Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Training spatial cognition enhances mathematical learning in a randomized study of 17,000 children

Abstract

Spatial and mathematical abilities are strongly associated. Here, we analysed data from 17,648 children, aged 6–8 years, who performed 7 weeks of mathematical training together with randomly assigned spatial cognitive training with tasks demanding more spatial manipulation (mental rotation or tangram), maintenance of spatial information (a visuospatial working memory task) or spatial, non-verbal reasoning. We found that the type of cognitive training children performed had a significant impact on mathematical learning, with training of visuospatial working memory and reasoning being the most effective. This large, community-based study shows that spatial cognitive training can result in transfer to academic abilities, and that reasoning ability and maintenance of spatial information is relevant for mathematics learning in young children.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Overview of the four training tasks and the percentages of time allocated to each in the five training plans.
Fig. 2: Predicted factor scores by test week, correlations with baseline mathematics and training curves of the density of children for each difficulty level and day of training.
Fig. 3: Effects of the training tasks on mathematical improvement.
Fig. 4: Predicted performance following differing amounts of VSWM and NVR training for different levels of baseline cognitive performance.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The data to replicate the main analysis (that is, mixed-effects model) are available at https://github.com/njudd/spatialcognition. Data for the baseline characteristics and graphs in this study are available upon request from the corresponding author.

Code availability

The code to replicate the main analysis (that is, mixed-effects model) is available at https://github.com/njudd/spatialcognition. Code for the baseline characteristics and graphs in this study is available upon request from the corresponding author.

References

  1. Wai, J., Lubinski, D. & Benbow, C. P. Spatial ability for STEM domains: aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its importance. J. Educ. Psychol. 101, 817–835 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Hawes, Z. & Ansari, D. What explains the relationship between spatial and mathematical skills? A review of evidence from brain and behavior. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 27, 465–482 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Mix, K. S. et al. Separate but correlated: the latent structure of space and mathematics across development. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 145, 1206–1227 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Peng, P., Namkung, J., Barnes, M. & Sun, C. A meta-analysis of mathematics and working memory: moderating effects of working memory domain, type of mathematics skill, and sample characteristics. J. Educ. Psychol. 108, 455–473 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Gathercole, S. E. & Brown, L. Working memory assessments at school entry as longitudinal predictors of National Curriculum attainment levels. Educ. Child Psychol. 20, 109–122 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Geary, D. C. Cognitive predictors of achievement growth in mathematics: a 5-year longitudinal study. Dev. Psychol. 47, 1539–1552 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Dillon, M. R., Kannan, H., Dean, J. T., Spelke, E. S. & Duflo, E. Cognitive science in the field: a preschool intervention durably enhances intuitive but not formal mathematics. Science 357, 47–55 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Newcombe, N. Harnessing Spatial Thinking to Support STEM Learning Working paper 161 (OECD iLibrary, 2017); https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/education/harnessing-spatial-thinking-to-support-stem-learning_7d5dcae6-en

  9. Stieff, M. & Uttal, D. How much can spatial training improve STEM achievement? Educ. Psychol. Rev. 27, 607–615 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Paying Attention to Spatial Reasoning, K-12: Support Document for Paying Attention to Mathematics Education (Ontario Ministry of Education, 2014); http://www.edu.gov.on.ca/eng/literacynumeracy/lnspayingattention.pdf

  11. Lohman, D. F. in Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence Vol. 4 (ed. Sternberg, R. J.) 181–248 (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1988).

  12. Carpenter, P. A. & Just, M. A. in Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence Vol. 3 (ed. Stenberg, R. J.) 221–252 (Erlbaum, 1986).

  13. Cheng, Y.-L. & Mix, K. S. Spatial training improves children’s mathematics ability. J. Cogn. Dev. 15, 2–11 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Hawes, Z., Moss, J., Caswell, B., Naqvi, S. & MacKinnon, S. Enhancing children’s spatial and numerical skills through a dynamic spatial approach to early geometry instruction: effects of a 32-week intervention. Cogn. Instr. 35, 236–264 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Lowrie, T., Logan, T. & Hegarty, M. The influence of spatial visualization training on students’ spatial reasoning and mathematics performance. J. Cogn. Dev. 20, 729–751 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Hawes, Z., Moss, J., Caswell, B. & Poliszczuk, D. Effects of mental rotation training on children’s spatial and mathematics performance: a randomized controlled study. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 4, 60–68 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Cornu, V., Schiltz, C., Pazouki, T. & Martin, R. Training early visuo-spatial abilities: a controlled classroom-based intervention study. Appl. Dev. Sci. 23, 1–21 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Rodán, A., Gimeno, P., Elosúa, M. R., Montoro, P. R. & Contreras, M. J. Boys and girls gain in spatial, but not in mathematical ability after mental rotation training in primary education. Learn. Individ. Differ. 70, 1–11 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Wright, H. et al. Improving Working Memory (Education Endowment Foundation, 2019); https://westminsterresearch.westminster.ac.uk/download/1d07359f1fda308387fc3679b914dac0eb89947b618f7f68f999a6f87793bfba/1174522/Working%20Memory.pdf

  20. Berger, E. M., Fehr, E., Hermes, H., Schunk, D. & Winkel, K. The Impact of Working Memory Training on Children’s Cognitive and Noncognitive Skills Discussion Paper No. 09/2020 (NHH Department of Economics, 2020); https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3622985

  21. Bergman-Nutley, S. & Klingberg, T. Effect of working memory training on working memory, arithmetic and following instructions. Psychol. Res. 78, 869–877 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Roberts, G. et al. Academic outcomes 2 years after working memory training for children with low working memory: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatr. 170, e154568 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Schwaighofer, M., Fischer, F. & Bühner, M. Does working memory training transfer? A meta-analysis including training conditions as moderators. Educ. Psychol. 50, 138–166 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Simons, D. J. et al. Do “brain-training” programs work? Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 17, 103–186 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Francis, G. Too good to be true: publication bias in two prominent studies from experimental psychology. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 19, 151–156 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Green, C. S. et al. Improving methodological standards in behavioral interventions for cognitive enhancement. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 3, 2–29 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Mackintosh, N. & Mackintosh, N. J. IQ and Human Intelligence (Oxford Univ. Press, 2011).

  28. Bergman Nutley, S. et al. Gains in fluid intelligence after training non-verbal reasoning in 4-year-old children: a controlled, randomized study. Dev. Sci. 14, 591–601 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Klauer, K. J. & Phye, G. D. Inductive reasoning: a training approach. Rev. Educ. Res. 78, 85–123 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Mackey, A. P., Hill, S. S., Stone, S. I. & Bunge, S. A. Differential effects of reasoning and speed training in children. Dev. Sci. 14, 582–590 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Nemmi, F. et al. Behavior and neuroimaging at baseline predict individual response to combined mathematical and working memory training in children. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 43–51 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Fischer, U., Moeller, K., Bientzle, M., Cress, U. & Nuerk, H.-C. Sensori-motor spatial training of number magnitude representation. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 177–183 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Outhwaite, L. A., Faulder, M., Gulliford, A. & Pitchford, N. J. Raising early achievement in math with interactive apps: a randomized control trial. J. Educ. Psychol. 111, 284–298 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Klingberg, T. et al. Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD—a randomized, controlled trial. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 44, 177–186 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J. & Perrig, W. J. Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 6829–6833 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Schmiedek, F., Lövdén, M. & Lindenberger, U. Hundred days of cognitive training enhance broad cognitive abilities in adulthood: findings from the COGITO study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2, 27 (2010).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Roid, G. H. & Miller, L. J. Leiter International Performance Scale—Revised: Examiner’s Manual (Stoelting, 1997).

  38. Mix, K. S. Why are spatial skill and mathematics related? Child Dev. Perspect. 13, 121–126 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Lortie-Forgues, H. & Inglis, M. Rigorous large-scale educational RCTs are often uninformative: should we be concerned? Educ. Res. 48, 158–166 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Bloom, H. S., Hill, C. J., Black, A. R. & Lipsey, M. W. Performance trajectories and performance gaps as achievement effect-size benchmarks for educational interventions. J. Res. Educ. Eff. 1, 289–328 (2008).

    Google Scholar 

  41. Abelson, R. P. A variance explanation paradox: when a little is a lot. Psychol. Bull. 97, 129–133 (1985).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Funder, D. C. & Ozer, D. J. Evaluating effect size in psychological research: sense and nonsense. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 2, 156–168 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Butterworth, B. & Kovas, Y. Understanding neurocognitive developmental disorders can improve education for all. Science 340, 300–305 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Uttal, D. H. et al. The malleability of spatial skills: a meta-analysis of training studies. Psychol. Bull. 139, 352–402 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Neuburger, S., Jansen, P., Heil, M. & Quaiser-Pohl, C. Gender differences in pre-adolescents’ mental-rotation performance: do they depend on grade and stimulus type? Pers. Individ. Differ. 50, 1238–1242 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Revelle, W. psych: procedures for psychological, psychometric, and personality research. https://www.scholars.northwestern.edu/en/publications/psych-procedures-for-personality-and-psychological-research (Northwestern University, 2019).

  47. R Core Development Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2014).

  48. Rosseel, Y. Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling and more. Version 0.5-12 (BETA). J. Stat. Softw. 48, 1–36 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Enders, C. K. & Bandalos, D. L. The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Struct. Equ. Model. 8, 430–457 (2001).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Hu, L. & Bentler, P. M. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 6, 1–55 (1999).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Putnick, D. L. & Bornstein, M. H. Measurement invariance conventions and reporting: the state of the art and future directions for psychological research. Dev. Rev. 41, 71–90 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Chen, F. F. Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct. Equ. Model. 14, 464–504 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Lenth, R. emmeans: estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/emmeans/index.html (Univ. Iowa, 2019).

Download references

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge R. Almeida, D. Sjölander, J. Beckeman, B. Sauce and D. Zhang for extensive help with various aspects of the study. This work was supported by contributions from M. Westman and S. Westman, along with funding from The Swedish Medical Research Foundation. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

N.J. and T.K. contributed equally in all aspects of the study.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Torkel Klingberg.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

T.K. holds an unpaid position as Chief Scientific Officer for the non-profit organization Cognition Matters. N.J. declares no competing interests.

Additional information

Peer review information Nature Human Behaviour thanks Kelly S. Mix and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Figs. 1–7 and Supplementary Tables 1–5.

Reporting Summary

Peer Review File

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Judd, N., Klingberg, T. Training spatial cognition enhances mathematical learning in a randomized study of 17,000 children. Nat Hum Behav 5, 1548–1554 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01118-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01118-4

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing