OKN with the percentage of visible stimulus. To determine whether the Naka-Rushton was a good fit to our data—compared to simpler fits with 2 free parameters—we compared adjusted R2 estimates of goodness-of-fit for the Naka-Rushton to a log-linear fit (of log-Gain, vs. linear-% visible stimulus) . The adjusted R2 for the Naka-Rushton fits were higher in 11 of 16 participants (with an average adjusted R2 across all participants of 0.722) compared to the log-linear fit (average adjusted R2 of 0.530). Naka-Rushton fits on individual data gave estimates of Rmax of 0.80 ± 0.16 (mean ± 1 SD) and L50 of 0.087 ± 0.086. 
Figure 3.
 
Estimated gain of the optokinetic response plot against the percentage of stimulus that was visible to our sixteen participants. Error bars indicate ±1 SD around the mean. Data have been fit with a Naka-Rushton function (described in “Statistical analysis”).
Figure 3.
 
Estimated gain of the optokinetic response plot against the percentage of stimulus that was visible to our sixteen participants. Error bars indicate ±1 SD around the mean. Data have been fit with a Naka-Rushton function (described in “Statistical analysis”).
Pooling data across participants (Fig. 4), OKN gain was positively correlated with the spatial extent of the peripheral visual field, and ranged from 0.38 ± 0.20 measured with a 5-deg. diameter foveal stimulus-patch to 0.92 ± 0.15 measured with a full-field stimulus (r = 0.88, P = 0.0017). A 1-way repeated-measure ANOVA was conducted to measure the effect of SVFL on OKN gain. There was a significant effect of SVFL F(3.85,57.76) = 56.82, P < 0.0001 (nb fractional degrees of freedom result from Greenhouse-Geisser correction). The change in the mean OKN gain values was captured with the Naka-Rushton fit parameters (Rmax = 1.16, L50 = 0.12, n = 0.49, goodness of Naka-Rushton fit: R2 = 0.94). Although the rightmost large-gray circle - corresponding to the full-field condition appears to be above the Naka-Rushton fit, it is not an outlier (z = 0.93, n = 16, P = 0.3524). The deviation of the mean gain from the Naka-Rushton expected value for the full field condition was 0.61 × SD (Fig. 4). 
Figure 4.
 
Results averaged across participants; OKN gain increases with the extent of the peripheral visual field that is stimulated. Small colored dots are the mean gain (across 8 trials) for each participant (Note that the color coding of each individual participant is consistent with Fig. 3). Large gray circles are the mean gain across participants. Mean values have been fit with a Naka-Ruston fit (solid black line). Error bars are ±1SD.
Figure 4.
 
Results averaged across participants; OKN gain increases with the extent of the peripheral visual field that is stimulated. Small colored dots are the mean gain (across 8 trials) for each participant (Note that the color coding of each individual participant is consistent with Fig. 3). Large gray circles are the mean gain across participants. Mean values have been fit with a Naka-Ruston fit (solid black line). Error bars are ±1SD.
Experiment 2: Effect of Simulated Contrast Loss on OKN Gain Across Two Visual Field Extents
Figure 5 plots the OKN gain against % of stimulus contrast for our 16 participants. We note considerable variation both in the maximum OKN-response obtained and in the rate of increase of OKN, as a function of stimulus contrast. The Naka-Rushton fits on individual data revealed the Rmax values of 0.75 ± 0.18 and 0.80 ± 0.19 (mean ± SD) and the C50 values of 4.4% ± 1.9% and 3.7% ± 1.3% for 50% and full field condition, respectively. 
Figure 5.
 
OKN gain plot against stimulus contrast for all sixteen participants. Red is full field and blue is 50% field. Data have been fitted with a Naka-Rushton function. Error bars span ±1 SD around the mean.
Figure 5.
 
OKN gain plot against stimulus contrast for all sixteen participants. Red is full field and blue is 50% field. Data have been fitted with a Naka-Rushton function. Error bars span ±1 SD around the mean.
The OKN gain was correlated with log contrast levels of the stimulus ranging from 0.06 ± 0.09 at 1.6% contrast to 0.73 ± 0.21 at full contrast in the 50% field condition (r = 0.88, P = 0.010); and ranging from 0.10 ± 0.13 to 0.83 ± 0.17 for the full-field condition (r = 0.90, P = 0.006). Two-way repeated measure ANOVA was conducted to measure the effect of contrast across two VF extents on OKN gain. Although there were significant effects of contrast F(2.14,32.07) = 120.50, P < 0.0001, and of visual field extent F(1.00,15.00) = 14.59, P = 0.002, there was no interaction between contrast and VF extent F(6,60) = 1.22, P = 0.30. 
The Rmax parameter of the Naka-Rushton fit increased from 0.74 to 0.81 between the 50% and full-field conditions. In contrast, the L50 parameter remained constant at 3.89% and 3.52% for the 50% and 100% field conditions, respectively. R-squared values for the Naka-Rushton fits were R2 = 0.997 and R2 = 0.995 for 50% field and full field conditions, respectively (Fig. 6). 
Figure 6.
 
Results averaged across participants; the effect of contrast on OKN-gain for 2 visual field extents. OKN gain increases with contrast across 50% (blue) and full field (red) conditions. Error bars show 1 SD, but for clarity only the positive and the negative error bars are printed for the full field and 50% field conditions, respectively.
Figure 6.
 
Results averaged across participants; the effect of contrast on OKN-gain for 2 visual field extents. OKN gain increases with contrast across 50% (blue) and full field (red) conditions. Error bars show 1 SD, but for clarity only the positive and the negative error bars are printed for the full field and 50% field conditions, respectively.
The sensitivity and specificity of our OKN-gain based algorithm for signaling visual field loss was estimated using a Monte-Carlo simulation (100,000 repeats) with data from Experiment 1 (Fig. 7a). When the extent of VF was 100%, a cutoff criterion of mean OKN gain − 2 × SD yielded sensitivity of 92%, and when the VF extent was 3%, the specificity was 94%. The value of d′ was greater for higher degrees of VF loss (Fig. 7b). 
Figure 7.
 
(a) Monte-Carlo simulation based on 100000 simulated subjects, showing percentage of successful detection of an abnormal visual field versus the extent of VF (on a log scale). (b) ROC curves and corresponding d’ values, here, calculated for 3, 28, 52 and 88% percent of stimulus that was visible.
Figure 7.
 
(a) Monte-Carlo simulation based on 100000 simulated subjects, showing percentage of successful detection of an abnormal visual field versus the extent of VF (on a log scale). (b) ROC curves and corresponding d’ values, here, calculated for 3, 28, 52 and 88% percent of stimulus that was visible.
Discussion
Our results show that the gain of involuntary optokinetic nystagmus correlates with both simulated peripheral visual field loss (in Experiment 1), and with the contrast of the stimulus (in Experiment 2). The results of Experiment 1 show that OKN gain reduces as the extent of simulated VF decreases. Our results reveal a similar trend to earlier studies, which show that reducing the area of retinal stimulation using both open28 and closed-loop masks36 decreases the gain of OKN. In the open loop design, the OKN gain dropped from 0.86 for the full field condition to 0.70 for the 5-deg. width central stimulus.28 With the closed-loop design the OKN gain dropped from 0.9 for the full field condition to 0.77 for a 20-deg. square central stimulus.36 Although in our study the OKN gain for the full field condition is comparable to previous studies (0.92), the gain elicited by a 5-deg. diameter central stimulus is about the half of what expected. This lower gain could be due to our use of a smaller, circular area of visible stimulus, compared to the vertical band used in other studies.28 The results of Experiment 2 showed a significant reduction of the OKN gain as a result of lowering the contrast. We also observed a significant effect of simulated VF on reducing OKN-gain across all contrast levels tested: simulated VF loss exacerbates the effect of contrast loss. 
The between-subject variation of OKN-gain that we observed is substantial but within the range reported by previous studies.28,40,48,49 The standard deviation (σ) of the mean for the full field condition was 0.15, which agrees with previous studies. For the 5-deg. diameter central stimulus in our study σ = 0.20, which is lower than σ = 0.33 reported by the only previous study using an open loop 5-deg. central stimulus (0.33 in Van Die and Collewijn's28 study). The lower σ in our study could be due to using masks with blurred edges, preventing them being used as a fixation target.50 
Factors that might contribute to variation in individual OKN gains include participants’ age, residual myopic refractive error and their level of attention. Age is unlikely to be a significant factor since it does not impact maximum eye velocity during the slow phase of OKN for stimulus velocities lower than ∼50 deg/s.51 We also think it is unlikely that residual myopic refractive error would significantly influence our results. Our stimuli were high contrast (100%) low spatial frequency (1.3 c/deg) patterns. Consequently the maximum residual myopic defocus present in our participants (0.50 and 0.75D) would have a negligible effect on their visibility. The anticipated sensitivity loss from myopic defocus would be only 30% (with −0.50D) or 50% (with −0.75D) so that our 100% contrast stimuli would remain 70 or 50 times their detection threshold, respectively.52 Finally, considering attention, Magnusson, Pyykko et al (1985),53 report that an auditory or vibration cue presented during presentation of OKN stimuli (drifting at 90 deg/s) could significantly increase mean OKN gain. While instructing patients to follow features of the stimulus (look OKN) creates more cortical activation compared to stare OKN,54 at the drifting velocity used in our study (10 deg/s), there was no difference in OKN-gain depending on whether participants were instructed to “look” or to “stare”.55 Conversely, participants selectively attending to superimposed masks could actually decrease OKN gain,50 which could explain the substantial drop in OKN-gain observed with a superimposed mask, even when it barely occludes the drifting carrier. Differences in selective attention to this feature likely contribute to the variation of OKN gain between participants. 
To minimize any impact on OKN gain from participants fixating areas of the mask, we used an open-loop masking paradigm and blurred the edges of the masks. Because high-contrast mask-edges can reduce OKN gain at velocities above 15 deg/s,56 we used a lower velocity of 10 deg/s. Even so, we observe a substantial difference in the OKN gain elicited by the 88%-field and full-field stimuli (Fig. 4). This discrepancy between the measured and expected OKN gain (based on our otherwise-excellent Naka-Rushton fits) in the full field condition is likely attributable to the presence of the mask, which is absent in the full field condition. The most likely explanation for this effect is the single video-frame delay (a maximum of 17 ms) that was present between the collection of the eye position sample, and the drawing of the next gaze contingent mask on the screen. In terms of our overall pattern of results then, we would expect this to have systematically reduced OKN gain in all SVFL conditions. We also note that any latency of the simulated scotoma would reduce OKN and therefore underestimate any predicted impact of glaucomatous field loss on OKN. In short, our findings likely represent a conservative estimate of the impact of real glaucomatous field loss on involuntary eye movements. 
In terms of the potential of this approach for quantifying visual field loss, there are potentially several advantages to an OKN technique over SAP. First, it provides the clinician with a single measure of the functional visual field, which makes monitoring of the progression simpler than SAP. Second, it is an objective measure, and as such it is likely to be less affected by patient capability and willingness to do the test. Third, the OKN paradigm requires no response or judgment on patients’ part, which should make it more acceptable by them. Finally, administration of the OKN test requires less expertise, compared with SAP and electrophysiological methods. All of these factors could potentially contribute to a reduction of the variability of VF assessment using this method. 
Given the limited impact of SVFL on OKN—recall a drop from 100% to 52% simulated visual field reduces gain by only 18%—is it likely that we will be able to base a test for quantifying glaucomatous vision loss on OKN? Loss of vision in the early stages of glaucoma does not only manifest as visual field loss. For example, patients that are pre-symptomatic (i.e. SAP indicates their visual fields are intact) may exhibit contrast sensitivity loss.5760 Such a loss of vision in early stage glaucoma may result from ganglion cell dysfunction rather than drop-out which could potentially contribute to OKN reduction. Indeed a preliminary report has been made that OKN gain is lower in pre-symptomatic patients (i.e. who have normal fields according to SAP) than controls under some conditions.61 We have also recently measured OKN gain in 41 patients with asymmetric visual field loss (a minimum 10% difference in visual field index across the eyes) from primary open angle glaucoma. We report that differences between the OKN gain of the better eye and the worst eye are correlated with differences in visual field index (for a 12.5% contrast pattern, r = 0.61, P < 0.0001).62 
However, a residual limitation on using the OKN technique is that it only gives an overall estimate of the functional visual field, with no topography. Spatial input to OKN is unlikely to be uniform across the peripheral field but localized binary m-sequence stimulation and spatial averaging (similar to multifocal electroretinography) could be used to obtain topographical maps. 
In conclusion, OKN shows promise for the objective measurement of peripheral visual function. Further testing in patients with organic field loss is required to determine whether it will have utility in the clinic as an objective measure of field loss arising from conditions such as glaucoma. 
Acknowledgments
Supported in part by HRC award 19/118 “Measuring visual field loss in glaucoma using involuntary eye movements.” 
Disclosure: S.M. Doustkouhi, None; P.R.K. Turn-bull, None; S.C. Dakin, None 
References
Weinreb RN, Aung T, Medeiros FA. The pathophysiology and treatment of glaucoma: a review. JAMA. 2014; 311: 1901–1911. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Resnikoff S, Pascolini D, Etya'Ale D, et al. Global data on visual impairment in the year 2002. Bull World Health Organ. 2004; 82: 844–851. [PubMed]
Nickells RW, Howell GR, Soto I, John SWM. Under pressure: cellular and molecular responses during glaucoma, a common neurodegeneration with axonopathy. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2012; 35: 153–179. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Turalba AV, Grosskreutz C. A review of current technology used in evaluating visual function in glaucoma. Semin Ophthalmol. 2010; 25: 309–316. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Shaarawy T, Sherwood MB, Hitchings RA, Crowston JG. Glaucoma. 2nd ed. London: Elsevier, Saunders; 2015.
Montolio FGJ, Wesselink C, Gordijn M, Jansonius NM. Factors that influence standard automated perimetry test results in glaucoma: test reliability, technician experience, time of day, and season. Invest Ophth Vis Sci. 2012; 53: 7010–7017. [CrossRef]
Mutlukan E. The effect of refractive blur on the detection sensitivity to light offsets in the central visual-field. Acta Ophthalmol (Copenh). 1994; 72: 189–194. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Anderson RS, McDowell DR, Ennis FA. Effect of localized defocus on detection thresholds for different sized targets in the fovea and periphery. Acta Ophthalmol Scan. 2001; 79: 60–63. [CrossRef]
Bengtsson B, Heijl A. False-negative responses in glaucoma perimetry: indicators of patient performance or test reliability? Invest Ophth Vis Sci. 2000; 41: 2201–2204.
Kutzko KE, Brito CF, Wall M. Effect of instructions on conventional automated perimetry. Invest Ophth Vis Sci. 2000; 41: 2006–2013.
Kelly DH. Frequency doubling in visual responses. J Opt Soc Am. 1966; 56: 1628–1633. [CrossRef]
Wall M, Ketoff KM. Random dot motion perimetry in patients with glaucoma and in normal subjects. Am J Ophthalmol. 1995; 120: 587–596. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ferreras A, Polo V, Larrosa JM, et al. Can frequency-doubling technology and short-wavelength automated perimetries detect visual field defects before standard automated perimetry in patients with preperimetric glaucoma? J Glaucoma. 2007; 16: 372–383. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Landers JA, Goldberg I, Graham SL. Detection of early visual field loss in glaucoma using frequency-doubling perimetry and short-wavelength automated perimetry. Arch Ophthalmol. 2003; 121: 1705–1710. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bosworth CF, Sample PA, Gupta N, Bathija R, Weinreb RN. Motion automated perimetry identifies early glaucomatous field defects. Arch Ophthalmol. 1998; 116: 1153–1158. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ventura LM, Porciatti V, Ishida K, Feuer WJ, Parrish RK, 2nd. Pattern electroretinogram abnormality and glaucoma. Ophthalmology. 2005; 112: 10–19. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Chan HL, Brown B. Multifocal ERG changes in glaucoma. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 1999; 19: 306–316. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Graham SL, Fortune B. . Electrophysiology in Glaucoma Assessment. St. Louis: Elsevier Ltd; 2015.
Park S, Park SH, Chang JH, Ohn Y-H. Study for analysis of the multifocal visual evoked potential. Korean J Ophthalmol. 2011; 25: 334–340. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Young B, Eggenberger E, Kaufman D. Current electrophysiology in ophthalmology: a review. Curr Opin Ophthalmol. 2012; 23: 497–505. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sethi S, Fay C, Bollinger K. Functional testing in glaucoma diagnosis. Curr Ophthalmol Rep. 2013; 1: 89–97. [CrossRef]
Pel JJM, van Beijsterveld MCM, Thepass G, van der Steen J. Validity and repeatability of saccadic response times across the visual field in eye movement perimetry. Transl Vis Sci Technol. 2013; 2: 3. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Jernigan ME. Structural analysis of eye movement response to visual field stimuli. Comput Biol Med. 1980; 10: 11–22. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Allen LE, Slater ME, Proffitt RV, Quarton E, Pelah A. A new perimeter using the preferential looking response to assess peripheral visual fields in young and developmentally delayed children. J AAPOS. 2012; 16: 261–265. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Trope GE, Eizenman M, Coyle E. Eye movement perimetry in glaucoma. Can J Ophthalmol. 1989; 24: 197–199. [PubMed]
Meethal NSK, Mazumdar D, Asokan R, et al. Development of a test grid using eye movement perimetry for screening glaucomatous visual field defects. Graef Arch Clin Exp. 2018; 256: 371–379. [CrossRef]
Xu A, Warren D, Doyle C, Papendieck A, Thurtell M, Wall M. Eye movement perimetry in glaucoma patients. Invest Ophth Vis Sci. 2013; 54: 3953.
Van Die GC, Collewijn H. Control of human optokinetic nystagmus by the central and peripheral retina: effects of partial visual field masking, scotopic vision and central retinal scotomata. Brain Res. 1986; 383: 185–194. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Silverman SE, Trick GL, Hart WM, Jr. Motion perception is abnormal in primary open-angle glaucoma and ocular hypertension. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1990; 31: 722–729. [PubMed]
Tran DB, Silverman SE, Zimmerman K, Feldon SE. Age-related deterioration of motion perception and detection. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 1998; 236: 269–273. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Dubois MFW, Collewijn H. Optokinetic reactions in man elicited by localized retinal motion stimuli. Vision Res. 1979; 19: 1105–1115. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Gresty M, Halmagyi M. Following eye movements in the absence of central vision. Acta Otolaryngol. 1979; 87 (5–6): 477–483. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Cheng M, Outerbridge J. Optokinetic nystagmus during selective retinal stimulation. Exp Brain Res. 1975; 23: 129–139. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Howard IP, Ohmi M. The efficiency of the central and peripheral retina in driving human optokinetic nystagmus. Vision Res. 1984; 24: 969–976. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Abadi RV, Pascal E. The effects of simultaneous central and peripheral field motion on the optokinetic response. Vision Res. 1991; 31: 2219–2225. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Abadi RV, Howard IP, Ohmi M, Lee EE. The effect of central and peripheral field stimulation on the rise time and gain of human optokinetic nystagmus. Perception. 2005; 34: 1015–1024. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Murasugi CM, Howard IP. Up-down asymmetry in human vertical optokinetic nystagmus and afternystagmus: contributions of the central and peripheral retinae. Exp Brain Res. 1989; 77: 183–192. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hood JD. Observations upon the neurological mechanism of optokinetic nystagmus with especial reference to the contribution of peripheral vision. Acta Otolaryngol. 1967; 63 (2–3): 208–215. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Valmaggia C, Charlier J, Gottlob I. Optokinetic nystagmus in patients with central scotomas in age related macular degeneration. Br J Ophthalmol. 2001; 85: 169–172. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Abadi RV, Pantazidou M. Monocular optokinetic nystagmus in humans with age-related maculopathy. Br J Ophthalmol. 1997; 81: 123–129. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Brainard DH. The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis. 1997; 10: 433–436. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Cornelissen FW, Peters EM, Palmer J. The eyelink toolbox: eye tracking with MATLAB and the psychophysics toolbox. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 2002; 34: 613–617. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Dakin SC, Turnbull PR. Similar contrast sensitivity functions measured using psychophysics and optokinetic nystagmus. Sci Rep. 2016; 6: 34514. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bizrah M, Dakin SC, Guo L, et al. A semi-automated technique for labeling and counting of apoptosing retinal cells. BMC Bioinformatics. 2014; 15: 169. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Knapp CM, Proudlock FA, Gottlob I. OKN asymmetry in human subjects: a literature review. Strabismus. 2013; 21: 37–49. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ghasia FF, Shaikh AG. Uncorrected myopic refractive error increases microsaccade amplitude. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2015; 56: 2531–2535. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Naka KI, Rushton WAH. S-potentials from colour units in the retina of fish (Cyprinidae). J Physiol. 1966; 185: 536–555.
Knapp CM, Gottlob I, McLean RJ, Proudlock FA. Horizontal and vertical look and stare optokinetic nystagmus symmetry in healthy adult volunteers. Invest Ophth Vis Sci. 2008; 49: 581–510. [CrossRef]
Wester ST, Rizzo JF, III, Balkwill MD, Wall C, III. Optokinetic nystagmus as a measure of visual function in severely visually impaired patients. Invest Ophth Vis Sci. 2007; 48: 4542–4548. [CrossRef]
Dyer RK, Abel LA. Effects of age and visual attention demands on optokinetic nystagmus suppression. Exp Eye Res. 2018; 183: 46–51. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Simons B, Büttner U. The influence of age on optokinetic nystagmus. Eur Arch Psychiatry Neurol Sci. 1985; 234: 369–73. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bradley A, Hook J, Haeseker J. A comparison of clinical acuity and contrast sensitivity charts: effect of uncorrected myopia. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 1991; 11: 218–226. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Magnusson M, Pyykko I, Jantti V. Effect of alertness and visual attention on optokinetic nystagmus in humans. Am J Otolaryngol. 1985; 6: 419–25. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kashou NH, Leguire LE, Roberts CJ, Fogt N, Smith MA, Rogers GL. Instruction dependent activation during optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) stimulation: an FMRI study at 3 T. Brain Res. 2010; 1336: 10–21. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Honrubia V, Downey WL, Mitchell DP, Ward PH. Experimental studies on optokinetic nystagmus II. Normal humans. Acta Otolaryngol. 1968; 65: 441–448. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Schor C, Narayan V. The influence of field size upon the spatial frequency response of optokinetic nystagmus. Vision Res. 1981; 21: 985–994. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Falcao-Reis F, O'Donoghue E, Buceti R, Hitchings RA, Arden GB. Peripheral contrast sensitivity in glaucoma and ocular hypertension. Br J Ophthalmol. 1990; 74: 712–716. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bron AJ. Contrast sensitivity changes in ocular hypertension and early glaucoma. Surv Ophthalmol. 1989; 33 (Suppl): 405–406; discussion 409–411. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Arden GB, Jacobson JJ. A simple grating test for contrast sensitivity: preliminary results indicate value in screening for glaucoma. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1978; 17: 23–32. [PubMed]
Adams AJ, Heron G, Husted R. Clinical measures of central vision function in glaucoma and ocular hypertension. Arch Ophthalmol. 1987; 105: 782–787. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Gonzalez EG, Trope GE, Samet S, Tarita-Nistor L. Asymmetry of optokinetic nystagmus in early glaucoma. Invest Ophth Vis Sci. 2019; 60: 1826.
Dakin SC, Mohammadpour Doustkouhi S, Kersten H, Turnbull PR, Yoon J, Danesh-Meyer H. Measuring visual field loss in glaucoma using involuntary eye movements. Invest Ophth Vis Sci. 2019; 60: 2468. [CrossRef]
Klistorner AI, Graham SL, Grigg JR, Billson FA. Multifocal topographic visual evoked potential: improving objective detection of local visual field defects. Invest Ophth Vis Sci. 1998; 39: 937–50.
Kim DE, Eizenman M, Trope GE, Kranemann C, editors. Eye movement perimetry. Proceedings of 17th International Conference of the Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society; 1995 20–23 Sept. 1995.
Dursteler MR, Wurtz RH. Pursuit and optokinetic deficits following chemical lesions of cortical areas MT and MST. J Neurophysiol. 1988; 60: 940–965. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Schiff D, Cohen B, Buttner-Ennever J, Matsuo V. Effects of lesions of the nucleus of the optic tract on optokinetic nystagmus and after-nystagmus in the monkey. Exp Brain Res. 1990; 79: 225–239. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hoffmann KP. Visual inputs relevant for the optokinetic nystagmus in. In: Freund HJ, Büttner U, Cohen B, Noth J, editors. Progress in brain research. Vol. 64: St. Louis: Elsevier; 1986. p. 75–84.
Figure 1.
 
Stimulus generation. (a) The stimulus was constructed using two layers. The layer at the back is the isotropic noise carrier which drifted to the left or the right. The layer at the front is a masking image—note the carrier is visible through the transparent part—which moved with the patient's gaze. (b) The resulting stimulus after occluding the carrier with the mask. From left to right, either 3%, 39%, or 88% of the carrier is visible through the mask.
Figure 1.
 
Stimulus generation. (a) The stimulus was constructed using two layers. The layer at the back is the isotropic noise carrier which drifted to the left or the right. The layer at the front is a masking image—note the carrier is visible through the transparent part—which moved with the patient's gaze. (b) The resulting stimulus after occluding the carrier with the mask. From left to right, either 3%, 39%, or 88% of the carrier is visible through the mask.
Figure 2.
 
(a) Gaze position across a 2-second trial, classified as “tracking” (green) or “saccade” (red) using the procedure illustrated below. (b) Estimated eye velocity. Dashed lines indicate the velocity threshold. Velocities whose magnitude exceeded the magnitude of the threshold were classed as saccades (red symbols). (c) Adaptive selection of saccade threshold from a range of candidate thresholds. The plot shows the aggregate distance moved by the eye during tracking and saccade, for each candidate saccade-threshold. The threshold selected maximized this distance. Aggregate distance is the distance eye moved along with the stimulus minus the distance eye moved against the stimulus drifting direction.
Figure 2.
 
(a) Gaze position across a 2-second trial, classified as “tracking” (green) or “saccade” (red) using the procedure illustrated below. (b) Estimated eye velocity. Dashed lines indicate the velocity threshold. Velocities whose magnitude exceeded the magnitude of the threshold were classed as saccades (red symbols). (c) Adaptive selection of saccade threshold from a range of candidate thresholds. The plot shows the aggregate distance moved by the eye during tracking and saccade, for each candidate saccade-threshold. The threshold selected maximized this distance. Aggregate distance is the distance eye moved along with the stimulus minus the distance eye moved against the stimulus drifting direction.
Figure 3.
 
Estimated gain of the optokinetic response plot against the percentage of stimulus that was visible to our sixteen participants. Error bars indicate ±1 SD around the mean. Data have been fit with a Naka-Rushton function (described in “Statistical analysis”).
Figure 3.
 
Estimated gain of the optokinetic response plot against the percentage of stimulus that was visible to our sixteen participants. Error bars indicate ±1 SD around the mean. Data have been fit with a Naka-Rushton function (described in “Statistical analysis”).
Figure 4.
 
Results averaged across participants; OKN gain increases with the extent of the peripheral visual field that is stimulated. Small colored dots are the mean gain (across 8 trials) for each participant (Note that the color coding of each individual participant is consistent with Fig. 3). Large gray circles are the mean gain across participants. Mean values have been fit with a Naka-Ruston fit (solid black line). Error bars are ±1SD.
Figure 4.
 
Results averaged across participants; OKN gain increases with the extent of the peripheral visual field that is stimulated. Small colored dots are the mean gain (across 8 trials) for each participant (Note that the color coding of each individual participant is consistent with Fig. 3). Large gray circles are the mean gain across participants. Mean values have been fit with a Naka-Ruston fit (solid black line). Error bars are ±1SD.
Figure 5.
 
OKN gain plot against stimulus contrast for all sixteen participants. Red is full field and blue is 50% field. Data have been fitted with a Naka-Rushton function. Error bars span ±1 SD around the mean.
Figure 5.
 
OKN gain plot against stimulus contrast for all sixteen participants. Red is full field and blue is 50% field. Data have been fitted with a Naka-Rushton function. Error bars span ±1 SD around the mean.
Figure 6.
 
Results averaged across participants; the effect of contrast on OKN-gain for 2 visual field extents. OKN gain increases with contrast across 50% (blue) and full field (red) conditions. Error bars show 1 SD, but for clarity only the positive and the negative error bars are printed for the full field and 50% field conditions, respectively.
Figure 6.
 
Results averaged across participants; the effect of contrast on OKN-gain for 2 visual field extents. OKN gain increases with contrast across 50% (blue) and full field (red) conditions. Error bars show 1 SD, but for clarity only the positive and the negative error bars are printed for the full field and 50% field conditions, respectively.
Figure 7.
 
(a) Monte-Carlo simulation based on 100000 simulated subjects, showing percentage of successful detection of an abnormal visual field versus the extent of VF (on a log scale). (b) ROC curves and corresponding d’ values, here, calculated for 3, 28, 52 and 88% percent of stimulus that was visible.
Figure 7.
 
(a) Monte-Carlo simulation based on 100000 simulated subjects, showing percentage of successful detection of an abnormal visual field versus the extent of VF (on a log scale). (b) ROC curves and corresponding d’ values, here, calculated for 3, 28, 52 and 88% percent of stimulus that was visible.
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×