Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-ws8qp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-19T01:37:53.262Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Memory for Radio Advertisements: the Effect of Program and Typicality

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 October 2013

Beatriz Martín-Luengo*
Affiliation:
Universidad del País Vasco UPV/EHU (Spain)
Karlos Luna
Affiliation:
Escola de Psicologia, Universidade do Minho (Portugal)
Malen Migueles
Affiliation:
Universidad del País Vasco UPV/EHU (Spain)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Beatriz Martín-Luengo. Dpto. Procesos Psicológicos Básicos y su Desarrollo. Facultad de Psicología. Universidad del País Vasco UPV/EHU. Avda. Tolosa, 70, 20018, Donostia-San Sebastián. Gipuzkoa (Spain). E-mail: beatriz.martin.luengo@gmail.com

Abstract

We examined the influence of the type of radio program on the memory for radio advertisements. We also investigated the role in memory of the typicality (high or low) of the elements of the products advertised. Participants listened to three types of programs (interesting, boring, enjoyable) with two advertisements embedded in each. After completing a filler task, the participants performed a true/false recognition test. Hits and false alarm rates were higher for the interesting and enjoyable programs than for the boring one. There were also more hits and false alarms for the high-typicality elements. The response criterion for the advertisements embedded in the boring program was stricter than for the advertisements in other types of programs. We conclude that the type of program in which an advertisement is inserted and the nature of the elements of the advertisement affect both the number of hits and false alarms and the response criterion, but not the accuracy of the memory.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

This research was supported by an FPU researcher training grant awarded to the first author from the Gobierno Vasco.

References

Brewer, W. E., & Treyens, J. C. (1981). Role of schemata in memory for places. Cognitive Psychology, 13, 207230. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(81)90008-6 Google Scholar
Donaldson, W. (1992). Measuring recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 275277. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.121.3.275 Google Scholar
Fung, H. H., & Cartesen, L. L. (2003). Sending memorable messages to the old: Age differences in preferences and memory for advertisements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 163178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.163 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grafman, J. (2002). The structured event complex and the human prefrontal cortex. In Stuss, D. T. & Knight, R. T. (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 292310). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. K. (2007). Reality monitoring and the media. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 981993. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.1393 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lampinen, J. M., Copeland, S. M., & Neuschatz, J. S. (2001). Recollections of things schematic: Room schemas revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 12111222. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.27.5.1211 Google ScholarPubMed
Luna, K., & Migueles, M. (2008). Typicality and misinformation: Two sources of distortion. Psicológica, 29, 179188.Google Scholar
Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (2005). Detection theory: A user’s guide (2 nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Migueles, M., & García-Bajos, E. (2012). The power of script knowledge and selective retrieval in the recall of daily activities. The Journal of General Psychology, 139, 100113. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2012.663817 Google Scholar
Moorman, M., Neijens, P. C., & Smit, E. G. (2007). The effects of program involvement on commercial exposure and recall in a naturalistic setting. Journal of Advertising, 36, 121137. http://dx.doi.org/10.2753/JOA0091-3367360109 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neuschatz, J. S., Lampinen, J. M., Preston, E. L., Hawkins, E. R., & Toglia, M. P. (2002). The effect of memory schemata on memory and the phenomenological experience of naturalistic situations. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 687708. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.824 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, G. V., Graesser, A. C., Zimmerman, J. A., & Riha, J. (1985). Script processing in a natural situation. Memory & Cognition, 13, 140144. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03197006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norris, C. E., & Colman, A. M. (1992). Context effects on recall and recognition of magazine advertisements. Journal of Advertising, 21, 3746. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00913367.1992.10673374 Google Scholar
Norris, C. E., & Colman, A. M. (1994). Effects of entertainment and enjoyment of television programs on perception and memory of advertisements. Social Behavior and Personality, 22, 365376. http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.1994.22.4.365 Google Scholar
Norris, C. E., & Colman, A. M. (1996). Context effects of radio programming on cognitive processing of embedded advertisements. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10, 473486. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199612)10:6<473::AID-ACP390>3.0.CO;2-# Google Scholar
Potter, R. F., & Callison, C. (2000). Sounds exciting!!:The effects of auditory complexity on listeners’ attitudes and memory for radio promotional announcements. Journal of Radio Studies, 7, 2951. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15506843jrs0701_5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snodgrass, J., & Corwin, J. (1988). Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Applications to dementia and amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 3450. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.117.1.34 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trafimow, D., & Wyer, R. S. (1993). Cognitive representation of mundane social events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 365376. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.64.3.365 CrossRefGoogle Scholar