Skip to main content
Log in

The effects of polysemy for Japanese katakana words

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In these experiments, the effects of polysemy were examined as a function of word frequency for Japanese katakana words, words which have consistent character-to-sound correspondences. In the lexical decision task, an additive relationship was observed between polysemy and frequency (i.e., polysemy effects were identical for high and low frequency katakana words). In the naming task, although no word frequency effect was observed, there was a significant polysemy effect which, as in the lexical decision task, was identical for high and low frequency words. The implications of these results for conclusions about the loci of polysemy and frequency effects in lexical decision and naming tasks are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Balota, D.A. & Chumbley, J.I. (1984). Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? The role of word frequency in the neglected decision stage, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 10: 340–357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D.A. & Chumbley, J.I. (1985). The locus of word-frequency effects in the pronunciation task: Lexical access and/or production? Journal of Memory and Language 24: 89–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D.A., Ferraro, R.F. & Connor, L.T. (1991). On the early influence of meaning in word recognition: A review of the literature. In: P. J. Schwanenflugel (ed.), The psychology of word meanings (pp. 187–221). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D.A. & Paul, S.T. (1996). Summation of activation: Evidence from multiple primes that converage and diverge within semantic memory, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 22: 827–845.

    Google Scholar 

  • Besner, D. (1983). Basic decoding components in reading: Two dissociable feature extraction processes, Canadian Journal of Psychology 37: 429–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Besner, D. (in press). Basic processes in reading: Multiple routes in localist and connectionist models. In: R.M. Klein & P.A. McMullen (eds.), Converging methods for understanding reading and dyslexia. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (in press).

  • Besner, D. & Hildebrandt, N. (1987). Orthographic and phonological codes in the oral reading of Japanese kana, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 13: 335–343.

    Google Scholar 

  • Besner, D. & McCann, R.S. (1987). Word frequency and pattern distortion in visual word identification and production: An examination of four classes of models. In: M. Coltheart (ed.), Attention and performance, Vol. 12 (pp. 201–219). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Besner, D., Twilley, L., McCann, R.S. & Seergobin, K. (1990). On the association between connectionism and data: Are a few words necessary? Psychological Review, 97: 432–446.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borowsky, R. & Masson, M.E.J. (1996). Semantic ambiguity effects in word identification, Journal of Experiment Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 22: 63–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, P., Lupker, S.J. & Colombo, L. (1994). Interacting sources of information in word naming: A study of individual differences, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 20: 537–554.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carr, T.H., Posner, M.I., Pollatsek, A. & Snyder, C.R.R. (1979). Orthography and familiarity effects in word processing, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 108: 389–414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chumbley, J.I. & Balota, D.A. (1984). A word's meaning affects the decision in lexical decision, Memory & Cognition 12: 590–606.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H.H. (1973). The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 12: 335–359.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (ed.), Strategies of information processing (pp. 151–216). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P. & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches, Psychological Review 100: 589–608.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Davelaar, E., Jonasson, J.T. & Besner, D. (1977). Access to the internal lexicon. In: S. Dornic (ed.), Attention and performance, Vol. 6 (pp. 535–555). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fera, P., Joordens, S., Balota, D.A., Ferraro, F.R. & Besner, D. (1992). Ambiguity in meaning and phonology: Effects on naming. Paper presented at the 33rd annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, St. Louis, MO, November 1992.

  • Forster, K.I. & Bednall, E.S. (1976). Terminating and exhaustive search in lexical access, Memory & Cognition 4: 53–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K.I. & Chambers, S.M. (1973). Lexical access and naming time, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 12: 627–635.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R., Katz, L. & Bentin, S. (1987). Strategies for visual word recognition and orthographical depth: A multilingual comparison, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 13: 104–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M.A. (1984). Resolving 20 years of inconsistent interactions between lexical familiarity and orthography, concreteness, and polysemy, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 113: 256–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grainger, J. & Jacobs, A.M. (1996). Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: A multiple read-out model, Psychological Review 103: 518–565.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hino, Y. & Lupker, S.J. (1996). The effects of polysemy in lexical decision and naming: An alternative to lexical access accounts, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 22: 1321–1335.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hino, Y. & Lupker, S.J. (in press). The effects of word frequency for Japanese kana and kanji words in naming and lexical decision: Can the dual-route model save the lexical-selection account? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.

  • James, C.T. (1975). The role of semantic information in lexical decisions, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1: 130–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jastrzembski, J.E. (1981). Multiple meanings, number of related meanings, frequency of occurrence, and the lexicon, Cognitive Psychology 13: 278–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jastrzembski, J.E. & Stanners, R.F. (1975). Multiple word meanings and lexical search speed, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 14: 534–537.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kellas, G., Ferraro, F.R. & Simpson, G.B. (1988). Lexical ambiguity and the timecourse of attentional allocation in word recognition, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 14: 601–609.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kucera, H. & Francis, W.N. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J.L. & Rumelhart, D.E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings, Psychological Review 88: 375–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Millis, M.L. & Buttons, S.B. (1989). The effect of polysemy on lexical decision time: Now you see it, now you don't, Memory & Cognition 17: 141–147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monsell, S. (1991). The nature and locus of word frequency effects in reading. In: D. Besner & G. W. Humphreys (eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 148–197). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monsell, S., Doyle, M.C. & Haggard, P.N. (1989). Effects of frequency on visual word recognition tasks: Where are they? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 118: 43–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J. (1969). Interaction of information in word recognition, Psychological Review 76: 165–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J. & Sasanuma, S. (1984). Lexical access in Japanese. In: L. Henderson (ed.), Orthographies and reading: Perspectives from cognitive psychology, neuropsychology and linguistics (pp. 25–42). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Language Research Institute (1970). Studies on the vocabulary of modern newspapers, Vol. 1: General descriptions and vocabulary frequency tables. Tokyo: Shuei Shuppan (in Japanese).

    Google Scholar 

  • National Language Research Institute (1971). Studies on the vocabulary of modern newspapers, Vol. 2: Vocabulary tables classified from the lexicological and grammatical point of view. Tokyo: Shuei Shuppan (in Japanese).

    Google Scholar 

  • National Language Research Institute (1993). Bunrui Goi Hyou [Floppy Disk Version]. Tokyo: Shuei Shuppan (in Japanese).

    Google Scholar 

  • Paap, K.R., McDonald, J.E., Schvaneveldt, R.W. & Noel, R.W. (1987). Frequency and pronounceability in visually presented naming and lexical decision tasks. In: M. Coltheart (ed.), Attention and performance Vol. 12 (pp. 221–243). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paap, K.R. & Noel, R.W. (1991). Dual-route models of print to sound: Still a good horse race, Psychological Research 53: 13–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plaut, D.C. & McClelland, J.L. (1993). Generalization with componential attractors: Word and nonword reading in an attractor network. Proceedings of the 15th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 824–829). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plaut, D.C., McClelland, J.L., Seidenberg, M.S. & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains, Psychological Review 103: 56–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M.I. & Carr, T.H. (1992). Lexical access and the brain: Anatomical constraints on cognitive models of word recognition, American Journal of Psychology 105: 1–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubenstein, H., Garfield, L. & Millikan, J.A. (1970). Homographic entries in the internal lexicon, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9: 487–494.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubenstein, H., Lewis, S.S. & Rubenstein, M.A. (1971). Homographic entries in the internal lexicon: Effects of systematicity and relative frequency of meanings, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 10: 57–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D.E. & McClelland, J.L. (1982). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model, Psychological Review 89: 60–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M.S. (1992). Beyond orthographic depth in reading: Equitable division of labor. In: R. Frost & L. Katz (eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (pp. 85–118). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M.S. & McClelland, J.L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming, Psychological Review 96: 521–568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method, Acta Psychologica 30: 276–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strain, E., Patterson, K. & Seidenberg, M.S. (1995). Semantic effects in single-word naming, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21: 1140–1154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Theios, J. & Muise, J.G. (1977). The word identification process in reading. In: N.J. Castellan, D.B. Pisoni & G.R. Potts (eds.), Cognitive theory, Vol. 2 (pp. 289–321). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden, G. C., Pennington, B.F. & Stone, G.O. (1990). Word identification in reading and the promise of subsymbolic psycholinguistics, Psychological Review 97: 488–522.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wydell, T.N., Butterworth, B. & Patterson, K. (1995). The inconsistency of consistency effects in reading: The case of Japanese kanji, Journal of Experiment Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21: 1155–1168.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hino, Y., Lupker, S.J., Sears, C.R. et al. The effects of polysemy for Japanese katakana words. Reading and Writing 10, 395–424 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008060924384

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008060924384

Navigation