Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T20:36:14.769Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive, neurophysiological, and functional correlates of proverb interpretation abnormalities in schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 May 2007

MICHAEL KIANG
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive Science, University of California–San Diego, La Jolla, California
GREGORY A. LIGHT
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California–San Diego, La Jolla, California
JOCELYN PRUGH
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive Science, University of California–San Diego, La Jolla, California
SEANA COULSON
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive Science, University of California–San Diego, La Jolla, California
DAVID L. BRAFF
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California–San Diego, La Jolla, California
MARTA KUTAS
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive Science, University of California–San Diego, La Jolla, California Department of Neurosciences, University of California–San Diego, La Jolla, California

Abstract

A hallmark of schizophrenia is impaired proverb interpretation, which could be due to: (1) aberrant activation of disorganized semantic associations, or (2) working memory (WM) deficits. We assessed 18 schizophrenia patients and 18 normal control participants on proverb interpretation, and evaluated these two hypotheses by examining within patients the correlations of proverb interpretation with disorganized symptoms and auditory WM, respectively. Secondarily, we also explored the relationships between proverb interpretation and a spectrum of cognitive functions including auditory sensory-memory encoding (as indexed by the mismatch negativity (MMN) event-related brain potential (ERP)); executive function; and social/occupational function. As expected, schizophrenia patients produced less accurate and less abstract descriptions of proverbs than did controls. These proverb interpretation difficulties in patients were not significantly correlated with disorganization or other symptom factors, but were significantly correlated (p < .05) with WM impairment, as well as with impairments in sensory-memory encoding, executive function, and social/occupational function. These results offer no support for disorganized associations in abnormal proverb interpretation in schizophrenia, but implicate WM deficits, perhaps as a part of a syndrome related to generalized frontal cortical dysfunction. (JINS, 2007, 13, 653–663.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2007 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Andreasen, N.C. (1977). Reliability and validity of proverb interpretation to assess mental status. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 18, 465472.Google Scholar
Andreasen, N.C. (1984a). Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms. Iowa City, IA: University of Iowa.
Andreasen, N.C. (1984b). Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms. Iowa City, IA: University of Iowa.
Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 556559.Google Scholar
Bagner, D.M., Melinder, M.R., & Barch, D.M. (2003). Language comprehension and working memory language comprehension and working memory deficits in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 60, 299309.Google Scholar
Baldeweg, T., Klugman, A., Gruzelier, J.H., & Hirsch, S.R. (2002). Impairment in frontal but not temporal components of mismatch negativity in schizophrenia. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 43, 111122.Google Scholar
Barch, D.M., Csernansky, J.G., Conturo, T., & Snyder, A.Z. (2002). Working and long-term memory deficits in schizophrenia: Is there a common prefrontal mechanism? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 478494.Google Scholar
Braff, D.L. & Beck, A.T. (1974). Thinking disorder in depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 31, 456459.Google Scholar
Braff, D.L. & Light, G.A. (2004). Preattentional and attentional cognitive deficits as targets for treating schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 174, 7585.Google Scholar
Bruder, G.E., Wexler, B.E., Sage, M.M., Gil, R.B., & Gorman, J.M. (2004). Verbal memory in schizophrenia: additional evidence of subtypes having different cognitive deficits. Schizophrenia Research, 68, 137147.Google Scholar
Brune, M. & Bodenstein, L. (2005). Proverb comprehension reconsidered—“theory of mind” and the pragmatic use of language in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 75, 233239.Google Scholar
Buss, A.H. & Lang, P.J. (1965). Psychological Deficit in Schizophrenia: I. Affect, Reinforcement, and Concept Attainment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 70, 224.Google Scholar
Cacciari, C., Reati, F., Colombo, M.R., Padovani, R., Rizzo, S., & Papagno, C. (2006). The comprehension of ambiguous idioms in aphasic patients. Neuropsychologia, 44, 13051314.Google Scholar
Carpenter, B.N. & Chapman, L.J. (1982). Premorbid status in schizophrenia and abstract, literal, or autistic proverb interpretation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 91, 151156.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.
Condray, R., Steinhauer, S.R., van Kammen, D.P., & Kasparek, A. (1996). Working memory capacity predicts language comprehension in schizophrenic patients. Schizophrenia Research, 20, 113.Google Scholar
Coulson, S. & Oakley, T. (2000). Blending basics. Cognitive Linguistics, 11, 175196.Google Scholar
Coulson, S. & Van Petten, C. (2002). Conceptual integration and metaphor: An event-related potential study. Memory and Cognition, 30, 958968.Google Scholar
Daban, C., Amado, I., Bayle, F., Gut, A., Willard, D., Bourdel, M.C., Loo, H., Olie, J.P., Millet, B., Krebs, M.O., & Poirier, M.F. (2002). Correlation between clinical syndromes and neuropsychological tasks in unmedicated patients with recent onset schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 113, 8392.Google Scholar
Delis, D.C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J.H. (2001). The Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System: Technical Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Delis, D.C., Kramer, J.H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B.A. (2000). California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT-II) Manual (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Dunn, L.M. & Dunn, L.M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Fauconnier, G. & Turner, M. (1998). Conceptual integration networks. Cognitive Science, 22, 133187.Google Scholar
Fauconnier, G. & Turner, M. (2002). The Way We Think: Conceptual Blending and the Mind's Hidden Complexities. New York: Basic Books.
First, M.B., Spitzer, R.L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J.B.W. (1995). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Gibbs, R.W., Jr. & Beitel, D. (1995). What proverb understanding reveals about how people think. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 133154.Google Scholar
Gick, M.L. & Holyoak, K.J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 138.Google Scholar
Glahn, D.C., Cannon, T.D., Gur, R.E., Ragland, J.D., & Gur, R.C. (2000). Working memory constrains abstraction in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 47, 3442.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, S. (2003). The psycholinguistics of metaphor. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7, 9296.Google Scholar
Gold, J.M., Carpenter, C., Randolph, C., Goldberg, T.E., & Weinberger, D.R. (1997). Auditory working memory and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 159165.Google Scholar
Goldman-Rakic, P.S. & Selemon, L.D. (1997). Functional and anatomical aspects of prefrontal pathology in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 23, 437458.Google Scholar
Goldstein, K. (1944). Methodological approach to the study of schizophrenic thought disorder. In J.S. Kasanin (Ed.), Language and Thought in Schizophrenia. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Gorham, D.R. (1956). Use of the proverbs test for differentiating schizophrenics from normals. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 20, 435440.Google Scholar
Haaland, K.Y., Vranes, L.F., Goodwin, J.S., & Garry, P.J. (1987). Wisconsin Card Sort Test performance in a healthy elderly population. Journal of Gerontology, 42, 345346.Google Scholar
Halford, G.S., Wilson, W.H., & Phillips, S. (1998). Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 803831; discussion 831–864.Google Scholar
Harrow, M., Tucker, G.J., & Adler, D. (1972). Concrete and idiosyncratic thinking in acute schizophrenic patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 26, 433439.Google Scholar
Hartman, M., Steketee, M.C., Silva, S., Lanning, K., & Andersson, C. (2003). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizophrenia: The role of working memory. Schizophrenia Research, 63, 201217.Google Scholar
Hauser, R.M. & Warren, J.R. (1996). A Socioeconomic Index for Occupations in the 1990 Census, Working Paper #96-01. Madison, WI: Center for Demography and Ecology, University of Wisconsin.
Heaton, R.K., Chelune, G.J., Talley, J.L., Kay, G.G., & Curtiss, G. (1993). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Hummel, J.E. & Holyoak, K.J. (2003). A symbolic-connectionist theory of relational inference and generalization. Psychological Review, 110, 220264.Google Scholar
Javitt, D.C., Doneshka, P., Grochowski, S., & Ritter, W. (1995). Impaired mismatch negativity generation reflects widespread dysfunction of working memory in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52, 550558.Google Scholar
Javitt, D.C., Strous, R.D., Grochowski, S., Ritter, W., & Cowan, N. (1997). Impaired precision, but normal retention, of auditory sensory (“echoic”) memory information in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 315324.Google Scholar
Kawakubo, Y. & Kasai, K. (2006). Support for an association between mismatch negativity and social functioning in schizophrenia. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 30, 13671368.Google Scholar
Kawakubo, Y., Kasai, K., Kudo, N., Rogers, M.A., Nakagome, K., Itoh, K., & Kato, N. (2006). Phonetic mismatch negativity predicts verbal memory deficits in schizophrenia. Neuroreport, 17, 10431046.Google Scholar
Kay, S.R., Opler, L.A., & Lindenmayer, J.P. (1989). The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS): Rationale and standardisation. British Journal of Psychiatry Supplement, 7, 5967.Google Scholar
Keefe, R.S., Bilder, R.M., Harvey, P.D., Davis, S.M., Palmer, B.W., Gold, J.M., Meltzer, H.Y., Green, M.F., Miller, D.D., Canive, J.M., Adler, L.W., Manschreck, T.C., Swartz, M., Rosenheck, R., Perkins, D.O., Walker, T.M., Stroup, T.S., McEvoy, J.P., & Lieberman, J.A. (2006). Baseline Neurocognitive Deficits in the CATIE Schizophrenia Trial. Neuropsychopharmacology, 31, 20332046.Google Scholar
Kostova, M., Passerieux, C., Laurent, J.P., & Hardy-Bayle, M.C. (2005). N400 anomalies in schizophrenia are correlated with the severity of formal thought disorder. Schizophrenia Research, 78, 285291.Google Scholar
Kremen, W.S., Seidman, L.J., Faraone, S.V., Toomey, R., & Tsuang, M.T. (2004). Heterogeneity of schizophrenia: A study of individual neuropsychological profiles. Schizophrenia Research, 71, 307321.Google Scholar
Kurtz, M.M. & Wexler, B.E. (2006). Differences in performance and learning proficiency on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in schizophrenia: Do they reflect distinct neurocognitive subtypes with distinct functional profiles? Schizophrenia Research, 81, 167171.Google Scholar
Lee, J. & Park, S. (2005). Working memory impairments in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 599611.Google Scholar
Light, G.A. & Braff, D.L. (2005a). Mismatch negativity deficits are associated with poor functioning in schizophrenia patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 127136.Google Scholar
Light, G.A. & Braff, D.L. (2005b). Stability of mismatch negativity deficits and their relationship to functional impairments in chronic schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 17411743.Google Scholar
Light, G.A., Swerdlow, N.R., & Braff, D.L., (in press). Preattentive sensory processing as indexed by the MMN and P3a brain responses is associated with cognitive and psychosocial functioning in healthy adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
McGrath, J., Chapple, B., & Wright, M. (2001). Working memory in schizophrenia and mania: Correlation with symptoms during the acute and subacute phases. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 103, 181188.Google Scholar
McKibbin, C.L., Brekke, J.S., Sires, D., Jeste, D.V., & Patterson, T.L. (2004). Direct assessment of functional abilities: Relevance to persons with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 72, 5367.Google Scholar
Melinder, M.R. & Barch, D.M. (2003). The influence of a working memory load manipulation on language production in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 29, 473485.Google Scholar
Michie, P.T. (2001). What has MMN revealed about the auditory system in schizophrenia? International Journal of Psychophysiology, 42, 177194.Google Scholar
Miller, D.D., Arndt, S., & Andreasen, N.C. (1993). Alogia, attentional impairment, and inappropriate affect: Their status in the dimensions of schizophrenia. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 34, 221226.Google Scholar
Moretti, R., Torre, P., Antonello, R.M., Cazzato, G., & Bava, A. (2002). Ten-Point Clock Test: A correlation analysis with other neuropsychological tests in dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 17, 347353.Google Scholar
Moritz, S., Mersmann, K., Kloss, M., Jacobsen, D., Wilke, U., Andresen, B., Naber, D., & Pawlik, K. (2001). “Hyper-priming” in thought-disordered schizophrenic patients. Psychological Medicine, 31, 221229.Google Scholar
Moritz, S., Woodward, T.S., Kuppers, D., Lausen, A., & Schickel, M. (2003). Increased automatic spreading of activation in thought-disordered schizophrenic patients. Schizophrenia Research, 59, 181186.Google Scholar
Morrison, R.G., Krawczyk, D.C., Holyoak, K.J., Hummel, J.E., Chow, T.W., Miller, B.L., & Knowlton, B.J. (2004). A neurocomputational model of analogical reasoning and its breakdown in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 260271.Google Scholar
Naatanen, R. (2003). Mismatch negativity: Clinical research and possible applications. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 48, 179188.Google Scholar
Naatanen, R., Gaillard, A.W., & Mantysalo, S. (1978). Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted. Acta Psychologica (Amsterdam), 42, 313329.Google Scholar
Naatanen, R., Paavilainen, P., Alho, K., Reinikainen, K., & Sams, M. (1989). Do event-related potentials reveal the mechanism of the auditory sensory memory in the human brain? Neuroscience Letters, 98, 217221.Google Scholar
Patterson, T.L., Goldman, S., McKibbin, C.L., Hughs, T., & Jeste, D.V. (2001). UCSD Performance-Based Skills Assessment: Development of a new measure of everyday functioning for severely mentally ill adults. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 27, 235245.Google Scholar
Reed, J.L. (1968). The proverbs test in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 114, 317321.Google Scholar
Reich, J.H. (1981). Proverbs and the modern mental status exam. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 22, 528531.Google Scholar
Sato, Y., Yabe, H., Todd, J., Michie, P., Shinozaki, N., Sutoh, T., Hiruma, T., Nashida, T., Matsuoka, T., & Kaneko, S. (2003). Impairment in activation of a frontal attention-switch mechanism in schizophrenic patients. Biological Psychology, 62, 4963.Google Scholar
Shimkunas, A.M., Gynther, M.D., & Smith, K. (1966). Abstracting ability of schizophrenics before and during phenothiazine therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 14, 7983.Google Scholar
Shimkunas, A.M., Gynther, M.D., & Smith, K. (1967). Schizophrenic responses to the Proverbs Test: Abstract, concrete, or autistic? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 72, 128133.Google Scholar
Silver, H., Feldman, P., Bilker, W., & Gur, R.C. (2003). Working memory deficit as a core neuropsychological dysfunction in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 18091816.Google Scholar
Spitzer, M., Braun, U., Hermle, L., & Maier, S. (1993). Associative semantic network dysfunction in thought-disordered schizophrenic patients: Direct evidence from indirect semantic priming. Biological Psychiatry, 34, 864877.Google Scholar
Sponheim, S.R., Surerus-Johnson, C., Leskela, J., & Dieperink, M.E. (2003). Proverb interpretation in schizophrenia: the significance of symptomatology and cognitive processes. Schizophrenia Research, 65, 117123.Google Scholar
Waltz, J.A., Lau, A., Grewal, S.K., & Holyoak, K.J. (2000). The role of working memory in analogical mapping. Memory and Cognition, 28, 12051212.Google Scholar