Skip to main content
Log in

The Changing Relationship Between Anatomic and Cognitive Explanation in the Neuropsychology of Language

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Changing trends in the approach to neurolinguistics are reviewed. We suggest that these trends are marked by a distinct convergence between linguistic/cognitive and anatomic/physiological approaches to the study of aphasia. With respect to the former, we cite the refinement of analysis of language symptoms and the introduction of experimental methods that reveal real-time aspects of language processing. With respect to the latter, we cite the technical advances in static and dynamic brain imaging that have allowed the in vivo analysis of lesion sites in aphasic patients, and the identification of foci of metabolic activity during linguistic/cognitive tasks in normal brains. We cite recent imaging studies of category-specific lexical dissociations as examples of the productive convergence of anatomic and technological advances to illuminate a particularly challenging problem.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Assal, G., & Buttet, J. (1973). Troubles du schéma corporel lors des atteintes hémispheriques gauches. Praxis, 62, 172-179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., McDonald, J., MacWhinney, B., & Appelbaum, M. (1991). A maximum likelihood procedure for the analysis of group and individual data in aphasia research. Brain and Language, 40, 231-265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benton, A. L., & Joynt, R. J. (1960). Early descriptions of aphasia. Archives of Neurology, 3, 109-126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broca, P. (1861). Perte de la parole. Ramollissement chronique et destruction partielle du lobe antérieur gauche du cerveau. Bulletin de la Société d'Anthropologie, 2, 235-237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broca, P. (1885). Sur le siège de la faculté du langage articulé. Bulletin de la Société d'Anthropologie, 6, 337-393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Canseco-Gonzalez, E. (1991). Contextual impenetrability of lexical access: An electrophysiological approach. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Brandeis University.

  • Caplan, D. (1988). On the role of group studies in neuropsychological and pathopsychological research. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5, 565-582.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caramazza, A. (1984). The logic of neuropsychological research and the problem of patient classification in aphasia. Brain and Language, 21, 9-20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caramazza, A. (1986). On drawing inferences about the structure of normal cognitive systems from the analysis of patterns of impaired performance: The case for single-patient studies. Brain and Cognition, 5, 41-66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charcot, J.-M. (1883). Des variétés de l'aphasie—De la cécité verbale. Progrès Médical, 11, 441-444.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Patterson, K., & Marshall, J. C. (Eds.) (1980). Deep dyslexia. London, England: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dejerine, J. (1892). Contribution a l'étude anatomo-pathologique et clinique des différentes varieties de cécité verbale. Memoires de la Société de Biologie, 4, 61-90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, A. (1987). Intimations of modularity, or, the modelarity of mind: Doing cognitive neuropsychology without syndromes. In M. Coltheart, G. Sartori, & R. Job (Eds.), The cognitive neuropsychology of language (pp 397-404). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finkelnburg, R. (1870). Vortrag in der niederrheinische Gesellschaft der Aerzte. Berliner Klinischer Wochenschrift, 37, 449-450; 38, 460–463.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freund, C. S. (1889). Uber optische Aphasie und Seelenblindheit. Archiv von Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 20, 276-297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M. S., Bogen J. E., & Sperry, R. W. (1965). Observations on visual perception after disconnexion of the cerebral hemispheres in man. Brain, 88, 221-236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geschwind, N. (1965). Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man. Brain, 88, 237-294; 88, 585–644.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, K. (1906). Zur Frage der amnestischen Aphasie. Archiv fur Psychiatrie und Neurologie, 41, 911-950.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodglass, H. (1993). Understanding aphasia. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodglass, H., & Budin, C. (1988). Category-and modality-specific dissociations in word-comprehension and concurrent phonological dyslexia: A case study. Neuropsychologia, 267, 67-78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodglass, H., Klein, B., Carey, P., & Jones, K. (1966). Specific semantic word categories in aphasia. Cortex, 2, 74-89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodglass, H., & Wingfield, A. (1993). Selective preservation of a lexical category in aphasia: Dissociations in comprehension of body parts and geographical place names following focal brain lesion. Memory, 1, 313-328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodglass, H., Wingfield, A., Hyde, M. R., & Theurkauf, J. C. (1986). Category specific dissociations in naming and recognition by aphasic patients. Cortex, 22, 87-102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, J., Berndt, R. S., & Caramazza, A. (1985). Category-specific naming deficit following cerebral infarction. Nature, 316, 439-440.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, J., & Gordon, B. (1992). Neural subsystems for object knowledge. Nature, 359, 60-64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Head, H. (1926). Aphasia and kindred disorders of speech. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, J. H. (1866). Notes on the physiology and pathology of language. Medical Times and Gazette, 1, 659.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, J. H. (1874). On the duality of the brain. Medical Press and Circular, 17, 19-22; 17, 41–46; 17, 63–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohn, S. E. (1989). Verb finding in aphasia. Cortex, 25, 57-69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kussmaul, A. (1877). Die Störungen der Sprache. Leipzig, Germany: Bogel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M., Schriefers, H., Vorberg, D., Meyer, A. S., Pechman, T., & Haringa, J. (1991). The time course of lexical access in speech production. Psychological Review, 98, 122-142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lichtheim, O. (1884). On aphasia. Brain, 7, 443-484.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liepmann, H., & Maas, O. (1907). Ein Fall von linksseitiger Agraphie und Apraxie bei rechtseitiger Lähmung. Monatsschrift für Psychiatrie und Neurologie, 10, 214-227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, J., & Newcombe, F. (1973). Patterns of paralexia: A psycholinguistic approach, Journal of Psycholinguisic Research, 2, 175-199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, A., Wiggs, C. L., Ungerleider, L. G., & Haxby, J. V. (1996). Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge. Nature, 379, 649-651.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehler, J., & Morton, J. (1984). On reducing language to biology. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 1, 83-116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, D., Schvaneveldt, R., & Ruddy, M. (1975). Loci of contextual effects on visual word recognition. In P. Rabbitt & S. Dornic (Eds.), Attention and performance V. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miceli, G., Silveri, M. C., Villa, G., & Caramazza, A. (1984). On the basis of agrammatics' difficulty in producing main verbs. Cortex, 20, 207-220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milberg, W., & Blumstein, S. E. (1981). Lexical decision and aphasia: Evidence for semantic processing. Brain and Language, 14, 371-385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Naeser, M. A. (1982). Language behavior in stroke patients: Cortical vs. subcortical lesion sites on CT scans. Trends in Neurosciences, 5, 53-59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neeley, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 226-254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, J. M. (1946). Agnosia, apraxia, and aphasia: Their value in cerebral localization. New York: Hoeber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paterson, A., & Zangwill, O. L. (1944). Disorders of visual space perception associated with lesions of the right cerebral hemisphere. Brain, 67, 331-358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pitres, A. (1898). L'aphasie amnésique et ses variétés cliniques. Progrès Médical, 28, 17-23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prather, P., Zurif, E. B., Stern, C., & Rosen, T. J. (1992). Slowed lexical access in Broca's aphasia. Brain and Language, 43, 336-348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raichle, M. E. (1994). Images of the mind: Studies with modern imaging techniques. Annual Review of Psychology, 45, 333-356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raichle, M. E. (1996). Interview. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 189-195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., & Sereno, S. C. (1994). Eye movements in reading. Psycholinguistic studies. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 57-81). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sartori, G., Job. R., Miozzo, M., Zago, S., & Marchiori, G. (1993). Category-specific form-knowledge deficit in a patient with herpes simplex encephalitis. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 280-299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M. F. (1984). What the classical aphasia categories can't do for us and why. Brain and Language, 21, 3-8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semenza, C., & Goodglass, H. (1985). Identification of body parts in brain-injured subjects. Neuropsychologia, 23, 161-176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semenza, C., & Zettin, M. (1989). Evidence from aphasia for the role of proper names as pure referring expressions. Nature, 342, 678-679.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swinney, D. (1979). Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re)consideration of contextual effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 645-659.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tissot, R., Mounin, G., & Lhermitte, F. (1973). L'agrammatisme. Brussels, Belgium: Dessart.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, & R. J. W. Mansfield (Eds.), The analysis of visual behavior (pp. 549-586). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wapner, W., & Gardner, H. (1979). A note on patterns of comprehension and recovery in global aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 29, 765-771.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K., & McCarthy, E. (1983). Category-specific access dysphasia. Brain, 106, 859-878.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K., & McCarthy, E. (1987). Categories of knowledge: Further fractionations and an attempted integration. Brain, 110, 1273-1296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1980). Word form aphasia. Brain, 103, 99-112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category-specific semantic impairment. Brain, 107, 829-853.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wernicke, C. (1874). Der aphasische Symptomenkomplex. Breslau, Germany: Cohn und Weigert.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamadori, A., & Albert, M. L. (1973). Word category aphasia. Cortex, 9, 112-125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zaidel, E., & Peters, A. (1981). Phonological encoding and idiographic reading by the disconnected right hemisphere: Two case studies. Brain and Language, 14, 205-234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zurif, E., Gardner, H., & Brownell, H. (1989). The case against the case against group studies. Brain and Cognition, 10, 237-255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zurif, E. B., Swinney, D., Prather, P., Solomon, J., & Bushell, C. (1993). An on-line analysis of syntactic processing in Broca's and Wernicke's aphasia. Brain and Language, 45, 448-464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zurif, E. B., Swinney, D., Prather, P., Wingfield, A., & Brownell, H. (1995). The allocation of memory resources during sentence comprehension: Evidence from the elderly. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 24, 165-182.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Goodglass, H., Wingfield, A. The Changing Relationship Between Anatomic and Cognitive Explanation in the Neuropsychology of Language. J Psycholinguist Res 27, 147–165 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023293814792

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023293814792

Keywords

Navigation