INTRODUCTION

An essential feature of drug addiction is deficient inhibitory control (IC), or the ability to withhold a prepotent or overlearned response. These deficits manifest themselves not only in an inability to control urges to use drugs (Kalivas and Volkow, 2005) but also in an inability to inhibit responses not directly related to drug use. Numerous studies, for instance, have demonstrated that addicted individuals perform worse than healthy controls on laboratory tasks requiring IC (Fillmore and Rush, 2002; Lane et al, 1998; Spinella, 2002).

Deficits in IC among drug abusers are thought to result from aberrant functioning of frontal lobe areas that control the balance between behavioral activation and inhibition (Koob and Volkow, 2009). Supportive neuroimaging studies have shown relative hypoactivation of frontal and cingulate brain areas during response inhibition in cocaine users compared with non-users (Hester and Garavan, 2004; Kaufman et al, 2003; Li et al, 2008). Similarly, marijuana users exhibit differential patterns of right frontal and cingulate activation during response inhibition (Gruber and Yurgelun-Todd, 2005; Tapert et al, 2007). In addition, other cortical areas, including right insula (Kaufman et al, 2003), have been implicated in IC deficits.

Nicotine-dependent individuals, in addition to having baseline IC deficits as seen in other addicted populations (Spinella, 2002), exhibit further worsening of IC after tobacco cessation (Hatsukami et al, 1989; McClernon et al, 2008; Pettiford et al, 2007; Powell et al, 2002; Zack et al, 2001). In laboratory studies, abstinence from smoking has been shown to result in worse antisaccade performance (Pettiford et al, 2007; Powell et al, 2002) and more commission errors on continuous performance tasks (CPT) (Hatsukami et al, 1989; McClernon et al, 2008; Zack et al, 2001). Moreover, the administration of nicotine lozenges or gum to abstinent smokers results in better inhibitory control compared with placebo (Dawkins et al, 2007; Larrison et al, 2004), suggesting that the effects of smoking abstinence on response inhibition are due to abstinence from nicotine and not due to non-nicotine factors associated with smoking (eg, taste, behavioral ritual, and so on; also see Bekker et al, 2005). Finally, the degree to which nicotine (4 mg lozenge) improves antisaccade performance after overnight abstinence was found to positively correlate with self-reported nicotine dependence and also predicted relapse during the first week after quitting (Powell et al, 2004). As such, abstinence-induced IC deficits may be a critical mechanism underlying smoking lapse/relapse.

To our knowledge, no studies to date have investigated the neural substrates of IC deficits during smoking withdrawal. In this preliminary study, dependent smokers underwent functional MRI scans while they completed an ‘X–Y’ variant of the Go/No-Go task (Garavan et al, 2003; Garavan et al, 2002). On the basis of previous studies, we hypothesized that smoking abstinence would worsen IC (ie, increase errors of commission on stop trials). With respect to imaging data, we hypothesized that smoking abstinence would either (1) decrease activation in response to STOP signals reflecting a functional deficit in IC or (2) increase activation possibly reflecting compensatory activation. To minimize type I error, we restricted our imaging analyses to two regions of interest (ROIs) identified in a meta-analysis of Go/No-Go studies (Simmonds et al, 2008) and thought to be critical substrates of inhibitory control (Aron and Poldrack, 2005; Garavan et al, 2006): right inferior frontal cortext (rIFC) and presupplementary motor area (pre-SMA).

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

A total of 18 adult dependent smokers (7 women) completed all aspects of the study. To be enrolled, participants had to report smoking 10 cigarettes per day for at least 2 years, have an afternoon expired-air carbon monoxide (CO) level greater than 10 p.p.m. (to establish smoking status), be right handed, free of serious health problems (eg, hypertension), not currently undergoing treatment for a psychiatric illness, free of medications altering CNS functioning, test negative for illicit drug use, not have any conditions making MRI research unsafe, and, among females, have a negative serum pregnancy test. Two participants were excluded from analyses because they reported falling asleep in the scanner; one participant was excluded because of an incidental finding during acquisition of a structural scan. Thus, the final sample consisted of 15 dependent smokers.

Procedure

All participants first completed a 1.5-h screening session, in which they read and signed an Institutional Review Board-approved informed consent form, completed questionnaires regarding smoking history and suitability for fMRI research, and provided a breath sample. They were also placed in a mock scanner in order to habituate them to the scanning environment.

Participants who passed screening then completed a 1-h training session in which they were familiarized with the task. After training, participants completed two fMRI sessions: once while smoking satiated and once after a 24-h period of biologically verified smoking abstinence, which, as in previous studies, they were required to maintain for an additional 24 h after the scanning session (McClernon et al, 2009; Kozink et al, 2010). Requiring 24-h abstinence after the abstinence-condition scan minimizes any confounds that might stem from anticipating smoking. As part of the informed consent process, participants were informed that participation would require 48-h abstinence and signed an honor statement promising to maintain abstinence for that period of time. Session order was counterbalanced. Continued abstinence was also verified 24 h after the abstinent-condition scanning session during a 0.5-h quit check in which breath and saliva samples were collected.

Biochemical Verification

Expired-air CO concentrations were measured using a handheld CO monitor (Vitalograph, Lenexa, KS) and calculated by subtracting the background (ambient) CO from the peak CO reading. Criterion CO in the abstinence condition was 8 p.p.m. Saliva samples were stored in an on-site −80°C freezer. Assays for nicotine (collected at scanning sessions and 24-h quit check and used to characterize manipulation effects on nicotine levels) were performed using gas chromatography modified for use of a capillary column (Jacob et al, 1981).

Nicotine dependence was measured using the six-item Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence (FTND; Heatherton et al, 1991).

fMRI Task

Participants completed an X–Y Go/NoGo task (Garavan et al, 2002). Briefly, participants were presented with a streaming, alternating pattern of the letters X and Y at 1 Hz. They were instructed to press the button for each stimulus presented (GO trials), unless the alternating pattern was disrupted, at which point they were to withhold their response to the repeated letter (STOP trials). The interval between STOP trials was jittered between 10 and 24 s (mean=16.5 s). To balance performance across the sample, stimulus timing was individually tailored on the basis of a participant's performance at training. Stimulus onset asynchrony was held constant at 1 s, but the duration of the stimulus was manipulated between 600, 700, 800, and 900 ms, followed by a blank screen of 400, 300, 200, or 100 ms, respectively. The appropriate timing was selected on the basis of the parameters that produced at least 50% accuracy on STOP trials, calculated as the number of correctly inhibited responses on STOP trials divided by the total number of STOP trials. Total task time was 12.5 min and included 708 GO stimuli and 42 STOP stimuli. Accuracy for GO and STOP trials and reaction time (RT) on GO trials were analyzed using paired t-tests in SPSS.

Imaging Methods

Scanning was preformed on a 4.0-T GE LX NVi scanner with 41-mT/m gradients (General Electric, Waukesha, Wisconsin). Blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) functional images were collected for 34 contiguous slices (4 mm thick) parallel to the horizontal plane connecting the anterior and posterior commissures. An inverse spiral pulse sequence sensitive to BOLD contrast was used, with TR=1.5 s, TE=6 ms, FOV=24 cm, matrix=64 × 64, flip angle=60°, and in-plane resolution=3.75 mm2. After completion of the functional data collection, a T1-weighted 3D fast spoiled gradient-recalled (FSPGR) structural image was collected for 68 slices (1.9 mm thick), with TR=12.3 s, TE=5.4 ms, FOV=24 cm, matrix=256 × 256, flip angle=20°, and in-plane resolution=0.9375 mm2.

Data Analysis

Preprocessing was conducted using statistical parametric mapping software (SPM5; Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London) to remove noise and artifacts. The first four volumes of each run were discarded to allow for T1 stabilization. All functional images underwent correction for acquisition timing and for head motion using rigid-body rotation and translation (Friston et al, 1994). Each participant's data were then subsequently warped into a standard stereotaxic space (Montreal Neurological Institute) with an isotropic 2 mm voxel size and smoothed with an 8 mm FWHM Gaussian filter.

Each participant's data from each session were entered into a first-level whole brain analysis using the General Linear Model (Friston et al, 1994). Two event types were included in the model: correctly inhibited responses (STOPS) and errors of commission. Each event type was coded as a delta regressor and convolved with a canonical HRF. Motion parameters were also included in the model as a nuisance covariate. WFU Pickatlas (Maldjian et al, 2004; Maldjian et al, 2003) was used to create the two ROIs of interest (rIFC and pre-SMA) by drawing spheres with 10 mm radii centered around x=40, y=30, z=26 and x=2, y=18, z=40, respectively (Simmonds et al, 2008). Using MarsBar (Brett et al, 2002), percentage of BOLD signal change for the correctly identified STOP trials within each ROI was extracted for each participant and condition. The signal values from the peak of the hemodynamic response (HDR, 6 s after onset) were then input into SPSS 17.0 to examine differences in the amplitude of response between conditions using paired t-tests. Given the small number of tests conducted, all hypotheses were tested at p<0.05.

RESULTS

Sample Characteristics

The final sample (n=15) was 40.0% female (n=6). Reported racial/ethnic group membership was 86.7% Caucasian (n=13) and 13.3% African American (n=2). Mean age was 31.4 years (SD=10.4). The sample reported smoking a mean of 16.2 cigarettes/day (SD=4.1) for 14.2 years (SD=9.4). The mean FTND score was 4.8 (SD=1.6), suggesting that the sample was moderately nicotine dependent. At screening, the mean expired-breath CO concentration was 24.7 (SD=13.6), whereas the mean salivary cotinine was 269.9 ng/ml (SD=146.8).

Abstinence Verification

Expired-breath CO concentrations indicated compliance with study requirements. In the satiated condition, mean CO level (p.p.m.) was 29.2 (SD=20.3), whereas in the 24-h abstinent condition, mean CO was 2.2 (SD=0.9). Mean CO 24 h after the abstinent fMRI session was 2.0 (SD=2.1).

The results of salivary nicotine analyses were consistent with CO results and also indicative of compliance. In the satiated and abstinent conditions, mean salivary nicotine (ng/ml) was 406.8 (SD=293.7) and 13.9 (SD=8.6), respectively.

Behavioral Results

Performance variables (GO trial accuracy and RT; STOP trial accuracy) were entered into paired t-tests evaluating the effects of condition (satiated, abstinent). Consistent with previous studies, abstinence was associated with significantly decreased accuracy on STOP trials (t=−3.16, p=0.007; satiated mean=75.2%, SD=13.1; abstinent mean=62.9%, SD=22.1). Smoking abstinence significantly affected neither GO accuracy (p=0.1; satiated mean=99.5, SD=0.8; abstinent mean=98.8, SD=1.8) nor RT (p=0.86; satiated mean=334.3, SD=47.9; abstinent mean=332.8, SD=63.0).

fMRI Results

We examined the effect of smoking condition on HDR amplitude in rIFC and pre-SMA ROIs. The amplitude of HDR at 6 s after stimulus onset in rIFC in response to STOP trials was greater in the abstinent as compared with the satiated condition (t(14)=−2.84, p=0.026, see Figure 1), whereas smoking condition had no effect on pre-SMA activity.

Figure 1
figure 1

Percentage signal change in rIFC (left column) and pre-SMA (right column) regions of interests (ROIs) for STOP trials in the satiated and 24-h abstinent conditions. Top row, ROIs were spheres with 10 mm radii centered around x=40, y=30, z=26 and x=2, y=18, z=40 (MNI) for rIFC and pre-SMA, respectively (Simmonds et al, 2008). Bottom row. Signal at the peak of the hemodynamic response was greater in the abstinent as compared with satiated condition, p=0.026 in rIFC; no difference was observed in pre-SMA.

PowerPoint slide

DISCUSSION

This study used event-related fMRI to examine the effects of smoking abstinence on neural correlates of IC during a Go/No-Go task. Consistent with previous studies, smoking abstinence worsened IC as measured by errors of commission on STOP trials, while leaving GO trial performance unaffected. In addition, smoking abstinence resulted in increased IC activity in rIFC compared with satiety, but had no effect on pre-SMA. The findings provide novel information regarding the effects of smoking abstinence on neural activity associated with IC and suggest that greater neural resources are dedicated to IC during withdrawal in order to maintain performance.

Effects of Smoking Abstinence on Inhibitory Control

In the present study, smoking abstinence worsened IC as evidenced by more errors of commission on STOP trials of a Go/NoGo task. The size of this effect was large (Cohen's d=0.93), with 87% (13 of 15) participants exhibiting decreased IC. At the same time, neither RT nor accuracy on GO trials was affected by smoking abstinence. These findings replicate numerous previous studies that have observed abstinence-induced IC deficits across multiple tasks, including antisaccade and continuous performance tasks (Hatsukami et al, 1989; McClernon et al, 2008; Pettiford et al, 2007; Powell et al, 2002). For instance, we previously showed that overnight smoking abstinence decreased smokers’ ability to withhold shifting their gaze to novel visual stimuli (antisaccade trials), but had no effect on prosaccade trial accuracy (Pettiford et al, 2007).

The imaging results of the present study provide novel evidence that IC deficits during withdrawal are associated with modulation of transient brain activation in rIFC but not in the medial frontal (pre-SMA) cortex. We specifically chose an ROI in right IFC as 1) activations in this region have been observed across numerous imaging studies of IC (Garavan et al, 2006; Garavan et al, 1999; Konishi et al, 1999) and 2) lesions of this region result in IC deficits (Aron et al, 2003; Aron et al, 2004). Pre-SMA has similarly been implicated in inhibitory control (Aron et al, 2007; Floden and Stuss, 2006; Isoda and Hikosaka, 2007; Mostofsky et al, 2003). Activation in these areas is common to inhibition across response modalities (Xue et al, 2008). In a recent meta-analysis of Go/No-Go fMRI studies (Simmonds et al, 2008), successful inhibition most robustly activated regions in (1) the right frontal cortex on the border between IFG and MFG and (2) pre-SMA. The coordinates of these activations were used as the centers of the ROI used in our analysis.

The exact nature of the role of rIFC in IC remains unclear. The rIFC is part of the ventral attention system involved in the orienting of attention to behaviorally relevant or salient stimuli (Corbetta et al, 2008; Hampshire et al, 2009; Linden et al, 1999), and recent imaging studies suggest that its role in IC is more related to these attentional functions than to the cessation of behavior per se (Chao et al, 2009; Duann et al, 2009; Hampshire et al, 2010; Sharp et al, 2010). For instance, a recent fMRI study in which attentional and behavioral control aspects of IC were dissociated showed that rIFC is not involved in IC after removing activation associated with attentional capture (Sharp et al, 2010). In this same study, activation in pre-SMA survived any correction for attentional processes. In addition, a more refined view of rIFC is emerging, in which specific subregions are differentially involved in aspects of inhibitory control, including selective attention to task-relevant stimuli and updating of behavioral plans (Chikazoe et al, 2009; Verbruggen et al, 2010). In an fMRI study using a Go/No-Go task that included infrequent GO in addition to infrequent STOP trials (Chikazoe et al, 2009), the posterior aspect of the inferior frontal gyrus (pIFG) is active in response to STOP trials after removing activation in response to infrequent GO trials (ie, response inhibition specific), whereas activation is observed in inferior frontal junction (IFJ) by infrequent as compared with frequent GO trials (ie, orienting of attention novel/salient stimuli). Similarly, an rTMS study (Verbruggen et al, 2010) that selectively inactivated right IFC subregions found that rIFG is critical to inhibitory control through its updating of action plans, whereas IFJ is involved in attention to task-critical stimuli. As such, the accumulating literature suggests that successful IC is dependent on right frontal-mediated attentional control and action planning/updating, and medial prefrontal (pre-SMA)-mediated behavioral control (ie, cessation of a prepotent response).

Our analysis was restricted to trials in which participants successfully inhibited a response. As such, the finding that smoking abstinence increased transient activation in rIFC suggests the possibility that abstinence places increased attentional demands in order to successfully inhibit behavior even at suboptimal levels. Other studies have observed similar effects of smoking abstinence to increase (or nicotine to decrease) transient activation during attention (Azizian et al, 2010; Kozink et al, 2010; Thiel and Fink, 2008; Vossel et al, 2008). In addition, consistent with the interpretation that smoking abstinence increases demands on IFC processing to maintain performance, drug users, compared with non-drug-using controls, exhibit increased frontal activation during successful inhibition, even when no differences in performance are observed (Roberts and Garavan, 2010; Tapert et al, 2007).

Although our findings suggest that smoking abstinence modulates rIFC function, our findings do not rule out the possibility that smoking abstinence alters the operation of behavioral control centers including pre-SMA, as we only examined trials in which behavioral control was successful. However, the fact that smoking abstinence did not increase activation in pre-SMA suggests the possibility that no additional demands are placed on this region in the withholding of responses. As such, our findings provide preliminary evidence that abstinence-induced inhibitory control deficits may involve deficits of attention to a greater degree than motor inhibition. This interpretation is consistent with a large literature showing that smoking abstinence robustly disrupts attentional processes (Heishman et al, 1994; Levin et al, 2006), including the detection of novel stimuli (Froeliger et al, 2009).

Limitation and Future Directions

Strengths of this study include the within-subjects design, use of a well-established task with known neural correlates, and an ROI-based analytical approach that reduces risk of type I error. However, several limitations should be noted. First, the sample size is small and, as such, the results should be considered preliminary. Moreover, the lack of a non-smoking control group complicates interpretation—greater activation in the abstinent condition may reflect either a return to normal presmoking levels or a disruption of baseline functioning. Assessments at time points more distal to abstinence would also help to address this question. In addition, relative to GO stimuli, STOP signals in the current paradigm are rare and thus responses to them may reflect inhibitory control, the detection of a novel stimulus, or some combination of these factors. Future studies that incorporate paradigms with tasks that differentiate neural activity associated with these two processes can help elucidate this question. On a related note, we used a single rIFC ROI based on previous research using the X-Y Go/No-Go paradigm. However, as noted above, there is important functional variation in IFC subregions—future studies can make use of multiple IFC ROIs to further pinpoint smoking abstinence effects. Finally, previous research has examined brain activation in response to errors of commission on STOP trials; however, adequate numbers of such trials were not available for analysis in the present study. Future studies in which task difficulty is adaptively modified to increase errors of commission can better address this issue.

CONCLUSIONS

This study is a novel investigation of the neural substrates that underlie disruption of IC during withdrawal and adds to the growing database on the effects of smoking abstinence on neurocognitive functioning (McClernon and Gilbert, 2004). We observed that smoking abstinence increased activation in rIFC, which suggests that IC deficits following abstinence are due to increased demand on attention processes rather than the disruption or modulation of stopping processes per se. Future studies using tasks that allow for the dissociation of attentional and behavioral control aspects of IC can shed additional light on the mechanisms that underlie abstinence-induced IC deficits. In addition, the present results suggest that rIFC activation may be a clinically relevant biomarker of smoking cessation treatment effects and a potential target for new interventions.