Skip to main content
Log in

Designing Instructional Examples to Reduce Intrinsic Cognitive Load: Molar versus Modular Presentation of Solution Procedures

  • Published:
Instructional Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

It is usually assumed that successful problemsolving in knowledge-rich domains depends onthe availability of abstract problem-typeschemas whose acquisition can be supported bypresenting students with worked examples.Conventionally designed worked examples oftenfocus on information that is related to themain components of problem-type schemas, namelyon information related to problem-categorymembership, structural task features, andcategory-specific solution procedures. However,studying these examples might be cognitivelydemanding because it requires learners tosimultaneously hold active a substantial amountof information in working memory. In ourresearch, we try to reduce intrinsic cognitiveload in example-based learning by shifting thelevel of presenting and explaining solutionprocedures from a `molar' view – that focuseson problem categories and their associatedoverall solution procedures – to a more`modular' view where complex solutions arebroken down into smaller meaningful solutionelements that can be conveyed separately. Wereview findings from five of our own studiesthat yield evidence for the fact thatprocessing modular examples is associated witha lower degree of intrinsic cognitive load andthus, improves learning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Atkinson, R.K., Catrambone, R. & Merrill, M.M. (2003) Aiding transfer in statistics: Examining the use of conceptually oriented equations and elaborations during subgoal learning, Journal of Educational Psychology 95(4): 762–773.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R.K., Derry, S.J., Renkl, A. & Wortham, D.W. (2000) Learning from examples: Instructional principles from the worked examples research, Review of Educational Research 70(2): 181–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catrambone, R. (1994) Improving examples to improve transfer to novel problems, Memory and Cognition 22(5): 606–615.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catrambone, R. (1998) The subgoal learning model: Creating better examples to improve transfer to novel problems, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 127(4): 355–376.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catrambone, R. & Holyoak, K.J. (1989) Overcoming contextual limitations on problemsolving transfer, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 15(6): 1147–1156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M.T.H., Bassok, M., Lewis, M., Reimann, P. & Glaser, R. (1989) Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in learning to solve problems, Cognitive Science 13(2): 145–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M.T.H., de Leeuw, N., Chiu, M.-H. & LaVancher, C. (1994) Eliciting self-explanations improves understanding, Cognitive Science 18(3): 439–477.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, D.D. (1992) Role of analogical reasoning in the induction of problem categories, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 18(5): 1103–1124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derry, S.J. (1989) Strategy and expertise in solving word problems. In: C.B. McCormick, G. Miller & M. Pressley (eds), Cognitive Strategy Research: From Basic Research to Educational Applications, pp. 269–302. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gagné, R.M. (1962) The acquisition of knowledge, Psychological Review 69(4): 355–365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerjets, P. & Scheiter, K. (2003) Goal configurations and processing strategies as moderators between instructional design and cognitive load: Evidence from hypertext-based instruction, Educational Psychologist 38(1): 33–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerjets, P., Scheiter, K. & Catrambone, R. (in press) Reducing cognitive load and fostering cognitive skill acquisition: Benefits of category-avoiding examples. In: R. Alterman & D. Kirsh (eds), Proceedings of the 25th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

  • Gerjets, P., Scheiter, K. & Kleinbeck, S. (in press) Instructional examples in hypertext-based learning and problem solving: Comparing transformational and derivational approaches to example design. In: H.M. Niegemann, R. Brünken & D. Leutner (eds), Instructional Design for Multimedia Learning. Muenster: Waxmann.

  • Gerjets, P., Scheiter, K. & Tack, W.H. (2000) Resource-adaptive selection of strategies in learning from worked-out examples. In: L.R. Gleitman & A.K. Joshi (eds), Proceedings from the 22nd Annual Conference from the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 166–171. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gick, M.L. & Holyoak, K.J. (1983) Schema induction and analogical transfer, Cognitive Psychology 15(1): 1–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, S.G. & Staveland, L.E. (1988) Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results of experimental and theoretical research. In: P.A. Hancock & N. Meshkati (eds), Human Mental Workload, pp. 139–183. Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R.E. (1981) Frequency norms and structural analysis of algebra story problems into families, categories, and templates, Instructional Science 10: 135–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F.G.W.C. & Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (1994) Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem solving skills: A cognitive load approach, Journal of Educational Psychology 86(1): 122–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pirolli, P. & Recker, M. (1994) Learning strategies and transfer in the domain of programming, Cognition and Instruction 12(3): 235–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quilici, J.L. & Mayer, R.E. (1996) Role of examples in how students learn to categorize statistics word problems, Journal of Educational Psychology 88(1): 144–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reed, S.K. (1993) A schema-based theory of transfer. In: D.K. Detterman & R.J. Sternberg (eds), Transfer on Trial: Intelligence, Cognition, and Instruction, pp. 39–67. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reed, S.K. (1999) Word Problems. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reed, S.K., Dempster, A. & Ettinger, M. (1985) Usefulness of analogous solutions for solving algebra word problems, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 11(1): 106–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A. (1997) Learning from worked-out examples: A study on individual differences, Cognitive Science 21(1): 1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A. (1999) Learning mathematics from worked-out examples: Analyzing and fostering self-explanations, European Journal of Psychology of Education 14(4): 477–488.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A. (2002) Learning from worked-out examples: Instructional explanations supplement self-explanations, Learning and Instruction 12(5): 529–556.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A. & Atkinson, R.K. (2003) Structuring the transition from example study to problem solving in cognitive skills acquisition: A cognitive load perspective, Educational Psychologist 38(1): 15–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, B.H. (1989) Distinguishing types of superficial similarities: Different effects on the access and use of earlier problems, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 15(3): 456–468.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schuh, J., Gerjets, P. & Scheiter, K. (2003) Supporting learning from worked-out examples in computer-based learning environments. In: F. Schmalhofer, R. Young & G. Katz (eds), Proceedings of the European Cognitive Science Conference 2003, pp. 301–306. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sowder, L. (1985) Cognitive psychology and mathematical problem solving: A discussion of Mayer's paper. In: E.A. Silver (ed), Teaching and Learning Mathematical Problem Solving: Multiple Research Perspectives, pp. 139–146. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (1994) Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design, Learning and Instruction 4(4): 295–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. & Cooper, G. (1985) The use of worked examples as a substitute for problem solving in learning algebra, Cognition and Instruction 2(1): 59–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. & Paas, F.W.C. (1998) Cognitive architecture and instructional design, Educational Psychology Review 10(3): 251–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • VanLehn, K. (1989) Problem solving and cognitive skill acquisition. In: M.I. Posner (ed), Foundations of Cognitive Science, pp. 527–579. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • VanLehn, K. (1996) Cognitive skill acquisition, Annual Review of Psychology 47: 513–539.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Merriënboer, J.J.G., Kirschner, P.A. & Kester, L. (2003) Taking the load of a learner's mind: Instructional design for complex learning, Educational Psychologist 38(1): 5–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (1990) Strategies for programming instruction in high school: Program completion vs. program generation, Journal of Educational Computing Research 6(3): 265–287.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peter Gerjets.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gerjets, P., Scheiter, K. & Catrambone, R. Designing Instructional Examples to Reduce Intrinsic Cognitive Load: Molar versus Modular Presentation of Solution Procedures. Instructional Science 32, 33–58 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1023/B:TRUC.0000021809.10236.71

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/B:TRUC.0000021809.10236.71

Navigation