Skip to main content
Log in

The Educational Relevance of Research in Cognitive Neuroscience

  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The benefits of incorporating findings from cognitive neuroscience into the field of educational psychology are considered. The first section begins with arguments against the idea that one can ignore the brain when positing a model of student learning or motivation. The second section describes limitations in the methods used to reveal brain-cognition relations. In the third section, properties of the brain and brain development are described. The fourth section summarizes the cognitive neuroscientific research on attention, memory, reading, and math. Finally, areas of future research in cognitive neuroscience are suggested that would help answer important questions about individual and developmental differences in student learning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to Read. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1995). Learning and Memory: An Integrated Approach, Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R., and Schooler, L. J. (1991). Reflections on the environment in memory. Psychol. Sci., 2: 396–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arlin, P. K. (1981). Piagetian tasks as predictors of reading and math readiness in grades K-1. J. Educ. Psychol. 73: 712–721.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R. C., and Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In Spence, K. W., and Spence, J. T. (eds.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory (Vol. 2), Academic, New York, pp. 89–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Badian, N. A. (1983). Dyscalculia and nonverbal disorders of learning. In Myklebust, H. R. (ed.), Progress in Learning Disabilities (Vol. 5), Stratton, New York, pp. 235–264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., and Abbott, R. D. (1992). The unit of analysis and the constructive processes of the learner: Key concepts for educational neuropsychology. Educ. Psychol. 27: 223–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. W. (1992). How might emotions affect learning? In Christianson, S. A. (ed.), The Handbook of Emotion and Memory: Research and Theory, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 3–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B., and Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1995). Evaluation of a program to teach phonemic awareness to young children: 2-and 3-year follow-up and a new preschool trial. J. Educ. Psychol. 87: 488–503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrnes, J. P. (1992). The conceptual basis of procedural learning. Cognit. Devel. 7: 235–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrnes, J. P. (1995). Domain specificity and the logic of using general ability as an independent variable or covariate. Merrill-Palmer Quart. 41: 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrnes, J. P. (1996). Cognitive Development and Learning in Instructional Contexts, Allyn & Bacon, Needham Hts., MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrnes, J. P., and Guthrie, J. T. (1992). Prior conceptual knowledge and textbook search. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 17: 8–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrnes, J. P., and Takahira, S. (1994). Why students perform well and others perform poorly on SAT-math items. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 19: 63–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caramazza, A. (1992). Is cognitive neuropsychology possible? J. Cognit. Neurosci. 4: 80–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1992). The role of the frontal lobes in the regulation of cognitive development. Brain Cognit. 20: 51–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casey, B. J., Cohen, J. D., Noll, D. C., Schneider, W., Giedd, J. N., and Rappaport, J. L. (1996). Functional magnetic resonance imaging: Studies of cognition. In Bigler, E. D. (ed.), Neuroimaging II: Clinical Applications, Plenum, New York, pp. 299–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casey, M. B., Nuttall, R., and Benbow, C. P. (1995). The influence of spatial ability on gender differences in mathematics college entrance test scores across diverse samples. Devel. Psychol. 31: 697–705.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ceci, S. J. (1990). On Intelligence...More or Less: A Bio-ecological Treatise on Intellectual Development, Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cherry, S. R., and Phelps, M. E. (1996). Imaging brain function with positron emission tomography. In Toga, A. W., and Mazziotta, J. C. (eds.), Brain Mapping: The Methods, Academic Press, Orlando, FL, pp. 191–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, M. S. (1996). Rapid MRI and functional applications. In Toga, A. W., and Mazziotta, J. C. (eds.), Brain Mapping: The Methods, Academic Press, Orlando, FL, pp. 191–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J., and Ekman, P. (1995). The Nature of Emotion, Oxford University Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dempster, F. N. (1992). The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: Toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging. Devel. Rev. 12: 45–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (1993). Neuropsychological insights into the meaning of object concept development. In Johnson, M. H. (ed.), Brain Development and Cognition: A Reader, Blackwell, Oxford, pp. 208–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J., Burgess, P., and Emslie, H. (1995). Fluid intelligence after frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia 33: 261–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M. (1992). Bright Air, Brilliant Fire: On the Matter of Mind, Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engel, S. A., Rumelhart, D. E., Wandell, B. A., Lee, A. T., Glover, G. H., Chichilnisky, E. J., and Shaden, M. N. (1994). fMRI of human visual cortex. Nature 369: 525–526.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1975). The Language of Thought, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, N. A., and Bell, M. A. (1990). Electrophysiological indices of frontal lobe development: Relations to cognitive and affective behavior in human infants over the first year of life. In Diamond, A. (ed.), The Development and Neural Bases of Higher Cortical Functions New York Academy of Sciences, New York, pp. 677–704.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frearson, W. M., and Eysenck, H. J. (1986). Intelligence, reaction time (RT), and a new “odd-man-out” RT paradigm. Person. Indiv. Diff. 7: 807–817.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaddes, W. H. (1983). Applied educational neuropsychology: Theories and problems. J. Learn. Disab. 16: 511–515.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of Mind, Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garrett, M. F. (1990). Sentence processing. In Osherson, D. N., and Lasnik, H. (eds.), An Invitation to Cognitive Science (Vol. 1): Language, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 133–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. (1993). Mathematical disabilities: Cognitive, neuropsychological, and genetic components. Psychol. Bull. 114: 345–362.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gevins, A. (1996). Electrophysiological imaging of brain function. In Toga, A. W., and Mazziotta, J. C. (eds.), Brain Mapping: The Methods, Academic Press, Orlando, FL, pp. 259–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1986). Setting the stage: Neural development before birth. In Peterson, S. L., Klivington, K. A., and Peterson, R. W. (eds.), The Brain, Cognition, and Education, Academic, New York, pp. 233–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1994). Specification of higher cortical functions. In Broman, S. H., and Grafman, J. (eds.), Atypical Cognitive Deficits in Developmental Disorders: Implications for Brain Function, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenough, W. T., Black, J. E., and Wallace, C. S. (1987). Experience and brain development. Child Devel. 58: 539–559.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halpern, D. F. (1992). Sex Differences in Cognitive Abilities (2nd Ed.), Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harnishfeger, K. K., and Bjorklund, D. F. (1994). A developmental perspective on individual differences in inhibition. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 3: 331–355.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1949). The Organization of Behavior, Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hellige, J. B. (1990). Hemispheric asymmetry. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 41: 55–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hiebert, J. (1987). Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge: The Case of Mathematics, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinshaw, S. P. (1994). Attention Deficits and Hyperactivity in Children, Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoebel, B. G. (1988). Neuroscience and motivation: Pathways and peptides that define motivational systems. In Atkinson, R. C., Herrnstein, R. J., Lindzey, G., and Luce, R. D. (eds.), Steven's Handbook of Experimental Psychology (Vol. 1): Perception and Motivation, Wiley, New York, pp. 547–620.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hubel, D. H. and Wiesel, T. N. (1962). Receptive fields, binocular interaction, and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. J. Physiol. 160: 106–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher, P. R. (1993). Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development. In Johnson, M. H. (ed.), Brain Development and Cognition: A Reader, Blackwell, Oxford, pp. 112–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hynd, G. W., Marshall, R., and Gonzales, J. (1991). Learning disabilities and presumed central nervous system dysfunction. Learn. Disab. Quart. 14: 283–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iran-Nejad, A., Hidi, S., and Wittrock, M. C. (1992). Reconceptualizing relevance in education from a biological perspective. Educ. Psychol. 27: 407–414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. J., Craik, F. I. M., and Begg, I. (1979). Effects of decision difficulty on recognition and recall. J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav. 18: 585–600.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, W. M., Merzenich, M. M., and Recanzone, G. (1990). Neocortical representational dynamics in adult primates: Implications for neuropsychology. Neuropsychologia 28: 573–584.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A. R. (1987). The g factor beyond factor analysis. In Ronning, R. R., Glover, J. A., Conoley, J. C., and Witt, J. C. (eds.), The Influence of Cognitive Psychology on Testing, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. H. (1993). Constraints on cortical plasticity. In Johnson, M. H. (ed.), Brain Development and Cognition: A Reader, Blackwell, Oxford, pp. 703–721.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R. V. (1991). Processing time declines exponentially during childhood and adolescence. Devel. Psychol. 27: 259–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. (1989). Mechanisms and development of hemisphere specialization in children. In Reynolds, C. R., and Fletcher-Janzen, E. (eds.), Handbook of Clinical Child Neuropsychology, Plenum, New York, pp. 69–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kolb, B. (1993). Brain development, plasticity, and behavior. In Johnson, M. H. (ed.), Brain Development and Cognition: A Reader, Blackwell, Oxford, pp. 338–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M., and Intriligator, J. M. (1992). Is cognitive neuropsychology plausible? The perils of sitting on a one-legged stool. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 4: 96–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M., and Koenig, O. (1992). Wet Mind: The New Cognitive Neuroscience, The Free Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., and Van Petten, C. (1988). Event-related brain potential studies of language. In Ackles, P., Jennings, J. R., and Coles, M. (eds.), Advances in Psychophysiology, JAI Press, Greenwich, CT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K. S. (1929). Brain Mechanisms and Intelligence, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1973). The Working Brain, Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information, Freeman, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, A., Haxby, J. V., Lalonde, F. M., Wiggs, C. L., and Ungerleider, L. G. (1995). Discrete cortical regions associated with knowledge of color and knowledge of action. Science 270: 102–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, A., Wiggs, C. L., Ungerleider, L. G., and Haxby, J. V. (1996). Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge. Nature 379: 649–652.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E., and Sims, V. K. (1994). For whom is a picture worth a thousand words? Extensions of a dual-coding theory of multimedia learning. J. Educ. Psychol. 86: 389–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, R. A., and Warrington, E. K. (1990). Cognitive Neuropsychology: A Clinical Introduction, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey, M., and Caramazza, A. (1987). Cognitive mechanisms in normal and impaired number processing. In Deloche, G., and Seron, X. (eds.), Mathematical Disabilities, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 201–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKinney, M. L., and McNamara, K. J. (1991). Heterochrony: The Evolution of Ontogeny, Plenum Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moonen, C. T. W. (1995). Imaging of human brain activation with functional MRI. Biol. Psychiat. 37: 141–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive Psychology, Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. and Simon, H.A. (1972). Human Problem Solving, Erlbaum, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Boyle, M. W., Benbow, C. P., and Alexander, J. E. (1995). Sex-differences, hemispheric laterality, and associated brain activity in the intellectually gifted. Devel. Neuropsychol. 11: 415–443.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Leary, D. M. (1993). Do cortical areas emerge from a protocortex? In Johnson, M. H. (ed.), Brain Development and Cognition: A Reader, Blackwell, Oxford, pp. 323–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading Ability, Oxford University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., and McCutchen, D. (1987). Schooled language competence: Linguistic abilities in reading and writing. In Rosenberg, S. (ed.), Advances in Applied Psycholinguistics, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 105–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, S. E., Fox, P. T., Snyder, A. Z., and Raichle, M. E. (1990). Activation of extrastriate and frontal cortical areas by visual words and word-like stimuii. Science 240: 1041–1044.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1993). The epigenetic system and the development of cognitive functions. In Johnson, M. H. (ed.), Brain Development and Cognition: A Reader, Blackwell, Oxford, pp. 31–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1989). Foundations of Cognitive Science, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1997). Cognitive Neuroscience and the Development of Attention. Invited Address at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Washington, D.C., April.

  • Posner, M. I., Inhoff, A. W., Friedrich, F. J., and Cohen, A. (1987). Isolating attentional systems: A cognitive-anatomical analysis. Psychobiology 15: 107–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., Peterson, S. E., Fox, P. T., and Raichle, M. E. (1988). Localization of cognitive operations in the human brain. Science 240: 1627–1631.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M., and McCormick, C. B. (1995). Advanced Educational Psychology for Educators, Researchers, and Policymakers, HarperCollins, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M., Levin, J., and Delaney, H. D. (1982). The mnemonic keyword method. Rev. Educ. Res. 52: 61–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1984). Computation and Cognition, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rakic, P. (1993). Intrinsic and extrinsic determinants of neocortical parcellation: A radial unit model. In Johnson, M. H. (ed.), Brain Maturation and Cognition: A Reader, Blackwell, Oxford, pp. 93–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raynor, K., and Pollatsek, A. (1989). The Psychology of Reading, Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Recanzone, G. H., Schreiner, C. E., and Merzenich, M. M. (1993). Plasticity in the frequency representation of primary auditory cortex following discrimination training in adult owl monkeys. J. Neurosci. 13: 87–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reschly, D. J., and Gresham, F. M. (1989). Current neuropsychological diagnosis of learning problems: A leap of faith. In Reynolds, C. R., and Fletcher-Janzen, E. (eds.), Handbook of Clinical Child Neuropsychology, Plenum, New York, pp. 503–519.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, T. E., and Berridge, K. C. (1993). The neural basis of drug craving: An incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res. Rev. 18: 247–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In Rosch, E., and Lloyd, B. B. (eds.), Cognition and Categorization, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 27–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E. (1989). The architecture of mind: A connectionist approach. In Posner, M. I. (ed.), Foundations of Cognitive Science, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 133–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rushton, J. P. (1995). Race, Evolution, and Behavior, Transaction, New Brunswick, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schachter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn., Mem., and Cognit., 13: 501–518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segal, N. L. (1989). Origins and implications of handedness and relative birth weight for IQ in monozygotic twin pairs. Neuropsychologia 27: 549–561.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sejnowski, T. J., and Churchland, P. S. (1989). Brain and cognition. In Posner, M. I. (ed.), Foundations of Cognitive Science, pp. 301–358.

  • Shallice, T. (1991). Precis of from neuropsychology to mental structure. Behav. Brain Sci. 14: 429–469.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, S. E. (1996). Dyslexia. Sci. Am. 94: 98–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. R. (1987). Memory and Brain, Oxford University Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., and Siegel, L. S. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. J. Educ. Psychol. 86: 24–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steinmetz, H., Herzog, A., Schlaug, G., Huang, Y., and Lanke, R. (1995). Brain (a)symmetry in monozygotic twins. Cereb. Cortex 5: 296–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1989). Domain-specificity versus domain-generality: The life and impending death of a false dichotomy. Merrill-Palmer Quart. 35: 115–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (1991). Concepts of human memory. In Squire, L. R., Weinberger, N. M., Lynch, G., and McGaugh, J. L. (eds.), Memory: Organization and Locus of Change, Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 3–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. Kapur, S., Markowitsch, H. J., Craik, F. I. M., Habib, R., and Houle, S. (1994). Neuroanatomical correlates of retrieval in episodic memory: Auditory sentence recognition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 91: 2012–2015.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W. E., Herriman, M. L., and Nesdale, A. R. (1988). Metalinguistic abilities and beginning reading. Read. Res. Q. 23: 134–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., and Tanzman, M. S. (1991). Bridging the gap between cognitive and neuropsychological conceptualizations of reading disability. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 3: 191–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, P. A. (1991). Studying intelligence the hard way. Intelligence 15: 389–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind and Society, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychol. Rev. 92: 548–573.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., and Gelman, S. A. (1992). Cognitive development: Foundational theories and core domains. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 43: 337–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wickett, J. C., and Vernon, P. A. (1994). Peripheral nerve conduction velocity, reaction time, and intelligence: An attempt to replicate Vernon and Mori (1992). Intelligence 18: 127–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittrock, M. C. (1986). Education and recent research on attention and knowledge acquisition. In Friedman, S. L., Klivington K. A., and Peterson, R. W. (eds.), The Brain, Cognition, and Education, Academic Press, New York, pp. 151–170.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Byrnes, J.P., Fox, N.A. The Educational Relevance of Research in Cognitive Neuroscience. Educational Psychology Review 10, 297–342 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022145812276

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022145812276

Navigation