Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-dfsvx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T13:36:21.151Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Dynamic Assessment of Social Cognition in Individuals with Multiple Sclerosis: A Pilot Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 November 2015

Helen M. Genova*
Affiliation:
Kessler Foundation, West Orange, New Jersey Rutgers, New Jersey Medical School, Newark, New Jersey
Christopher J. Cagna
Affiliation:
Kessler Foundation, West Orange, New Jersey
Nancy D. Chiaravalloti
Affiliation:
Kessler Foundation, West Orange, New Jersey Rutgers, New Jersey Medical School, Newark, New Jersey
John DeLuca
Affiliation:
Kessler Foundation, West Orange, New Jersey Rutgers, New Jersey Medical School, Newark, New Jersey
Jean Lengenfelder
Affiliation:
Kessler Foundation, West Orange, New Jersey Rutgers, New Jersey Medical School, Newark, New Jersey
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Helen M. Genova, 300 Executive Drive Suite 70, West Orange NJ 07052. E-mail: hgenova@kesslerfoundation.org

Abstract

It has recently been reported that individuals with multiple sclerosis (MS) are impaired on tasks requiring emotional processing and social cognition, including tasks of Theory of Mind (ToM) and facial affect recognition. The current pilot study examined the ability of individuals with MS to understand and interpret lies and sarcasm using a dynamic task: The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT). Fifteen individuals with MS and 15 healthy controls (HCs) performed the Social Inference-Enriched subtest of the TASIT, in which they viewed video-taped social interactions in which lies and sarcasm are presented. Additionally, tests of cognition were also administered to better understand the relationship between specific cognitive abilities and the ability to understand lies and sarcasm. The MS group showed impairments in the ability to interpret and understand lies and sarcasm relative to HCs. These impairments were correlated with several cognitive abilities including processing speed, working memory, learning and memory, and premorbid IQ. The results indicate that the TASIT is a sensitive measure of social cognition in individuals with MS. Furthermore, performance on the TASIT was related to cognitive abilities. Results are discussed in terms of social cognition deficits in MS and how they relate to cognitive abilities. (JINS, 2016, 22, 83–88)

Type
Brief Communication
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Chiaravalloti, N.D., & DeLuca, J. (2008). Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurology, 7, 11391151. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70259-X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delis, D.C., Kramer, J.H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B.A. (2000). California Verbal Learning Test: Second Edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Henry, J.D., Phillips, L.H., Beatty, W.W., McDonald, S., Longley, W.A., Joscelyne, A., & Rendell, P.G. (2009). Evidence for deficits in facial affect recognition and theory of mind in multiple sclerosis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(2), 277285. doi:10.1017/S1355617709090195 Google Scholar
Kipps, C.M., Nestor, P.J., Acosta-Cabronero, J., Arnold, R., & Hodges, J.R. (2009). Understanding social dysfunction in the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia: The role of emotion and sarcasm processing. Brain, 132(Pt 3), 592603. doi:10.1093/brain/awn314 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kraemer, M., Herold, M., Uekermann, J., Kis, B., Daum, I., Wiltfang, J., & Abdel-Hamid, M. (2013). Perception of affective prosody in patients at an early stage of relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Journal of Neuropsychology, 7(1), 91106. doi:10.1111/j.1748-6653.2012.02037.x Google Scholar
Mathersul, D., Palmer, D.M., Gur, R.C., Gur, R.E., Cooper, N., Gordon, E., & Williams, L.M. (2009). Explicit identification and implicit recognition of facial emotions: II. Core domains and relationships with general cognition. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31(3), 278291. doi:10.1080/13803390802043619 Google Scholar
McDonald, S., Bornhofen, C., Shum, D., Long, E., Saunders, C., & Neulinger, K. (2006). Reliability and validity of The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT): A clinical test of social perception. Disability and Rehabilitation, 28(24), 15291542. doi:10.1080/09638280600646185 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, S., Darke, S., Kaye, S., & Torok, M. (2013). Deficits in social perception in opioid maintenance patients, abstinent opioid users and non-opioid users. Addiction (Abingdon, England), 108(3), 566574. doi:10.1111/add.12040 Google Scholar
McDonald, S., Flanagan, S., Rollins, J., & Kinch, J. (2003). TASIT: A new clinical tool for assessing social perception after traumatic brain injury. The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 18, 219238. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12802165 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mike, A., Strammer, E., Aradi, M., Orsi, G., Perlaki, G., Hajnal, A., & Illes, Z. (2013). Disconnection mechanism and regional cortical atrophy contribute to impaired processing of facial expressions and theory of mind in multiple sclerosis: A structural MRI study. PloS One, 8(12), e82422. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0082422 Google Scholar
Ouellet, J., Scherzer, P.B., Rouleau, I., Métras, P., Bertrand-Gauvin, C., Djerroud, N., & Duquette, P. (2010). Assessment of social cognition in patients with multiple sclerosis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16(2), 287296. doi:10.1017/S1355617709991329 Google Scholar
Phillips, L.H., Henry, J.D., Scott, C., Summers, F., Whyte, M., & Cook, M. (2011). Specific impairments of emotion perception in multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychology, 25(1), 131136. doi:10.1037/a0020752 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polman, C.H., Reingold, S.C., Banwell, B., Clanet, M., Cohen, J.A., Filippi, M., & Wolinsky, J.S. (2011). Diagnostic criteria for multiple sclerosis: 2010 revisions to the McDonald criteria. Annals of Neurology, 69(2), 292302. doi:10.1002/ana.22366 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pöttgen, J., Dziobek, I., Reh, S., Heesen, C., & Gold, S.M. (2013). Impaired social cognition in multiple sclerosis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 84(5), 523528. doi:10.1136/jnnp-2012-304157 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prochnow, D., Donell, J., Schäfer, R., Jörgens, S., Hartung, H.P., Franz, M., & Seitz, R.J. (2011). Alexithymia and impaired facial affect recognition in multiple sclerosis. Journal of Neurology, 258(9), 16831688. doi:10.1007/s00415-011-6002-4 Google Scholar
Rao, S.M., Leo, G.J., Haughton, V.M. St, Aubin-Faubert, P., & Bernardin, L. (1989). Correlation of magnetic resonance imaging with neuropsychological testing in multiple sclerosis. Neurology, 39(2 Pt 1), 161166.Google Scholar
Smith, A. (1982). Symbol Digit Modalities Test: Manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Sumowski, J.F., Chiaravalloti, N., Wylie, G., & Deluca, J. (2009). Cognitive reserve moderates the negative effect of brain atrophy on cognitive efficiency in multiple sclerosis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(4), 606612. doi:10.1017/S1355617709090912 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sumowski, J.F., & Leavitt, V.M. (2013). Cognitive reserve in multiple sclerosis. Multiple Sclerosis (Houndmills, Basingstoke, England), 19(9), 11221127. doi:10.1177/1352458513498834 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Fourth Edition. San Antonio, TX: Pearson.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2011). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, Second Edition (WASI-II). San Antonio, TX: Pearson.Google Scholar