Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000054

Perception of risk to a stimulus associated with negative affect may depend on the level of emotional arousal it elicits. Sensation seeking is associated with a decreased level of arousal to risk stimuli. Individuals high in sensation seeking may require greater emotional arousal in order to induce harm avoidance. Poor control increases the salience of emotional cues, which may increase the magnitude of emotion associated with risk stimuli. In the present study, we found that, as sensation seeking decreased, high levels of poor control increased the likelihood of experiencing an emesis perception following disgust. The results support a harm avoidance system tied to emotions elicited from immediate threat that is potentiated by poor control.

References

  • Aiken, L. S., West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newburg Park, CA: Sage. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ajcardi, R. m., Therme, P. (2008). Sensation seeking, affective profile, and sports behavior in the prediction of risk. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 39, 356–372. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bywaters, M., Andrade, J. , Turpin, G. (2004). Determinants of the vividness of visual imagery: The effects of delayed recall, stimulus affect and individual differences. Memory, 12, 479–488. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carver, C. S. (2005). Impulse and constraint: Perspectives from personality psychology, convergence with theory in other areas, and potential for Integration. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 312–333. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Caseras, F. X., Fullana, M. A., Riba, J. , Barbanoj, M. J., Aluja, A. , Torrubia, R. (2006). Influence of individual differences in the Behavioral Inhibition System and stimulus content (fear versus blood-disgust) on affective startle reflex modulation. Biological Psychology, 72, 251–256. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chen, K. , Sheth, A. , Elliott, D. , Yeager, A. (2004). Prevalence and correlates of past-year substance use, abuse, and dependence in a suburban community sample of high-school students. Addictive Behaviors, 29, 413–423. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Curtis, V. , Aunger, R. , & Rabie, T. (2004). Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proceedings of the Royal Society London B, 271, S131–133. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Donohew, L., Zimmerman, R., Cupp, P. S. , Novak, S. , Colon, S. , & Abell, R. (2000). Sensation seeking, impulsive decision making, and risky sex: Implications for risk-taking and design of interventions. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 1079–1091. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dvorak, R. D., Simons, J. S. (2009). Moderation of resource depletion in the self-control strength model: Differing effects of two modes of self-control. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 572–583. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eysenck, S. B. G., Pearson, P. R., Easting, G. , Allsopp, J. F. (1985). Age norms for impulsiveness, venturesomeness and empathy in adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 613–619. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Feij, J. A., Orlebeke, J. F., Gazendam, A., Van Ziulen, R. W. (1985). Sensation seeking: Measurement and psychophysiological correlates. In J. Strelau F. H. Farley A. Gale, Eds., The biological bases of personality and behavior. Vol. 1: Theories, measurement techniques, and development (pp. 195–210). New York: Hemisphere/Harper. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fessler, D. M. T., Eng, S. J. , Navarrete, C. D. (2005). Elevated disgust sensitivity in the first trimester of pregnancy: Evidence supporting the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 344–351. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fink, B. , Hamdaoui, A., Wenig, F. , Neave, N. (2010). Hand-grip strength and sensation seeking. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 789–793. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Giancola, P. R., Zeichner, A. , Newbolt, W. H. , & Stennett, R. B. (1994). Construct validity of the dimensions of Cloninger’s Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 627–636. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kendall, P. C., Williams, C. L. (1982). Assessing the cognitive and behavioral components of children’s self-management. In P. Karoly F. H. Kanfer, Eds., Self-management and behavior change (pp. 240–284). New York: Pergamon. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • LaRowe, S. D., Patrick, C. J., Curtin, J. J. , Kline, J. P. (2006). Personality correlates of startle habituation. Biological Psychology, 72, 257–264. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lissek, S. , Baas, J. M. P., Pine, D. S., Orme, K., Dvir, S. , Rosenberger, E. , Grillon, C. (2005). Sensation seeking and the aversive motivational system. Emotion, 5, 396–407. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • López Rodrigo, A. M., Paíno Pi¤eiro, M. M. , Martínez Suárez, P. C. , Caro, M. I. , & Lemos Giráldez, S. (1997). Hallucinations in a normal population: Imagery and personality influences. Psychology in Spain, 1, 10–16. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Oaten, M. , Stevenson, R. J., Case, T. I. (2009). Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 303–321. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Olatunji, B. O., McKay, D. (2006). Further exploration of the role of disgust sensitivity in anxiety and related disorders. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 19, 331–334. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Olatunji, B. O., Sawchuk, C. N., Lohr, J. M., de Jong, P. J. (2004). Disgust domains in the prediction of contamination fear. Behavior Research and Therapy, 42, 93–104. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Olatunji, B. O., Unoka, Z. S., Beran, E., David, B. , Armstrong, T. (2009). Disgust sensitivity and psychopathological symptoms: Distinctions from harm avoidance. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 31, 137–142. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rosenbloom, T. (2003). Risk evaluation and risky behavior of high and low sensation seekers. Social Behavior and Personality, 31, 375–386. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sales, B. D., Folkman, S. (2000). Ethics in research with human participants. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Simons, J. S., Dvorak, R. D., Batien, B. D. , Wray, T. B. (2010). Event-level associations between affect, alcohol intoxication, and acute dependence symptoms: Effects of urgency, self-control, and drinking experience. Addictive Behaviors, 35, 1045–1053. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Simons, J. S., Gaher, R. M., Oliver, M. N. I. , Bush, J. A., Palmer, M. A. (2005). An experience sampling study of associations between affect and alcohol use and problems among college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 66, 459–469. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, B. D., Perlstein, W. M. , Davidson, R. A., Michael, K. (1986). Sensation seeking: Differential effects of relevant, novel stimulation on electrodermal activity. Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 445–452. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wills, T. A., Cleary, S., Filer, M., Shinar, O. , Mariani, J. , & Spera, K. (2001). Temperament related to early-onset substance use: Test of a developmental model. Prevention Science, 2, 145–163. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wills, T. A., Walker, C. , Mendoza, D. , & Ainette, M. G. (2006). Behavioral and emotional self-control: Relations to substance use in samples of middle and high school students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 20, 265–278. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zheng, Y. , Xu, J. , Jin, Y. , Sheng, W. , Ma, Y. , Zhang, X. , & Shen, H. (2010). The time course of novelty processing in sensation seeking: An ERP study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 76, 57–63. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zuckerman, M. (1994). An alternative five-factor model for personality. In D. H. Hall C. F. Iverson, Jr. G. A. Kohnstamm R. P. Martin, Eds., The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 53–68). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Zuckerman, M. (2007). Sensation seeking and risk. In Sensation seeking and risky behavior (pp. 51–72). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zuckerman, M. , Como, P. (1983). Sensation seeking and arousal systems. Personality and Individual Differences, 4, 381–386. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zuckerman, M., Strelau, J., Farley, F. H. , Gale, A. (1985). Biological foundations of the sensation-seeking temperament. In J. Strelau F. H. Farley A. Gale, Eds., The biological bases of personality and behavior. Vol. 1: Theories, measurement techniques, and development (pp. 97–113). New York: Hemisphere/Harper. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar