Skip to main content
Log in

Societal and Managerial Implications of Implicit Social Cognition: Why Milliseconds Matter

  • Published:
Social Justice Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This article argues for the vulnerability of managerial work to unintended forms of racial and other bias. Recent insights into “implicit social cognition” are summarized, highlighting the prevalence of those mental processes that are relatively unconscious and automatic, and employed in understanding the self and others. Evidence from a response-time measure of implicit bias, the Implicit Association Test, (“IAT”; Greenwald, McGhee, and Schwartz, 1998) illustrates this phenomenon. Recent work on the predictive validity of the IAT demonstrates that social cognitive pitfalls threaten a) managers' explicit commitments to egalitarianism and meritocracy and b) managers' performance in their three primary roles of processing information, interacting with others, and making decisions (Mintzberg, 1973). Implicit bias influences managerial behavior in unexpected ways, and this influence is heightened in the messy, pressured, and distracting environments in which managers operate.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Asendorpf, J. B., Banse, R., and Muecke, D. (2002). Double dissociation between implicit and explicit personality self-concept: The case of shy behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 83(2): 380-393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Banaji, M. R. (2001). Implicit attitudes can be measured. In Roediger, H. L., III, Nairne, J. S., Neath, I., and Surprenant, A. M. (eds.), The Nature of Remembering: Essays in Honor of Robert G. Crowder, APA, Washington, DC, pp. 117-150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bertrand, M., and Mullainathan, S. (in press). Are Emily and Greg more employable than Lakisha and Jamal? A field experiment on labor market discrimination. Am. Econ. Rev.

  • Blair, I. V. (2002). The malleability of automatic stereotypes and prejudices. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 6: 242-261.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blank, R. M. (2001). An overview of trends in social and economic well-being, by race. In Smelser, N. J., Wilson, W. J., and Mitchell, F. (eds.), Racial Trends and Their Consequences, Vol. 1, National Academy Press, Washington, DC, pp. 21-39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brief, A. P., Dietz, J., Cohen, R. R., Pugh, S., and Vaslow, J. B. (2000). Just doing business: Modern racism and obedience to authority as explanations for employment discrimination. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 81(1): 72-97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J. A., and Jarvis, W. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychol. Bull. 119(2): 197-253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dasgupta, N., McGhee, D. E., Greenwald, A. G., and Banaji, M. R. (2000). Automatic preference for White Americans: Eliminating the familiarity explanation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 36(3): 316-328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, J. A., and Smith, T. W. (1994). General Social Surveys, 1972–1994: Cumulative Codebook, NORC, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deitch, E. A., Barsky, A., Butz, R. M., Chan, S., Brief, A. P., and Bradley, J. (2003). Subtle yet significant: The existence and impact of everyday racial discrimination in the workplace. Social Relations, 56(11), 1299-1324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. J. Exp. Pers. Psychol. 56(1): 5-18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Devine, P. G., Plant, E., Amodio, D. M., Harmon-Jones, E., and Vance, S. L. (2002). The regulation of explicit and implicit race bias: The role of motivations to respond without prejudice. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82(5): 835-848.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F. (2001). On the nature of contemporary prejudice: The third wave. J. Soc. Issues 57(4): 829-849.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., and Gaertner, S. L. (1991). Changes in the expression and assessment of racial prejudice. In Knopke, H. J., Norrell, R. J., and Roberts, R. W., Opening Doors: Perspectives on Race Relations in Contemporary America (pp 119-148). Tusculoosa: University of Alabama Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., and Gaertner, S. L. (2000). Aversive racism and selection decisions: 1989 and 1999. Psychol. Sci. 11(4): 315-319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., Validzic, A., Matoka, K., Johnson, B., and Frazier, S. (1997a). Extending the benefits of recategorization: Evaluations, self-disclosure, and helping. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 33(4): 401-420.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., and Gaertner, S. L. (2002). Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82(1): 62-68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., Johnson, C., Johnson, B., and Howard, A. (1997b). On the nature of prejudice: Automatic and controlled processes. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 33(5): 510-540.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egloff, B., and Schmukle, S. C. (2002). Predictive validity of an Implicit Association Test for assessing anxiety. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 83(6): 1441-1455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The MODE model as an integrative framework. In Zanna, M. P. (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 23, Academic Press, New York, pp. 75-109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., and Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobstrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69(6): 1013-1027.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., and Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and uses. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 54: 297-327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Florack, A., Scarabis, M., and Bless, H. (2001). When do associations matter? The use of automatic associations toward ethnic groups in person judgements. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 37(6): 518-524.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaertner, S. L., and Dovidio, J. F. (1977). The subtlety of White racism, arousal, and helping behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 35(10): 691-707.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaertner, S. L., and Dovidio, J. F. (1986). The aversive form of racism. In Dovidio, J. F., and Gaertner, S. L. (ed.), Prejudice, Discrimination, and Racism, Academic Press, Orlando, FL, pp. 61-89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaertner, S. L., and Dovidio, J. F. (2000). The aversive form of racism. In Stangor, C. (ed.), Stereotypes and Prejudice: Essential Readings Key Readings in Social Psychology, Psychology Press, College Park, MD, pp. 289-304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski, B., Ehrenberg, K., Banse, R., Zukova, J., and Klauer, K. C. (2003). It's in the mind of the beholder: The impact of stereotypic associations on category-based and individuating impression formation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 39(1): 16-30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., and Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychol. Rev. 102(1): 4-27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., and Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74(6): 1464-1480.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., and Nosek, B. A. (2001). Health of the Implicit Association Test at age 3. Z. Exp. Psychol. 48(2): 85-93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., and Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85(2): 197-216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hugenberg, K., and Bodenhausen, G. V. (2003). Facing prejudice: Implicit prejudice and the perception of facial threat. Psychological Science, 14(6): 640-643.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jamieson, D. W., and Zanna, M. P. (1989). Need for structure in attitude formation and expression. In Pratkanis, A. R., Breckler, S. J., et al. Attitude Structure and Function, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 383-406. (The third Ohio State University volume on attitudes and persuasion, xiv, 462 pp.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Kihlstrom, J. F. (2004). Implicit methods in social psychology. In Sansone, C., Morf, C. C., and Panter, A. T. (eds.), The SAGE Handbook of Methods in Social Psychology, Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., and Freund, T. (1983). The freezing and unfreezing of lay-inferences: Effects on impressional primacy, ethnic stereotyping, and numerical anchoring. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 19(5): 448-468.

    Google Scholar 

  • March, J. G., and Simon, H. A. (1993). Organizations, Blackwell Business, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • McConahay, J. B. (1983). Modern racism and modern discrimination: The effects of race, racial attitudes, and context on simulated hiring decisions. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 9(4): 551-558.

    Google Scholar 

  • McConnell, A. R., and Leibold, J. M. (2001). Relations among the Implicit Association Test, discriminatory behavior, and explicit measures of racial attitudes. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 37(5): 435-442.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 67, 371-378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mintzberg, H. (1973). The Nature of Managerial Work, Harper and Row, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mintzberg, H. (1975). The manager's job: Folklore and fact. Harv. Bus. Rev. 53(4): 49-61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M., and Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dyn. Theory Res. Pract. 6(1): 101-115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perlow, L. A. (1997). Finding Time, Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pratto, F., and Bargh, J. A. (1991). Stereotyping based on apparently individuating information: Trait and global components of sex stereotypes under attention overload. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 27(1): 26-47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reskin, B. (2002). Rethinking employment discrimination and its remedies. In Guillen, M. F., Collins, R., England, P., and Meyer, M. (eds.), New Directions in Economic Sociology, Sage, New York, pp. 218-244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richeson, J. A., and Shelton, J. (2003). When prejudice does not pay: Effects of interracial contact on executive function. Psychol. Sci. 14(3): 287-290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A. (2004). Shining a spotlight on implicit biases: Their nature, causes, and consequences. Soc. Justice Res. 17(2): 129-142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A., Feinberg, J., and Fairchild, K. (in press). Minority members' Implicit attitudes: Automatic ingroup bias as a function of group status. Soc. Cog. 20(4): 294-320.

  • Rudman, L. A., and Glick, P. (2001). Prescriptive gender stereotypes and backlash toward agentic women. J. Soc. Issues. 57(4): 743-762.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A., and Lee, M. R. (2002). Implicit and explicit consequences of exposure to violent and misogynous rap music. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 5(2): 133-150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sargent, M. J., and Theil, A. (2001). When do Implicit Attitudes Predict Behavior? On the Moderating Role of Attributional Ambiguity, Unpublished manuscript.

  • Schuette, R. A., and Fazio, R. H. (1995). Attitude accessibility and motivation as determinants of biased processing: A test of the MODE model. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 21(7): 704-710.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair, L., and Kunda, Z. (1999). Reactions to a Black professional: Motivated inhibition and activation of conflicting stereotypes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 77(5): 885-904.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. (1994). In Search of the Human Mind, Harcourt Brace College Publishers, Fort Worth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uhlmann, E. (2003). IAT Behavioral Validation Studies, Working List, Yale University, New Haven, CT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M. (2002). The Illusion of Conscious Will, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., and Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychol. Rev. 107(1): 101-126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Word, C. O., Zanna, M. P., and Cooper, J. (1974). The nonverbal mediation of self-fulfilling prophecies in interracial interaction. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 10(2): 109-120.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dolly Chugh.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Chugh, D. Societal and Managerial Implications of Implicit Social Cognition: Why Milliseconds Matter. Social Justice Research 17, 203–222 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1023/B:SORE.0000027410.26010.40

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/B:SORE.0000027410.26010.40

Navigation