Skip to main content
Log in

Is Human Behavior Autocorrelated? An Empirical Analysis

  • Published:
Journal of Behavioral Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The current review and analysis investigated the presence of serial dependency (or autocorrelation) in single-subject applied behavior-analytic research. While well researched, few studies have controlled for the number of data points that appeared in the time-series and, thus, the negative bias of the r coefficient, and the power to detect true serial dependency effects. Therefore, all baseline graphs that appeared in the Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis (JABA) between 1968 and 1993 that provided more than 30 data points were examined for the presence of serial dependency (N = 103). Results indicated that 12% of the baseline graphs provided a significant lag-1 autocorrelation, and over 83% of them had coefficient values less than or equal to (±.25). The distribution of the lag-1 autocorrelation coefficients had a mean of .10. Subsequent distributions of partial autocorrelations at lags two through seven had smaller means indicating that as the distance between observations increases (i.e., the lag), serial dependency decreased. Although serial dependency did not appear to be a common property of the single-subject behavioral experiments, it is recommended that, whenever statistical analyses are contemplated, its presence should always be examined. Alternatives for coping with the presence of significant levels of serial dependency were discussed in terms of: (a) using alternative statistical procedures (e.g., ARIMA models, randomization tests, the Shewhart quality-control charts); (b) correcting statistics of traditional parametric procedures (e.g., t, F); or (c) using the autocorrelation coefficient as an indicator and estimate of reliable intervention effects.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Baer, D. M. (1977). Perhaps it would be better not to know everything. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 167–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Busk, P. L., & Marascuilo, L. A. (1988). Autocorrelation in single-subject research: A counterargument to the myth of no autocorrelation. Behavioral Assessment, 10, 229–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeProspero, A., & Cohen, S. (1979). Inconsistent visual analyses of intrasubject data. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 12, 573–579.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edgington, E. S. (1980a). Random assignment and statistical tests for one-subject experiments. Behavioral Assessment, 2, 19–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edgington, E. S. (1992). Nonparametric tests for single-case experiments. In T. R. Kratochwill, & J. R. Levin (Eds.), Single-case research design and analysis: New directions for psychology and education (pp. 41–64). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Furlong, M. J., & Wampold, B. E. (1982). Intervention effects and relative variation as dimensions in experts' use of visual inference. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 15, 415–421.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottman, J. M. (1981). Time-series analysis: A comprehensive introduction for social scientists. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, D. P., Gottman, J. M., Jones, R. R., Gardner, W., Kazdin, A. E., & Vaught, R. S. (1980). Interrupted time-series analysis and its application to behavioral data. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 13, 543–559.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hibbs, D. A. (1974). Problems of statistical estimation and causal inference in time-series regression models. In H. L. Costner (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 252–308). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huitema, B. E. (1985). Autocorrelation in applied behavior analysis: A myth. Behavioral Assessment, 7, 107–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huitema, B. E. (1986). Autocorrelation in behavioral research: Wherefore art thou? In A. Poling, & R. W. Fuqua, (Eds.), Research methods in applied behavior analysis (pp.187–208). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huitema, B. E. (1988). Autocorrelation: 10 years of confusion. Behavioral Assessment, 10, 253–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huitema, B. E., & McKean, J. W. (1994). Reduced bias autocorrelation estimation: Three jackknife methods. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 54, 654–665.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, R. R., Vaught, R. S., & Weinrott, M. (1977). Time-series analysis in operant research. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 151–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, R. R., Weinrott, M. R., & Vaught, R. S. (1978). Effects of serial dependency on the agreement between visual and statistical inference. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11, 277–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Judd, C. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1981). Estimating the effects of social interventions. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kazdin, A. E. (1980). Obstacles in using randomization tests in single-case experimentation. Journal of Educational Statistics, 5, 253–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kazdin, A. E. (1982). Single-case research designs. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kazdin, A. E. (1984). Statistical analyses for single-case experimental designs. In D. H. Barlow & M. Hersen, Single case experimental designs: Strategies for studying behavior change (pp. 285–324). New York: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenny, D. A., & Judd, C. M. (1986). Consequences of violating the independence assumption in analysis of variance. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 422–431.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kratochwill, T. R., & Brody, G. H. (1978). A perspective on the controversy over employing statistical inference and implications for research and training in behavior modification. Behavior Modification, 2, 291–307.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ljung, G. M., & Box, G. E. P. (1978). On a measure of lack of fit in time series models. Biometrika, 65, 297–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matyas, T. A., & Greenwood, K. M. (1991). Problems in the estimation of autocorrelation in brief time series and some implications for behavioral data. Behavioral Assessment, 13, 137–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. (1974). Statistical inference for individual organism research: Mixed blessing or curse? Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7, 647–653.

    Google Scholar 

  • Minium, E. W. (1978). Statistical reasoning in psychology and education. New York: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ottenbacher, K. J. (1990). When is a picture worth a thousand p values? A comparison of visual and quantitative methods to analyze single-subject data. Journal of Special Education, 23, 436–449.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parsonson, B. S., & Baer, D. M. (1986). The graphic analysis of data. In A. Poling & R. W. Fuqua (Eds.), Research methods in applied behavior analysis: Issues and advances (pp. 157–186). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavur, R., & Nath, R. (1984). Exact F tests in an ANOVA procedure for dependent observations. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 19, 408–420.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pfadt, A., Cohen, I. L., Sudhalter, V., Romanczyk, R. G., & Wheeler, D. J. (1992). Applying statistical process control to clinical data: An illustration. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 551–560.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, J. P. N. (1983). Serially correlated errors in some single-subject designs. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 36, 269–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowley, G. L. (1989). Assessing error in behavioral data: Problems of sequencing. Journal of Educational Statistics, 26, 273–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rushe, R. H., & Gottman, J. M. (1993). Essentials in the design and analysis of time-series experiments. In G. Keren, & C. Lewis (Eds.), A handbook for data analysis in the behavioral sciences: Statistical issues (pp. 493–528). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheffe, H. (1959). The analysis of variance. New York: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharpley, C. (1986). Fallibility in the visual assessment of behavioural interventions: Time-series statistics to analyse time-series data. Behaviour Change, 3, 26–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharpley, C. F., & Alavosius, M. P. (1988). Autocorrelation in behavioral data: An alternative perspective. Behavioral Assessment, 10, 243–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sideridis, G. D., & Greenwood, C. R., (1996). Evaluating treatment effects in single-subject behavioral experiments using quality control charts. Journal of Behavioral Education, 6, 203–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suen, H. K., & Ary, D. (1989). Analyzing quantitative behavioral observation data. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suen, H. K., & Ary, D. (1987). Autocorrelation in applied behavior analysis: Myth or reality? Behavioral Assessment, 9, 125–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wampold, B. E., & Furlong, M. J. (1981). The heuristics of visual inference. Behavioral Assessment, 3, 79–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wampold, B. E., & Worsham, N. L. (1986). Randomization tests for multiple-baseline designs. Behavioral Assessment, 8, 135–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, D. J., & Chambers, D. S. (1992). Understanding statistical process control. Knoxville, TN: SPC Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Sideridis, G.D., Greenwood, C.R. Is Human Behavior Autocorrelated? An Empirical Analysis. Journal of Behavioral Education 7, 273–293 (1997). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022895805201

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022895805201

Navigation