Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-qsmjn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T00:19:45.091Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Relationship Between MAOA Gene Polymorphism and Test Anxiety

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 November 2013

Yangyang Liu*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Nanjing University, Nanjing, China
Zuhong Lu
Affiliation:
Researh center for learning science, Southeast University, Nanjing, China
*
address for correspondence: Yanghan Liu, Department of Psychology, Nanjing University, Han kou lu 22, Nanjing, China. E-mail: liuyangyang661@sina.com

Abstract

In a sample of 569 Chinese high school students, the present findings indicated that students with the 4-repeat genotype showed a higher level of test anxiety. Furthermore, the prediction of academic performance on test anxiety was stronger among students with the 3-repeat genotype than those with the 4-repeat genotype. The present findings suggest that mono-amine-oxidase type A gene polymorphism is significantly related to test anxiety.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2013 

During adolescence, many students suffer from anxiety disorders (Neil & Christensen, Reference Neil and Christensen2009). In previous research, researchers have proposed that too little serotonin (5-HT) in the brain leads to anxiety (Dayan & Huys, Reference Dayan and Huys2008). The mono-amine-oxidase type A (MAOA) gene encodes the MAOA enzyme that can render serotonin inactive by metabolizing it (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, McClay, Moffitt, Mill, Martin, Craig and Poulton2002). Previous research has identified a functional polymorphism of the MAOA gene (2-repeats, 3-repeats, 3.5-repeats, 4-repeats, and 5-repeats) according to a 30-bp insertion or deletion (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, McClay, Moffitt, Mill, Martin, Craig and Poulton2002). Researchers have found that the 4-repeat allele is associated with higher MAOA activity than the 3-repeat allele (Jonsson et al., Reference Jonsson, Norton, Gustavsson, Oreland, Owen and Sedvall2000). In empirical research, extremely limited evidence indicated that the MAOA gene polymorphism was related to generalized anxiety disorders (Tadic et al., Reference Tadic, Rujescu, Szegedi, Giegling, Singer, Möller and Dahmen2003). However, until now we have known little about how the MAOA gene polymorphism contributes to test anxiety. Therefore, the first objective of the present study was to explore the relationship between the MAOA gene polymorphism and test anxiety. It was expected that the 4-repeat allele would be related to a higher level of test anxiety.

Moreover, several studies have linked students’ academic performance to their trait test anxiety (Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, Reference Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham2003; Pintrich & DeGroot, Reference Pintrich and DeGroot1990). In these studies, trait test anxiety was found to be weakly or moderately related to academic performance (Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, Reference Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham2003; Pintrich & DeGroot, Reference Pintrich and DeGroot1990), which suggests that some variables may moderate the relationship between academic performance and test anxiety. In the present study, the second objective was to examine the moderating effects of the MAOA gene polymorphism on the relationship between academic performance and test anxiety. It was expected that the MAOA gene polymorphism would moderate the relationship between academic performance and test anxiety. In previous research, some studies have suggested that academic-related emotions should be measured in domain-specific ways (Goetz et al., Reference Goetz, Frenzel, Pekrun and Hall2006). Hence, the present study focused on students’ trait test anxiety in the subject of mathematics. We chose mathematics because most students said that mathematics was the most anxiety-inducing subject in their curriculum (Macher et al., Reference Macher, Paechter, Papousek and Ruggeri2012).

Methods

Participants and Procedure

Participants were recruited from six Chinese high schools located in an urban city (Nanjing). There were 569 Grade 10 students (269 girls) who provided their agreement to participate in the present study. Their mean age was 16.7 years (SD = 0.6, range 15.8–17.6). Students’ test anxiety was measured at the beginning of both Grade 10 and Grade 12 during classroom time. The interval between tests was nearly 25 months. We also collected students’ mathematical scores at Grade 10 from school records. Ethical approval was granted from our institution and informed consent was obtained from the participants and one of their parents.

Measures

Test anxiety

Test anxiety was assessed with a 5-item scale in Chinese (e.g., ‘A mathematical test always makes me feel anxious’). A 5-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) was used. The average score of the 5-point scale was computed. In the present study, the internal consistency reliability of the scale was excellent (α = 0.90).

Academic performance

Students’ mathematical scores were evaluated by city-wide standardized tests. Similar to other studies (Liu & Lu, Reference Liu and Lu2011), we standardized students’ mathematical scores in each high school. This method has been widely used by researchers to assess Chinese students’ academic performance (Liu & Lu, Reference Liu and Lu2011).

DNA collection and genotyping

Genomic DNA was extracted from cheek cells. Polymerase chain reaction (PCR) was performed to amplify the DNA fragment. The primer sequences, temperature, time, and cycles of the PCR program were set according to the conditions used in previous studies (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, McClay, Moffitt, Mill, Martin, Craig and Poulton2002). The length of PCR products was separated by using a 2% agarose gel electrophoresis.

Results

Descriptive data (mean values and standard deviations) and intercorrelations of test anxiety and academic performance are listed in Table 1. Path analysis was used to examine the longitudinal relationship between academic performance and test anxiety. Students’ test anxiety at Grade 10, mathematical scores at Grade 10, and gender were treated as predictors of their test anxiety at Grade 12. Statistics were performed with SPSS16.0 and AMOS 7.0. The results indicated that students’ mathematical performance (β = -0.08, p < .05) and test anxiety at Grade 10 (β = 0.52, p < .01) significantly predicted their test anxiety at Grade 12. Moreover, students’ mathematical performance at Grade 10 was negatively correlated with their test anxiety at Grade 10 (correlation coefficient = -0.07, p < .01. Boys had higher mathematical performance (t = 4.17, df = 567, p < .01) and lower anxiety scores (at Grade 10, t = -4.04, df = 567, p < .01; at Grade 12, t = -2.01, df = 567, p < .05) than girls.

TABLE 1 Descriptive Data and Intercorrelations Between Academic Performance and Test Anxiety

N = 569.

*p < .05, **p < .01.

Girls were coded as 1 and boys were coded as 2.

Furthermore, Table 2 shows the distribution of students’ genotypes. When we tested Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium for females’ MAOA genotypes, we combined girls with the 2/4-repeat genotype and the 4/5-repeat genotype with girls who carry the 3/4-repeat genotype. The results showed that females’ MAOA genotypes were in Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (χ2 = 0.52, df = 1, ns). As the MAOA gene is on the X chromosome, the phenomenon of gene suppression for girls can happen. Hence, the next analysis only included boys with the 3-repeat genotype and the 4-repeat genotype and girls with the 3/3-repeat genotype and the 4/4-repeat genotype. We combined the boys with the 3-repeat genotype and the girls with the 3/3-repeat genotype into one group (group 1) and combined the boys with the 4-repeat genotype and girls with the 4/4-repeat genotype into another group (group 2). Figure 1 shows the mean values and standard errors of students’ test anxiety scores at Grade 12 in the two groups.

TABLE 2 Genotype Distributions for the MAOA-Upstream Variable Number of Tandem Repeats (uVNTR) Polymorphism

FIGURE 1 The mean values and standard errors of test anxiety scores at Grade 12 for students with different genotypes.

In this study, ANOVA was used to analyze the effects of the MAOA gene polymorphism on test anxiety at Grade 12 across the two groups. During analysis, we controlled for students’ gender and academic performance at Grade 10, and their test anxiety at Grade 10. The results showed that the main effects of the MAOA gene polymorphism on test anxiety at Grade 12 were significant, F(1, 434) = 5.45, p < .05, χ2 = 0.01. Students in group 2 showed a higher level of test anxiety than students in group 1. Next, multi-group comparison was used to examine the moderating effects of the MAOA gene polymorphism on the relationship between academic performance and test anxiety by using AMOS 7.0. We compared unconstrained models in which parameters were freely estimated with constrained models in which the relationships between academic performance and test anxiety at Grade 12 were forced to be equal between groups 1 and 2. The results indicated that there were significant differences in the strength of the relationship between academic performance and test anxiety across students with two different kinds of alleles (Δχ2 = 3.68, df = 3, p < .05). The results revealed that the prediction of academic performance on test anxiety was only significant among students in group 1 (β = -0.16, p < .01).

Discussion

The present study significantly contributed to theory in several important ways. By using a Chinese sample, the present study first investigated the direct effects of the MAOA gene polymorphism on test anxiety in the subject of mathematics. Our findings provided initial supportive evidence about the significant relationship between the MAOA gene polymorphism and test anxiety. In line with previous Western studies (Tadic et al., Reference Tadic, Rujescu, Szegedi, Giegling, Singer, Möller and Dahmen2003), our data confirmed that the 4-repeat allele of the MAOA gene was related to a higher level of anxiety. In addition, as the previous studies were conducted in Western countries, our findings implied that the links between the MAOA gene polymorphism and anxiety could be generalized to Chinese adolescents.

Second, recently, researchers have become interested in investigating the interaction of gene-by-environment (G × E) on emotional outcomes (Laucht et al., Reference Laucht, Treutlein, Blomeyer, Buchmann, Schmid, Becker and Banaschewski2009). To our knowledge, the present study was the first study that examines the moderating effects of gene polymorphism on the relationship between academic performance and test anxiety. Consistent with the previous studies (Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, Reference Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham2003; Pintrich & DeGroot, Reference Pintrich and DeGroot1990), we found that Chinese high school students’ academic performance was weakly related to their test anxiety. Furthermore, in line with our expectations, our findings showed that the strength of the relationship between academic performance and test anxiety was stronger among students with the 3-repeat allele than those with the 4-repeat allele. Taken together, our findings suggested that the MAOA gene polymorphism was not only directly related to test anxiety but also indirectly moderates the effects of academic performance on test anxiety.

In this study there were some limitations that could be improved in further research. First, the participants were from one city in China, and further research may test the present findings by using samples from other cities. Second, the present study focused on the subject of mathematics; further studies may replicate the present findings in other academic domains. Third, the present study found that the MAOA gene polymorphism moderated the relationship between academic performance and test anxiety. Further studies may examine the moderating effects of other serotonin-related genes on this relationship.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the National Natural Science Fund of China (31301027), the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities, a project funded by the Priority Academic Program Development of Jiangsu Higher Education Institutions. The 12th Five Year Plan of Jiangsu Education, and Programs for the Philosophy and Social Sciences Research of Higher Learning Institutions of Jiangsu (2012SJD190007) for supporting this study.

References

Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., . . . Poulton, R. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297, 851854.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2003). Personality traits and academic examination performance. European Journal of Personality, 17, 237250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dayan, P., & Huys, Q. J. M. (2008). Serotonin, inhibition, and negative mood. PLOS Computational Biology, 4 (2), e4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., Pekrun, R., & Hall, N. C. (2006). The domain specificity of academic emotional experiences. Journal of Experimental Education, 75, 529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jonsson, E. G., Norton, N., Gustavsson, J. P., Oreland, L., Owen, M. J., & Sedvall, G. C. (2000). A promoter polymorphism in the monoamine oxidase A gene and its relationships to monoamine metabolite concentrations in CSF of healthy volunteers. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 34, 239244.Google Scholar
Laucht, M., Treutlein, J., Blomeyer, D., Buchmann, A. F., Schmid, B., Becker, K., . . . Banaschewski, T. (2009). Interaction between the 5-HTTLPR serotonin transporter polymorphism and environmental adversity for mood and anxiety psychopathology: Evidence from a high-risk community sample of young adults. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 12, 737747.Google Scholar
Liu, Y., & Lu, Z. (2011). Trajectories of Chinese students’ sense of school belonging and academic achievement over the high school transition period. Learning & Individual Differences, 21, 187190.Google Scholar
Macher, D., Paechter, M., Papousek, I., & Ruggeri, K. (2012). Statistics anxiety, trait anxiety, learning behavior, and academic performance. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 27, 483498.Google Scholar
Neil, A. L., & Christensen, H. (2009). Efficacy and effectiveness of school-based prevention and early intervention programs for anxiety. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 208215.Google Scholar
Pintrich, P., & DeGroot, E. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 3340.Google Scholar
Tadic, A., Rujescu, D., Szegedi, A., Giegling, I., Singer, P., Möller, H. J., & Dahmen, N. (2003). Association of a MAOA gene variant with generalized anxiety disorder, but not with panic disorder or major depression. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 117B, 16.Google Scholar
Figure 0

TABLE 1 Descriptive Data and Intercorrelations Between Academic Performance and Test Anxiety

Figure 1

TABLE 2 Genotype Distributions for the MAOA-Upstream Variable Number of Tandem Repeats (uVNTR) Polymorphism

Figure 2

FIGURE 1 The mean values and standard errors of test anxiety scores at Grade 12 for students with different genotypes.