Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-x24gv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-01T04:35:35.928Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Reading ability mediates the relationship between education and executive function tasks

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 January 2006

AMANDA SCHAFER JOHNSON
Affiliation:
Institute of Gerontology, Wayne State University, Detroit, Michigan
LISA J. FLICKER
Affiliation:
Institute of Gerontology, Wayne State University, Detroit, Michigan
PETER A. LICHTENBERG
Affiliation:
Institute of Gerontology, Wayne State University, Detroit, Michigan

Abstract

Neuropsychological test results are affected by multiple factors, but usually age and education are the only variables by which norms are stratified. Some authors have questioned whether these variables alone are sufficient (e.g., Marcopulos et al., 1997; Manly et al., 2002), since such norms have lead to problems, such as poor specificity for African Americans on dementia screening devices (Fillenbaum et al., 1990). Recent research has shown that reading ability, a measure of educational quality, attenuated racial differences in test performance (Manly et al., 2002). We specifically examined whether reading ability would account for a greater amount of variance than education in executive function tests in a population traditionally subject to poor educational quality. Results determined that reading ability accounted for a significantly greater amount of variance than years of education for Letter-Number Sequencing, Similarities, COWA, Trail Making Test, and Coloured Progressive Matrices. Reading ability was found to significantly mediate the relationship between each of these tests and education. Animal naming appears to be least affected by educational quality or quantity. These findings hold implications for the interpretation of neuropsychological test results, especially in those exposed to substandard educational quality, and for the way that test norms are constructed. (JINS, 2006, 12, 64–71.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2006 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Albert, S.M. & Teresi, J.A. (1999). Reading ability, education, and cognitive status assessment among older adults in Harlem, New York City. American Journal of Public Health, 89, 9597.Google Scholar
Benton, A.L. & Hamsher, K. (1976). Multilingual Aphasia Examination. Iowa City: University of Iowa.
Chaplewski, E. (2002). Facing the future: City of Detroit needs assessment of older adults. Detroit, MI: Department of Senior Citizens.
Diaz-Asper, C.M., Schretlen, D.J., & Pearlson, G.D. (2004). How well does IQ predict neuropsychological test performance in normal adults? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 8290.Google Scholar
Double, K.L., Halliday, G.M., Kril, J.J., Harasty, J.A., Cullen, K., Brooks, W.S., Creasey, H., & Broe, G.A. (1996). Topography of brain atrophy during normal aging and Alzheimer's Disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 17, 513521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fillenbaum, G., Heyman, A., William, K., Prosnitz, B., & Burchett, B. (1990). Sensitivity and specificity of standardized screens of cognitive impairment and dementia among elderly black and white community residents. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 43, 651660.Google Scholar
Froehlich, T.E., Bogardus, S.T., & Inouye, S.K. (2001). Dementia and race: Are there differences between African Americans and Caucasians? Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 49, 477484.Google Scholar
Giovannetti, T., Lamar, M., Cloud, B.S., Swenson, R., Fein, D., Kaplan, E., & Libon, D.J. (2001). Different underlying mechanisms for deficits in concept formation in dementia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 16, 547560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenwald, R., Hedges, L.V., & Laine, R.D. (1996). The effect of school resources on student achievement. Review of Educational Research, 66, 361396.Google Scholar
Griffin, S.L., Mindt, M.R., Rankin, E.J., Ritchie, A.J., & Scott, J.G. (2002). Estimating premorbid intelligence: Comparison of traditional and contemporary methods across the intelligence continuum. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17, 497507.Google Scholar
Gurland, B.J., Wilder, D.E., Cross, P., Teresi, J., & Barrett, V.W. (1992). Screening scales for dementia: Toward reconciliation of conflicting cross-cultural findings. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 7, 105113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmbeck, G.N. (2002). Post-hoc probing of significant moderational and mediational effects in studies of pediatric populations. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 27, 8796.Google Scholar
Loring, D.W. (Ed.). (1999). INS dictionary of neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University Press.
Johnstone, B. & Wilhelm, K.L. (1996). The longitudinal stability of the WRAT-R Reading subtest: Is it an appropriate estimate of premorbid intelligence? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 282285.Google Scholar
Lezak, M.D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Manly, J.J., Jacobs, D.M., Touradji, P., Small, S.A., & Stern, Y. (2002). Reading level attenuates difference in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 341348.Google Scholar
Manly, J.J., Byrd, D.A., & Touradji, P. (2004). Acculturation, reading level, and neuropsychological test performance among African American elders. Applied Neuropsychology, 11, 3746.Google Scholar
Marcie, P., Roudier, M., Goldblum, M., & Boller, F. (1993). Principal component analysis of language performances in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Communication Disorders, 26, 5363.Google Scholar
Marcopulos, B.A., McLain, C.A., & Giuliano, A.J. (1997). Cognitive impairment or inadequate norms? A study of healthy, rural, older adults with limited education. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 11, 111131.Google Scholar
Mast, B.T., Fitzgerald, J., Steinberg, J., MacNeill, S.E., & Lichtenberg, P.A. (2001). Effective screening for Alzheimer's disease among older African Americans. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15, 196202.Google Scholar
Miles, T.P. (2001). Dementia, race, and education: A cautionary note for clinicians and researchers. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 49, 490.Google Scholar
Mills, C.J., Ablard, K.E., & Brody, L.E. (1993). The Raven's Progressive Matrices: Its usefulness for identifying gifted/talented students. Roeper Review, 15, 183186.Google Scholar
Raven, J. (1965). The Coloured Progressive Matrices Test. London: Lewis Publishers.
Reitan, R. & Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead-Reitan Neurological Test Battery: Theory and clinical interpretation. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press.
Reynolds, C.R., Chastain, R.L., Kaufman, A.S., & McLean, J.E. (1987). Demographic characteristics and IQ among adults: Analysis of the WAIS-R standardization sample as a function of the stratification variables. Journal of School Psychology, 25, 323342.Google Scholar
Royall, D.R., Cabello, M., & Polk, M.J. (1998). Executive dyscontrol: An important factor affecting the level of care received by older retirees. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 46, 15191524.Google Scholar
Royall, D.R., Mulroy, A.R., Chiodo, L.K., & Polk, M.J. (1999). Clock drawing is sensitive to executive control: A comparison of six methods. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 54B, 328333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, J.J. & Paolo, A.M. (2001). Exploratory factor analysis of the WAIS-II in a mixed patient sample. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 16, 151156.Google Scholar
Schmand, B., Geerlings, M.I., Jonker, C., & Lindeboom, J. (1998). Reading ability as an estimator of premorbid intelligence: Does it remain stable in emergent dementia? Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20, 4251.Google Scholar
Sobel, M.E. (1988). Direct and indirect effect in linear structural equation models. In J.S. Long (Ed.), Common problems/proper solutions: Avoiding error in quantitative research (pp. 4664). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Spreen, O. & Strauss, E. (1998). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary. New York: Oxford University Press.
U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare. (1966). Equality of educational opportunity. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Third Edition. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace.
Welsh, K.A., Fillenbaum, G., Wilkinson, W., Heyman, A., Mohs, R.C., Stern, Y., Harrell, L., Edland, S.D., & Beekly, D. (1995). Neuropsychological test performance in African-American and white patients with Alzheimer's disease. Neurology, 45, 22072211.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, G.S. (1993). WRAT-3 administration manual. Wilmington, Delaware: Wide Range.