Introduction

Cardiovascular diseases including subarachnoid haemorrhage (SAH) are a leading cause of death in the industrialised countries. An SAH is fatal in up to 50% of the cases, although a decline in case fatality rate has been observed over the past decade. Many of the surviving patients have moderate to severe neurological deficits.1 The incidence of SAH varies between seven and 20 per 100 000 persons per year and approximately 10% of the patients report a positive family history.2,3 Bromberg et al3 suggested that SAH in the consecutive generations occurred at an earlier age as compared to previous generations. In both sporadic and in familial cases SAH occurs more frequently in women than in men.4 This difference is attributed to the higher occurrence of an SAH in women over 50 years of age in the sporadic cases.5 Lozano found a sex ratio difference (M:F=75:100) for all ages in pedigrees irrespective of the mode of inheritance.4

Intracranial saccular aneurysms (IAs) are responsible for 85% of SAH.6 As based on autopsy studies about 2–3% of the population has an asymptomatic IA, in familial cases the prevalence is estimated at 9.5–10.5%.3 Over 200 families have been described since the first report on the familial occurrence of IA was presented by Chambers in 1954.7,8 Segregation analysis did not reveal one mode of inheritance, both autosomal recessive (AR) and autosomal dominant (AD) patterns of inheritance have been described.7,9

In our study, we included only families with at least two relatives in two consecutive generations affected by IA. Anticipation was studied only in families with SIA in consecutive generations. All proven IAs irrespective of symptoms were included in the study of the sex ratios.

Materials and methods

Families were included in this study only after the diagnosis of a symptomatic IA (SIA), a presymptomatic IA (PIA) or an intracranial aneurysm without information on the symptoms (IA) in at least two relatives in consecutive generations had been proven by radiological, preoperative or post-mortem investigations. Aneurysms were designated symptomatic when either an SAH had occurred or neurological impairment was caused by the aneurysm. Families were excluded when aneurysms were associated with other intracranial vascular or known heritable diseases (eg arteriovenous malformation, AD polycystic kidney disease, Marfan syndrome). Among 50 families ascertained for a genetic analysis 10 showed the disease in two consecutive generations. In addition, we searched the literature for families in which SIA, PIA or IA were shown in two consecutive generations. PMS and ML reviewed the data on the families presented as well as those from the literature.

We performed an extensive Medline search from 1966 onward with keywords aneurysm, cerebral, intracranial, familial, genetic(s) and subarachnoid haemorrhage to identify studies published between 1966 and 1999. The reference lists of all retrieved publications were scrutinised for additional family studies. This method of crosschecking was continued until no further publications were found. Either the most recent or the most extensive publication was used or data from several publications were combined avoiding counting patients twice when more than one report on the same family was identified.

The age of onset was determined only in SIA (thus excluding PIA and IA). We performed a first analysis (to avoid ascertainment errors) in those families in which several children were affected. Index patients were excluded and only the eldest affected child was included for the analysis.

On the basis of the results of this first analysis, anticipation was studied in all families known to date showing SIA in consecutive generations.

The age of onset distribution in the consecutive generations was evaluated with the Kolmogorov–Smirnoff test. The mean difference was calculated for the age of onset analysis comparing the generations. The mean age of onset was used in the analysis when multiple children were affected within a sibship (except for the first analysis in which only the eldest affected child was used). Differences in age of onset between generations and between genders for both generations were tested with a paired t-test. All SIA, PIA and IA patients were included in the sex ratio analysis. The sex ratio variation was analysed by a χ2-test. The statistical analyses were performed using SPSS for Windows, release 7.5.2.

Results

The pedigrees of the 10 families with SAH, SIA and/or PIA in two consecutive generations are depicted in Figure 1. One family (Figure 1j) has been published before, but additional data are presented in this paper.3,10,11,12 In all, two fathers, eight mothers, eight sons and 10 daughters were affected in those families. No family was found with affected relatives in three consecutive generations, as is exemplary shown in family J.

Figure 1
figure 1

(a–i) Newly presented families with SAH and/or IA in consecutive generations. (1j) Family previously reported with additional data. Data shown on age of onset (SIA), age at detection (PIA, IA), age at screening (when negative). Sequences of individuals in families altered and individuals not taking part in the GAIA study are omitted for privacy reasons.

Anticipation, the phenomenon that the disease becomes manifest at an earlier age in the next generations, is apparent for SAH in the presented families (Figure 1a, b, c, e, h and j). In six families, IA (SIA and PIA) were confirmed in both generations (Figure 1a, b, c, d, i and j). Two fathers with two sons and four mothers with five sons and four daughters were affected in these families.

SIA occurred in both generations in four of our families (Figure 1a, b, c and j), and in these families anticipation was established: The mean difference in age of onset between the generations was 21.2 years (P<0.003; paired t-test; t=9.180).

We reviewed the literature for families in which the diagnosis is SIA and/or PIA was evident in two consecutive generations to underline our observation on anticipation. We analysed all these families known to date with affected members in consecutive generations (Table 1a SIA families). When more than one child in one family was affected their mean age was used (the test was performed only on families from the literature: Table 1A minus Struycken's cases). The mean difference in age of onset between the generations was 23.0 years (P<0.0001; paired t-test; t=11.137).

Table 1 Families described in this paper and families described previously. (a) SIA and (b) SIA/PIA/IA in consecutive generations

For all families, the mean age of onset in the parents was 55.5 years (SD 12.5). There is no significant difference in the age of onset between fathers and mothers (two-tailed t-test; P=0.25). The mean age of onset in the children was 34 years (SD 11.2). No significant difference in the age of onset between daughters and sons was observed (two-tailed t-test, P=0.54). The mean difference between the generations was 22.4 years (P<0.0001; paired t-test; t=12.913). To avoid ascertainment bias and to underline the anticipation found in our own families anticipation was tested on a subset of families in which the index patient was excluded in the test. Only the age of onset in oldest affected child (other than the index patient) and in the parents was analysed (families and persons used in this analysis are depicted with an asterix in Table 1a). Even under these restrictions, a significant age of onset difference (mean difference=15.4 years; P=0.01; paired t-test: t=0.549) was noted. Finally, the data were combined (data shown in Table 1a). The age distribution was shown to be normal in both generations (Kolmogorov–Smirnov test: 0.495<Z<0.796). A difference in the age of onset between generations is shown. There is no evidence of proven IA in three consecutive generations in any family. Even in the large family (Figure 1j) there are no indications for IA in the third generation.

The sex ratio of affected parents (M:F=11:37) is different (χ2=6.52, P<0.02) from the sex ratio of their affected children (M:F=28:32). When sex ratios in both generations were compared with the general population (1:1.02 for the Netherlands), only the ratio in parents did differ (children: χ2=0.092, P=0.76; parents: χ2=12.49, P<0.001).

Discussion

A genetic predisposition for IA has been established. 3,7,13,14,15 Also for IA evidence for a familial component has been suggested.11,13 The prevalence for IA is estimated between 2 and 3% in the general population. In familial cases, the prevalence is estimated at 9.5–10.5%, with a relative risk of at least four for first-degree relatives.16,17,18 The high (87.5%) concordance of IAs in monozygotic twins underlines the genetic basis (although data figures on dizygotic twins are not available).15

We have collected families with affected persons in two consecutive generations. This was necessary for the analysis of age of onset and sex ratio analyses between generations.

Anticipation is established for SIA even when only the age of onset of the oldest affected child (which was not the index patient) is compared with the age of onset in the parent. The anticipation is not related to the sex of the transmitting parent. SIA has never been found in three consecutive generations. This disappearance of the phenotype is clearly illustrated in a large Dutch family (Figure 1j). In the third generation no relative harboured an aneurysm over a 15-year follow-up period. Ages at screening varied from 25–39 years, being an age at which, in the light of the aforementioned anticipation, IAs should be present.

The occurrence of the disease in two consecutive generations in itself points towards an AD inheritance pattern. This is strengthened by other IA families in which at least half of the children were affected.10,19,20,21 But if AD inheritance holds in these selected families, how do we explain that no families are seen in which the disease is manifest in three consecutive generations? Is it due to anticipation and subsequent extinction? Although the data discussed so far clearly indicate that in these selected families the inheritance pattern is consistent with AD inheritance, be it with anticipation and extinction, strong arguments for AR inheritance are present as well. More than 75% of all IA families do not show affected individuals in consecutive generations.7 The number of affected children keeps far from the 50% affected offspring as is observed in AD diseases. In our study, 40 of the 50 families did not show affected individuals in consecutive generations (data not shown). An argument for the contribution of recessive genes to the disease is the higher risk for siblings compared to children of a patient to harbour an aneurysm.22 Additional support comes from a family presented by Bromberg et al3 in which in a consanguineous marriage five out of 20 children are affected. In this particular case, a single defective disease gene may be present in homozygous state in all patients due to homozygosity by descent.

Pseudodominancy might explain observed anticipation and the absence of third generation affected family members in the families included in this study. The obvious AD inheritance is mimicked by homozygosity for a recessive IA gene in the parental generation. The presence of one heterozygous parent and one homozygous parent for the IA gene would then explain the occurrence of the disease in 50% of the offspring in some families.10,19,20,21

The development and the maintenance of the intracranial arteries as one entity suggest that one or more pathways, each containing several genes, showing either an AD or an AR inheritance, are involved. Especially, the development of cerebral artery bifurcations seems important since the apexes of these bifurcations are almost always involved in IAs. The IA phenotype is established depending on the steps in the pathway that are dysfunctioning. A polygenic model may explain all the observed phenomena. The sex ratio differences observed between generations are difficult to explain, but might be due to an over-representation of elderly affected women (as is seen in the sporadic cases) in the parental generation.23 However, no significant difference in the age of onset was found comparing both sexes in the parental generations.

It seems very unlikely that genomic imprinting, the epigenetic phenomenon in which the expression of genes is reversibly modified depending on the sex of the transmitting parents, has attributed to the observed phenomena of anticipation and sex ratios. No differences in anticipation were observed in the offspring of transmitting mothers and fathers. Besides, both females and males were found to be present in the paternal offspring and this also holds for the maternal offspring.

Our present effort is directed to the isolation and characterisation of the major genes contributing to IA. This will provide an insight into the mode of inheritance and will finally enable us to identify presymptomatically people at high risk for developing IA and subsequent SAH.