Main

We have studied the spatial misalignment perceived between moving and strobed objects and find that it varies systematically with the luminance of the objects. Our results favour an explanation for these perceived misalignments based on differential visual latencies, rather than on motion extrapolation. Thus, accurate visually guided motor actions are likely to depend on motor instead of perceptual compensation.

Evidence for a mechanism based on motion extrapolation1,2,3 comes from the flash-lag phenomenon4, in which a continuously moving object is perceived to be ahead of a stationary strobed object when the two retinal images are physically aligned. But because visual latency varies according to the properties of a stimulus, including its luminance5,9, this mechanism would have to compensate appropriately for a range of stimulus-dependent variations in latency to ensure that real-time, visually guided responses are accurate. An alternative, previous explanation, invoking a longer delay for the processing of a flashing stimulus, was based on attentional mechanisms10.

According to the hypothesis based on differential visual latencies, the observed spatial lead of the moving central segment in Fig. 1a is directly proportional to the difference between the latencies of the strobed and the moving central segments. For a given stimulus, the visual latency varies inversely with its luminance5,9, so the observed spatial lead in the flash-lag paradigm should vary according to the luminance of the strobed and moving central segments. Increasing the luminance of the moving central segments but not that of the strobed segments should decrease the delay of the moving central segment (dm) while that of the strobed segment (ds) remains constant. The latency-difference hypothesis therefore predicts that the observed spatial lead of the moving central segment should increase.

Figure 1: Luminance-dependent misalignments between moving and strobed targets.
figure 1

a, The stimulus was a continuously rotating central segment (40 rev min−1) and two flanking strobed segments (5 ms). b,The observed temporal lead of the moving central segment is shown as a function of its detectability (0 = threshold) for three subjects (S.S.P., G.P. and T.L.N.). Positive values on the y-axis represent a temporal lead in perceiving the moving central segment relative to the strobed segments. The observed spatial lead was converted into a temporal lead by dividing it by the velocity of the moving central segment. c,The observed temporal lead of the moving central segment is shown as a function of the detectability of the strobed segments (0 = threshold) for the same subjects.

To test this prediction, we measured the spatial lead of the moving central segment as a function of the detectability of the central segment while keeping the detectability of the strobed segments constant. Here we use detectability to refer to the number of log units of luminance (Lu) above the detection threshold; detectability of the strobed segments was 0.3 Lu for subjects S.S.P. and G.P., and 0.5 Lu for T.L.N. The temporal lead of the moving central segment averaged across subjects increases systematically from 20 to 70 ms when its detectability increases by 1.0 Lu (Fig. 1b).

Increasing the luminance of the strobed segments while keeping that of the moving central segment constant should decrease ds, while dmremains constant. The latency-difference hypothesis predicts that the observed spatial lead of the moving central segment should decrease and, if the luminance of the strobed segments is high enough, the moving central segment should be perceived to lag behind spatially. We tested this prediction by measuring spatial lead as a function of the detectability of the strobed segments, while keeping the detectability of the moving central segment constant (1.5 Lu above the detection threshold for subjects G.P. and T.L.N., and 0.8 Lu for S.S.P.). The observed temporal lead of the moving central segment averaged across subjects decreases systematically from 80 to −30 ms as the detectability of the strobed segments increases by 1.5 to 2.0 Lu (Fig. 1c).

These results support predictions of the latency-difference hypothesis and show that the motion-extrapolation mechanism does not compensate for stimulus-dependent variations in latency. Indeed, theoretical calculations show that the putative motion-extrapolation mechanism must be undercompensating by at least 120 ms to account for the data in Fig. 1. But a motion-extrapolation mechanism that does not adequately compensate for variations in visual latency would not appreciably improve the accuracy of real-time visually guided behaviour.